Abstract
Optimism is linked to a range of positive social and cognitive outcomes across development. Yet decades of work in psychological science has revealed that optimism declines throughout early childhood. Despite this well-documented decline, there is no agreed-upon theory that accounts for developmental changes in optimism. In this Perspective, we synthesize cognitive, computational, social and neural evidence and discuss three candidate mechanisms that might underlie declines in optimism with age: learning from experience, changing theories of success and wishful thinking, and shifts in valenced learning biases. We argue that declining optimism across childhood is best characterized by an account that integrates these theories. Specifically, we suggest that environmental factors impact the pace at which children’s theories and valenced learning biases change with age, and consequently the rate at which their optimism declines. This account suggests that optimism should be conceptualized as an adaptive bias that signals the nature of one’s environment and leads to specific recommendations for future lines of enquiry.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles
$59.00 per year
only $4.92 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout



Similar content being viewed by others
References
Flavell, J. H., Friedrichs, A. G. & Hoyt, J. D. Developmental changes in memorization processes. Cognit. Psychol. 1, 324–340 (1970).
Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J. & Merriman, W. E. Persistent overconfidence despite practice: the role of task experience in preschoolers’ recall predictions. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 103, 152–166 (2009).
Yussen, S. R. & Levy, V. M. Developmental changes in predicting one’s own span of short-term memory. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 19, 502–508 (1975).
Zhang, X., Carrillo, B. & Leonard, J. A. Children predict improvement on novel motor tasks. Preprint at OSF https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/WTYXN (2024).
Coote, K. & Livesey, D. Optimism bias in children’s motor performance expectations. Australian Educ. Dev. Psychol. 16, 52–61 (1999).
Plumert, J. M. Relations between children’s overestimation of their physical abilities and accident proneness. Dev. Psychol. 31, 866–876 (1995).
Schneider, W. Performance prediction in young children: effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Dev. Sci. 1, 291–297 (1998).
Parsons, J. E. & Ruble, D. N. The development of achievement-related expectancies. Child. Dev. 48, 1075–1079 (1977).
Hennefield, L. & Markson, L. The development of optimistic expectations in young children. Cognit. Dev. 63, 101201 (2022).
Wente, A. O. et al. Young children are wishful thinkers: the development of wishful thinking in 3‐ to 10‐year‐old children. Child. Dev. 91, 1166–1182 (2020).
Lockhart, K. L., Goddu, M. K. & Keil, F. C. Overoptimism about future knowledge: early arrogance? J. Posit. Psychol. 12, 36–46 (2017).
Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. Dispositional optimism. Trends Cognit. Sci. 18, 293–299 (2014).
Chang, E. C., Chang, R. & Sanna, L. J. Optimism, pessimism, and motivation: relations to adjustment. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 3, 494–506 (2009).
Rincón Uribe, F. A., Neira Espejo, C. A. & Pedroso, J. D. S. The role of optimism in adolescent mental health: a systematic review. J. Happiness Stud. 23, 815–845 (2022).
Lu, M. S., Hennefield, L., Tillman, R. & Markson, L. Optimistic children engage in more constructive risk-taking behaviors. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 47, 72–81 (2023).
Schulz, L. The origins of inquiry: inductive inference and exploration in early childhood. Trends Cognit. Sci. 16, 382–389 (2012).
Bonawitz, E. B., van Schijndel, T. J. P., Friel, D. & Schulz, L. Children balance theories and evidence in exploration, explanation, and learning. Cognit. Psychol. 64, 215–234 (2012).
Gopnik, A. The scientist as child. Philosophy Sci. 63, 485–514 (1996).
Gweon, H. Inferential social learning: cognitive foundations of human social learning and teaching. Trends Cognit. Sci. 25, 896–910 (2021).
Kruger, J. & Dunning, D. Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 1121–1134 (1999).
Adolph, K. E. Learning to move. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 213–218 (2008).
Adolph, K. E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Ishak, S., Karasik, L. B. & Lobo, S. A. Locomotor experience and use of social information are posture specific. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1705–1714 (2008).
Zhao, X. et al. Culture moderates the relationship between self-control ability and free will beliefs in childhood. Cognition 210, 104609 (2021).
