Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Perspective
  • Published:

A unified account of why optimism declines in childhood

Abstract

Optimism is linked to a range of positive social and cognitive outcomes across development. Yet decades of work in psychological science has revealed that optimism declines throughout early childhood. Despite this well-documented decline, there is no agreed-upon theory that accounts for developmental changes in optimism. In this Perspective, we synthesize cognitive, computational, social and neural evidence and discuss three candidate mechanisms that might underlie declines in optimism with age: learning from experience, changing theories of success and wishful thinking, and shifts in valenced learning biases. We argue that declining optimism across childhood is best characterized by an account that integrates these theories. Specifically, we suggest that environmental factors impact the pace at which children’s theories and valenced learning biases change with age, and consequently the rate at which their optimism declines. This account suggests that optimism should be conceptualized as an adaptive bias that signals the nature of one’s environment and leads to specific recommendations for future lines of enquiry.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Empirical evidence for children’s optimism bias.
Fig. 2: Three candidate mechanisms for why optimism declines with age.
Fig. 3: Effects of environment on developmental trajectories of declines in optimism with age.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Flavell, J. H., Friedrichs, A. G. & Hoyt, J. D. Developmental changes in memorization processes. Cognit. Psychol. 1, 324–340 (1970).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J. & Merriman, W. E. Persistent overconfidence despite practice: the role of task experience in preschoolers’ recall predictions. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 103, 152–166 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Yussen, S. R. & Levy, V. M. Developmental changes in predicting one’s own span of short-term memory. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 19, 502–508 (1975).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Zhang, X., Carrillo, B. & Leonard, J. A. Children predict improvement on novel motor tasks. Preprint at OSF https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/WTYXN (2024).

  5. Coote, K. & Livesey, D. Optimism bias in children’s motor performance expectations. Australian Educ. Dev. Psychol. 16, 52–61 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Plumert, J. M. Relations between children’s overestimation of their physical abilities and accident proneness. Dev. Psychol. 31, 866–876 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Schneider, W. Performance prediction in young children: effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Dev. Sci. 1, 291–297 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Parsons, J. E. & Ruble, D. N. The development of achievement-related expectancies. Child. Dev. 48, 1075–1079 (1977).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Hennefield, L. & Markson, L. The development of optimistic expectations in young children. Cognit. Dev. 63, 101201 (2022).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Wente, A. O. et al. Young children are wishful thinkers: the development of wishful thinking in 3‐ to 10‐year‐old children. Child. Dev. 91, 1166–1182 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Lockhart, K. L., Goddu, M. K. & Keil, F. C. Overoptimism about future knowledge: early arrogance? J. Posit. Psychol. 12, 36–46 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. Dispositional optimism. Trends Cognit. Sci. 18, 293–299 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Chang, E. C., Chang, R. & Sanna, L. J. Optimism, pessimism, and motivation: relations to adjustment. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 3, 494–506 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Rincón Uribe, F. A., Neira Espejo, C. A. & Pedroso, J. D. S. The role of optimism in adolescent mental health: a systematic review. J. Happiness Stud. 23, 815–845 (2022).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Lu, M. S., Hennefield, L., Tillman, R. & Markson, L. Optimistic children engage in more constructive risk-taking behaviors. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 47, 72–81 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Schulz, L. The origins of inquiry: inductive inference and exploration in early childhood. Trends Cognit. Sci. 16, 382–389 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Bonawitz, E. B., van Schijndel, T. J. P., Friel, D. & Schulz, L. Children balance theories and evidence in exploration, explanation, and learning. Cognit. Psychol. 64, 215–234 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Gopnik, A. The scientist as child. Philosophy Sci. 63, 485–514 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Gweon, H. Inferential social learning: cognitive foundations of human social learning and teaching. Trends Cognit. Sci. 25, 896–910 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Kruger, J. & Dunning, D. Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 1121–1134 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Adolph, K. E. Learning to move. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 213–218 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Adolph, K. E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Ishak, S., Karasik, L. B. & Lobo, S. A. Locomotor experience and use of social information are posture specific. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1705–1714 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Zhao, X. et al. Culture moderates the relationship between self-control ability and free will beliefs in childhood. Cognition 210, 104609 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Leonard, J. A., Cordrey, S. R., Liu, H. Z. & Mackey, A. P. Young children calibrate effort based on the trajectory of their performance. Dev. Psychol. 59, 609–619 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Cimpian, A. in Handbook of Competence and Motivation, Second Edition: Theory and Application (eds Elliot, A. J., Dweck, C. S. & Yeager, D. S.) 387–407 (Guilford Press, 2017).