Leonard, J. A., Cordrey, S. R., Liu, H. Z. & Mackey, A. P. Young children calibrate effort based on the trajectory of their performance. Dev. Psychol. 59, 609–619 (2023).
Cimpian, A. in Handbook of Competence and Motivation, Second Edition: Theory and Application (eds Elliot, A. J., Dweck, C. S. & Yeager, D. S.) 387–407 (Guilford Press, 2017).
Butler, R. in Handbook of Competence and Motivation (eds Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S.) 202–221 (Guilford Press, 2005).
Rosenholtz, S. J. & Simpson, C. The formation of ability conceptions: developmental trend or social construction? Rev. Educ. Res. 54, 31–63 (1984).
Stipek, D. J. & Daniels, D. H. Declining perceptions of competence: a consequence of changes in the child or in the educational environment? J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 352–356 (1988).
Kamins, M. L. & Dweck, C. S. Person versus process praise and criticism: implications for contingent self-worth and coping. Dev. Psychol. 35, 835–847 (1999).
Cimpian, A. The impact of generic language about ability on children’s achievement motivation. Dev. Psychol. 46, 1333–1340 (2010).
Cimpian, A., Mu, Y. & Erickson, L. C. Who is good at this game? Linking an activity to a social category undermines children’s achievement. Psychol. Sci. 23, 533–541 (2012).
Muenks, K., Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S. I can do this! The development and calibration of children’s expectations for success and competence beliefs. Dev. Rev. 48, 24–39 (2018).
Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S. & Wigfield, A. Changes in children’s self-competence and values: gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child. Dev. 73, 509–527 (2002).
Moutsiana, C. et al. Human development of the ability to learn from bad news. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 16396–16401 (2013).
Zhang, X., McDougle, S. D. & Leonard, J. A. People accurately predict the shape of skill learning curves. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rwuc9 (2024).
Carroll, P. J., Sweeny, K. & Shepperd, J. A. Forsaking optimism. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 10, 56–73 (2006).
Sweeny, K. & Krizan, Z. Sobering up: a quantitative review of temporal declines in expectations. Psychol. Bull. 139, 702–724 (2013).
Shepperd, J. A., Ouellette, J. A. & Fernandez, J. K. Abandoning unrealistic optimism: performance estimates and the temporal proximity of self-relevant feedback. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 844–855 (1996).
Sweeny, K., Carroll, P. J. & Shepperd, J. A. Is optimism always best? Future outlooks and preparedness. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 302–306 (2006).
Nicholls, J. G. The development of the concepts of effort and ability, perception of academic attainment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require more ability. Child. Dev. 49, 800–814 (1978).
Chapman, M. & Skinner, E. A. Children’s agency beliefs, cognitive performance, and conceptions of effort and ability: individual and developmental differences. Child. Dev. 60, 1229–1238 (1989).
Heyman, G. D., Gee, C. L. & Giles, J. W. Preschool children’s reasoning about ability. Child. Dev. 74, 516–534 (2003).
Miller, A. A developmental study of the cognitive basis of performance impairment after failure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 49, 529–538 (1985).
Nicholls, J. G. & Miller, A. T. Reasoning about the ability of self and others: a developmental study. Child. Dev. 55, 1990–1999 (1984).
Folmer, A. S. et al. Age-related changes in children’s understanding of effort and ability: implications for attribution theory and motivation. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 99, 114–134 (2008).
Ganesan, K. & Steinbeis, N. Effort-related decision-making and its underlying processes during childhood. Dev. Psychol. 57, 1487–1496 (2021).
Hiebert, E. H., Winograd, P. N. & Danner, F. W. Children’s attributions for failure and success in different aspects of reading. J. Educ. Psychol. 76, 1139–1148 (1984).
Nicholls, J. G. Development of perception of own attainment and causal attributions for success and failure in reading. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 94–99 (1979).
Whitehead, G. I. III, Anderson, W. F. & Mitchell, K. D. Children’s causal attributions to self and other as a function of outcome and task. J. Educ. Psychol. 79, 192–194 (1987).
Kushnir, T., Gopnik, A., Chernyak, N., Seiver, E. & Wellman, H. M. Developing intuitions about free will between ages four and six. Cognition 138, 79–101 (2015).
Stipek, D. J. & Hoffman, J. M. Development of children’s performance-related judgments. Child. Dev. 51, 912–914 (1980).
Stipek, D. J., Roberts, T. A. & Sanborn, M. E. Preschool-age children’s performance expectations for themselves and another child as a function of the incentive value of success and the salience of past performance. Child. Dev. 55, 1983–1989 (1984).
Xia, M., Poorthuis, A. M. G., Zhou, Q. & Thomaes, S. Young children’s overestimation of performance: a cross‐cultural comparison. Child. Dev. 93, e207–e221 (2022).
Bernard, S., Clément, F. & Mercier, H. Wishful thinking in preschoolers. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 141, 267–274 (2016).
Leonard, J. A., Bennett-Pierre, G. & Gweon, H. Who is better? Preschoolers infer relative competence based on efficiency of process and quality of outcome. Proc. 41st Annu. Conf. Cognit. Sci. Soc. 639–645 (2019).
Muradoglu, M. & Cimpian, A. Children’s intuitive theories of academic performance. Child. Dev. 91, e902–e918 (2020).
Bamford, C. & Lagattuta, K. H. Optimism and wishful thinking: consistency across populations in children’s expectations for the future. Child. Dev. 91, 1116–1134 (2020).
Habicht, J., Bowler, A., Moses-Payne, M. E., & Hauser, T. U. Children are full of optimism, but those rose-tinted glasses are fading-Reduced learning from negative outcomes drives hyperoptimism in children. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 151, 1843–1853 (2022).
Dohmen, T., Quercia, S. & Willrodt, J. On the psychology of the relation between optimism and risk taking. J. Risk Uncertain. 67, 193–214 (2023).
Sharot, T. The optimism bias. Curr. Biol. 21, R941–R945 (2011).
Lefebvre, G., Lebreton, M., Meyniel, F., Bourgeois-Gironde, S. & Palminteri, S. Behavioural and neural characterization of optimistic reinforcement learning. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 1–9 (2017).
Sharot, T., Korn, C. W. & Dolan, R. J. How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1475–1479 (2011).
Sharot, T., Guitart-Masip, M., Korn, C. W., Chowdhury, R. & Dolan, R. J. How dopamine enhances an optimism bias in humans. Curr. Biol. 16, 1477–1481 (2012).
Williams, S. M. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Widespread origin of the primate mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb. Cortex 8, 321–345 (1998).
Hoops, D. & Flores, C. Making dopamine connections in adolescence. Trends Neurosci. 40, 709–719 (2017).
Braams, B. R., van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Peper, J. S. & Crone, E. A. Longitudinal changes in adolescent risk-taking: a comprehensive study of neural responses to rewards, pubertal development, and risk-taking behavior. J. Neurosci. 35, 7226–7238 (2015).
Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M. & Casey, B. J. A time of change: behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cogn. 72, 124–133 (2010).
Somerville, L. H. & Casey, B. Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 236–241 (2010).
Nussenbaum, K. & Hartley, C. A. Developmental change in prefrontal cortex recruitment supports the emergence of value-guided memory. eLife 10, e69796 (2021).
Insel, C., Kastman, E. K., Glenn, C. R. & Somerville, L. H. Development of corticostriatal connectivity constrains goal-directed behavior during adolescence. Nat. Commun. 8, 1605 (2017).
Silvers, J. A. & Guassi Moreira, J. F. Capacity and tendency: a neuroscientific framework for the study of emotion regulation. Neurosci. Lett. 693, 35–39 (2019).
Park, A. T. et al. Early childhood stress is associated with blunted development of ventral tegmental area functional connectivity. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 47, 100909 (2021).
Diamond, A. in Principles of Frontal lobe Function Vol. 466 (eds Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T.) 201–225 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2002).
Fiske, A. & Holmboe, K. Neural substrates of early executive function development. Dev. Rev. 52, 42–62 (2019).
Sjåstad, H. & Baumeister, R. Fast optimism, slow realism? Causal evidence for a two-step model of future thinking. Cognition 236, 105447 (2023).
Nussenbaum, K., Velez, J. A., Washington, B. T., Hamling, H. E. & Hartley, C. A. Flexibility in valenced reinforcement learning computations across development. Child. Dev. 93, 1601–1615 (2022).
Mai, X. et al. Brain activity elicited by positive and negative feedback in preschool-aged children. PLoS ONE 6, e18774 (2011).
Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W. & Kaplan, H. S. in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: Foundations 2nd edn (ed. Buss, D. M.) Vol. 1 88–114 (Wiley, 2016).
Nettle, D. & Frankenhuis, W. E. The evolution of life-history theory: a bibliometric analysis of an interdisciplinary research area. Proc. R. Soc. B. 286, 20190040 (2019).
Frankenhuis, W. E. & Gopnik, A. Early adversity and the development of explore–exploit tradeoffs. Trends Cognit. Sci. 27, 616–630 (2023).
Eckstein, M. K. et al. The interpretation of computational model parameters depends on the context. eLife 11, e75474 (2022).
Nussenbaum, K. & Hartley, C. A. Reinforcement learning across development: what insights can we draw from a decade of research? Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 40, 100733 (2019).
Dorfman, H. M., Bhui, R., Hughes, B. L. & Gershman, S. J. Causal inference about good and bad outcomes. Psychol. Sci. 30, 516–525 (2019).
Bjorklund, D. F. Why Youth Is Not Wasted on the Young: Immaturity in Human Development (Wiley, 2009).
Tooley, U. A., Bassett, D. S. & Mackey, A. P. Environmental influences on the pace of brain development. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 372–384 (2021).
McDermott, C. L. et al. Early life stress is associated with earlier emergence of permanent molars. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2105304118 (2021).
Belsky, J. Early-life adversity accelerates child and adolescent development. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 241–246 (2019).
Colich, N. L., Rosen, M. L., Williams, E. S. & McLaughlin, K. A. Biological aging in childhood and adolescence following experiences of threat and deprivation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 146, 721–764 (2020).
Heinonen, K. et al. Socioeconomic status in childhood and adulthood: associations with dispositional optimism and pessimism over a 21‐year follow‐up. J. Pers. 74, 1111–1126 (2006).
Robb, K. A., Simon, A. E. & Wardle, J. Socioeconomic disparities in optimism and pessimism. Int.J. Behav. Med. 16, 331–338 (2009).
Decker, A. L. et al. Striatal and behavioral responses to reward vary by socioeconomic status in adolescents. J. Neurosci. 44, e1633232023 (2024).
Heathcote, A., Brown, S. & Mewhort, D. J. K. The power law repealed: the case for an exponential law of practice. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 7, 185–207 (2000).
Thorndike, E. L. The Psychology of Learning Vol. 2 (Teachers College, Columbia Univ., 1913).
Newell, A. & Rosenbloom, P. S. in Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition (ed. Anderson, J. R.) 1–55 (Psychology Press, 1981).
Son, L. K. & Sethi, R. Metacognitive control and optimal learning. Cognit. Sci. 30, 759–774 (2006).
Stadler, M. A., & Frensch, P. A. (eds) Handbook of implicit learning (Sage, 1998).
Szulewski, A., Roth, N. & Howes, D. The use of task-evoked pupillary response as an objective measure of cognitive load in novices and trained physicians: a new tool for the assessment of expertise. Acad. Med. 90, 981–987 (2015).
Bian, L. & Leslie, S.-J. Gender stereotypes about intellectual ability emerge early and influence children’s interests. Science 355, 389–391 (2017).
Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., McAdoo, H. P. & García Coll, C. The home environments of children in the United States part I: variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child. Dev. 72, 1844–1867 (2001).
Bradley, R. H. & Corwyn, R. F. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 371–399 (2002).
Goudeau, S. et al. Unequal opportunities from the start: socioeconomic disparities in classroom participation in preschool. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 152, 3135–3152 (2023).
Hasan, N. & Power, T. G. Optimism and pessimism in children: a study of parenting correlates. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 185–191 (2002).
Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S. in Development of Achievement Motivation Ch. 4 (eds Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S.) 91–120 (Academic Press, 2002).
Häfner, I. et al. STEM motivation and course-taking: bidirectional relationships between parents and adolescents from middle school to college. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA) (2016).
Wang, S., Rubie-Davies, C. M. & Meissel, K. A systematic review of the teacher expectation literature over the past 30 years. Educ. Res. Eval. 24, 124–179 (2018).
Poon, K. The impact of socioeconomic status on parental factors in promoting academic achievement in Chinese children. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 75, 102175 (2020).
Harvey, D. G. & Slatin, G. T. The relationship between child’s SES and teacher expectations: a test of the middle-class bias hypothesis. Soc. Forces 54, 140–159 (1975).
Speybroeck, S. et al. The role of teachers’ expectations in the association between children’s SES and performance in kindergarten: a moderated mediation analysis. PLoS ONE 7, e34502 (2012).
Matheson, S. M., Asher, L. & Bateson, M. Larger, enriched cages are associated with ‘optimistic’ response biases in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 109, 374–383 (2008).
Wolfensohn, S. et al. Assessment of welfare in zoo animals: towards optimum quality of life. Animals 8, 110 (2018).
Harding, E. J., Paul, E. S. & Mendl, M. Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature 427, 312–312 (2004).
Brydges, N. M., Leach, M., Nicol, K., Wright, R. & Bateson, M. Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive bias in rats. Anim. Behav. 81, 169–175 (2011).
Douglas, C., Bateson, M., Walsh, C., Bédué, A. & Edwards, S. A. Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive biases in pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 139, 65–73 (2012).
Xia, M., Poorthuis, A. M. G. & Thomaes, S. Why do young children overestimate their task performance? A cross-cultural experiment. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 226, 105551 (2023).
Luo, R., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Song, L. Chinese parents’ goals and practices in early childhood. Early Child. Res. Q. 28, 843–857 (2013).
Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. in The Self: Interdisciplinary Approaches (eds Strauss, J. & Goethals, G. R.) 18–48 (Springer, 1991).
Sedikides, C. & Brewer, M. B. Individual Self, Relational Self, Collective Self (Psychology Press, 2015).
Wang, Y. & Ollendick, T. H. A cross-cultural and developmental analysis of self-esteem in Chinese and Western children. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 4, 253–271 (2001).
Wu, P. et al. Similarities and differences in mothers’ parenting of preschoolers in China and the United States. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 481–491 (2002).
Fang, Z., Grant, L. W., Xu, X., Stronge, J. H. & Ward, T. J. An international comparison investigating the relationship between national culture and student achievement. Educ. Asse Eval. Acc. 25, 159–177 (2013).
Doepke, M. & Zilibotti, F. Love, Money, and Parenting: How Economics Explains the Way We Raise Our Kids (Princeton Univ. Press, 2019).
Rogoff, B. Learning by observing and pitching in to family and community endeavors: an orientation. Hum. Dev. 57, 69–81 (2014).
Gaskins, S. in Children’s Engagement in the World: Sociocultural Perspectives (ed. Goncu, A.) 25–60 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1999).
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J. & Ellis-Davies, K. How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? Hum. Nat. 28, 367–394 (2017).
Ames, P. Learning to be responsible: young children transitions outside school. Learn. Cult. Soc. Interact. 2, 143–154 (2013).
Bateson, M., Desire, S., Gartside, S. E. & Wright, G. A. Agitated honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Curr. Biol. 21, 1070–1073 (2011).
Rygula, R., Golebiowska, J., Kregiel, J., Kubik, J. & Popik, P. Effects of optimism on motivation in rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 32 (2015).
Bethell, E. J., Holmes, A., MacLarnon, A. & Semple, S. Cognitive bias in a non-human primate: husbandry procedures influence cognitive indicators of psychological well-being in captive rhesus macaques. Anim. Welf. 21, 185–195 (2012).
Lipko-Speed, A. R. Can young children be more accurate predictors of their recall performance? J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 114, 357–363 (2013).
Forsberg, A., Blume, C. & Cowan, N. The development of metacognitive accuracy in working memory across childhood. Dev. Psychol. 57, 1297–1317 (2021).
Scheier, M. F. & Carver, C. S. Optimism, coping, and health: assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol. 4, 219–247 (1985).
Shepperd, J. A., Waters, E., Weinstein, N. D. & Klein, W. M. P. A primer on unrealistic optimism. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 232–237 (2015).
Lockhart, K. L., Chang, B. & Story, T. Young children’s beliefs about the stability of traits: protective optimism? Child. Dev. 73, 1408–1430 (2002).
Bateson, M. Optimistic and pessimistic biases: a primer for behavioural ecologists. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 12, 115–121 (2016).
Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M. & Phelps, E. A. Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature 450, 102–105 (2007).
Segerstrom, S. C. & Sephton, S. E. Optimistic expectancies and cell-mediated immunity: the role of positive affect. Psychol. Sci. 21, 448–455 (2010).
Xia, M., Poorthuis, A. M. G. & Thomaes, S. Children’s overestimation of performance across age, task, and historical time: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 95, 1001–1022 (2023).
Rhodes, M., Leslie, S.-J., Yee, K. M. & Saunders, K. Subtle linguistic cues increase girls’ engagement in science. Psychol. Sci. 30, 455–466 (2019).
Miller, D. I., Nolla, K. M., Eagly, A. H. & Uttal, D. H. The development of children’s gender-science stereotypes: a meta-analysis of 5 decades of U.S. draw-a-scientist studies. Child. Dev. 89, 1943–1955 (2018).
Peterson, C. & Seligman, M. E. P. Values in action (VIA) classification of strengths in A Life Worth Living: Contributions to Positive Psychology (eds Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Selega, I.) 29–48 (2006).
Bruininks, P. & Malle, B. F. Distinguishing hope from optimism and related affective states. Motiv. Emot. 29, 324–352 (2005).
Fleur, D. S., Bredeweg, B. & van den Bos, W. Metacognition: ideas and insights from neuro- and educational sciences. NPJ Sci. Learn. 6, 1–11 (2021).
Lavis, L. & Mahy, C. E. V. “I’ll remember everything no matter what!”: the role of metacognitive abilities in the development of young children’s prospective memory. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 207, 105117 (2021).
Schraw, G. A conceptual analysis of five measures of metacognitive monitoring. Metacognit. Learn. 4, 33–45 (2009).
Shtulman, A. & Carey, S. Improbable or impossible? How children reason about the possibility of extraordinary events. Child. Dev. 78, 1015–1032 (2007).
Shtulman, A. & Phillips, J. Differentiating “could” from “should”: developmental changes in modal cognition. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 165, 161–182 (2018).
Kushnir, T. How children learn to transcend limits: developmental pathways to possibility beliefs. Possibility Stud. Soc. 1, 451–460 (2023).
Leonard, J. A., Lee, Y. & Schulz, L. E. Infants make more attempts to achieve a goal when they see adults persist. Science 357, 1290–1294 (2017).
Leonard, J. A., Garcia, A. & Schulz, L. E. How adults’ actions, outcomes, and testimony affect preschoolers’ persistence. Child. Dev. 91, 1254–1271 (2020).
Shachnai, R., Kushnir, T. & Bian, L. Walking in her shoes: pretending to be a woman role model increases young girls’ persistence in science. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1818–1827 (2022).
Silver, I. M. & Shaw, A. Pint-sized public relations: the development of reputation management. Trends Cognit. Sci. 22, 277–279 (2018).
Asaba, M. & Gweon, H. Young children infer and manage what others think about them. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2105642119 (2022).
Ma, F., Zeng, D., Xu, F., Compton, B. J. & Heyman, G. D. Delay of gratification as reputation management. Psychol. Sci. 31, 1174–1182 (2020).
Engelmann, J. M. & Rapp, D. J. The influence of reputational concerns on children’s prosociality. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 92–95 (2018).
Thomaes, S., Brummelman, E. & Sedikides, C. Why most children think well of themselves. Child. Dev. 88, 1873–1884 (2017).
Elliot, A. J. & Church, M. A. A motivational analysis of defensive pessimism and self-handicapping. J. Pers. 71, 369–396 (2003).
Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. in Cross-Cultural Differences in Perspectives on the Self (eds Murphy-Berman, V. & Berman, J. J.) 18–74 (Univ. Nebraska Press, 2003).
Cameron, C. A., Lau, C., Fu, G. & Lee, K. Development of children’s moral evaluations of modesty and self-promotion in diverse cultural settings. J. Moral. Educ. 41, 61–78 (2012).
Goupil, L., Romand-Monnier, M. & Kouider, S. Infants ask for help when they know they don’t know. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 3492–3496 (2016).
Hembacher, E. & Ghetti, S. Don’t look at my answer: subjective uncertainty underlies preschoolers’ exclusion of their least accurate memories. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1768–1776 (2014).
Gonzales, C. R., Merculief, A., McClelland, M. M. & Ghetti, S. The development of uncertainty monitoring during kindergarten: change and longitudinal relations with executive function and vocabulary in children from low-income backgrounds. Child. Dev. 93, 524–539 (2022).
Williams, N. A., Davis, G., Hancock, M. & Phipps, S. Optimism and pessimism in children with cancer and healthy children: confirmatory factor analysis of the youth Life Orientation Test and relations with health-related quality of life. J. Pediatric Psychol. 35, 672–682 (2010).
Lemola, S. et al. A new measure for dispositional optimism and pessimism in young children. Eur. J. Pers. 24, 71–84 (2010).
Fischer, M. & Leitenberg, H. Optimism and pessimism in elementary school-aged children. Child. Dev. 57, 241–248 (1986).
Usán, P., Salavera, C. & Quílez-Robres, A. Self-efficacy, optimism, and academic performance as psychoeducational variables: mediation approach in students. Children 9, 420 (2022).
Gordeeva, T., Sheldon, K. & Sychev, O. Linking academic performance to optimistic attributional style: attributions following positive events matter most. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 35, 21–48 (2020).
Ey, S. et al. A new measure of children’s optimism and pessimism: the youth Life Orientation Test. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 548–558 (2005).
Seligman, M. E. P. Learned Optimism: How to Change Your Mind and Your Life (Knopf Doubleday, 2006).
Kassai, R., Futo, J., Demetrovics, Z. & Takacs, Z. K. A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence on the near-and far-transfer effects among children’s executive function skills. Psychol. Bull. 145, 165 (2019).
Walton, G. M. & Yeager, D. S. Seed and soil: psychological affordances in contexts help to explain where wise interventions succeed or fail. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 29, 219–226 (2020).
King, L. S., Humphreys, K. L. & Gotlib, I. H. The neglect–enrichment continuum: characterizing variation in early caregiving environments. Dev. Rev. 51, 109–122 (2019).
Ursache, A. & Noble, K. G. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology 53, 71–82 (2016).
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D. & Nisbett, R. E. Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: a test of the ‘overjustification’ hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 28, 129–137 (1973).
Wang, E., Radovanovic, M., Sommerville, J. & Leonard, J. A. Practice what you preach: consistent messages about the value of effort foster children’s persistence. Proc. 46th Annual Conf. Cognitive Science Society 46, 4091–4098 (2024).
Schonert-Reichl, K. A. et al. Enhancing cognitive and social–emotional development through a simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Psychol. 51, 52–66 (2015).
Yeager, D. S. et al. A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature 573, 364–369 (2019).
Yeager, D. S. et al. Teacher mindsets help explain where a growth-mindset intervention does and doesn’t work. Psychol. Sci. 33, 18–32 (2022).
Munakata, Y. & Michaelson, L. E. Executive functions in social context: implications for conceptualizing, measuring, and supporting developmental trajectories. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 3, 139–163 (2021).
Acknowledgements
The authors thank members of the Leonard Learning Lab, the Toronto Early Cognition Lab and J.A.L.’s writing group, as well as A. Mackey and T. Kushnir, for helpful comments on this manuscript. This research was supported by a Jacobs Foundation Research Fellowship awarded to J.A.L.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
The authors contributed equally to all aspects of the article.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Lori Markson, who co-reviewed with Laura Hennefield; Gail Heyman and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Leonard, J.A., Sommerville, J.A. A unified account of why optimism declines in childhood. Nat Rev Psychol 4, 35–48 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00384-z
Accepted:
Published:
Issue date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00384-z