  26. Butler, R. in Handbook of Competence and Motivation (eds Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S.) 202–221 (Guilford Press, 2005).

  27. Rosenholtz, S. J. & Simpson, C. The formation of ability conceptions: developmental trend or social construction? Rev. Educ. Res. 54, 31–63 (1984).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Stipek, D. J. & Daniels, D. H. Declining perceptions of competence: a consequence of changes in the child or in the educational environment? J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 352–356 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kamins, M. L. & Dweck, C. S. Person versus process praise and criticism: implications for contingent self-worth and coping. Dev. Psychol. 35, 835–847 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cimpian, A. The impact of generic language about ability on children’s achievement motivation. Dev. Psychol. 46, 1333–1340 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Cimpian, A., Mu, Y. & Erickson, L. C. Who is good at this game? Linking an activity to a social category undermines children’s achievement. Psychol. Sci. 23, 533–541 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Muenks, K., Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S. I can do this! The development and calibration of children’s expectations for success and competence beliefs. Dev. Rev. 48, 24–39 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S. & Wigfield, A. Changes in children’s self-competence and values: gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child. Dev. 73, 509–527 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Moutsiana, C. et al. Human development of the ability to learn from bad news. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 16396–16401 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Zhang, X., McDougle, S. D. & Leonard, J. A. People accurately predict the shape of skill learning curves. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rwuc9 (2024).

  36. Carroll, P. J., Sweeny, K. & Shepperd, J. A. Forsaking optimism. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 10, 56–73 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Sweeny, K. & Krizan, Z. Sobering up: a quantitative review of temporal declines in expectations. Psychol. Bull. 139, 702–724 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Shepperd, J. A., Ouellette, J. A. & Fernandez, J. K. Abandoning unrealistic optimism: performance estimates and the temporal proximity of self-relevant feedback. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 844–855 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Sweeny, K., Carroll, P. J. & Shepperd, J. A. Is optimism always best? Future outlooks and preparedness. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 302–306 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Nicholls, J. G. The development of the concepts of effort and ability, perception of academic attainment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require more ability. Child. Dev. 49, 800–814 (1978).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Chapman, M. & Skinner, E. A. Children’s agency beliefs, cognitive performance, and conceptions of effort and ability: individual and developmental differences. Child. Dev. 60, 1229–1238 (1989).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Heyman, G. D., Gee, C. L. & Giles, J. W. Preschool children’s reasoning about ability. Child. Dev. 74, 516–534 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Miller, A. A developmental study of the cognitive basis of performance impairment after failure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 49, 529–538 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Nicholls, J. G. & Miller, A. T. Reasoning about the ability of self and others: a developmental study. Child. Dev. 55, 1990–1999 (1984).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Folmer, A. S. et al. Age-related changes in children’s understanding of effort and ability: implications for attribution theory and motivation. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 99, 114–134 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ganesan, K. & Steinbeis, N. Effort-related decision-making and its underlying processes during childhood. Dev. Psychol. 57, 1487–1496 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hiebert, E. H., Winograd, P. N. & Danner, F. W. Children’s attributions for failure and success in different aspects of reading. J. Educ. Psychol. 76, 1139–1148 (1984).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Nicholls, J. G. Development of perception of own attainment and causal attributions for success and failure in reading. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 94–99 (1979).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Whitehead, G. I. III, Anderson, W. F. & Mitchell, K. D. Children’s causal attributions to self and other as a function of outcome and task. J. Educ. Psychol. 79, 192–194 (1987).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Kushnir, T., Gopnik, A., Chernyak, N., Seiver, E. & Wellman, H. M. Developing intuitions about free will between ages four and six. Cognition 138, 79–101 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Stipek, D. J. & Hoffman, J. M. Development of children’s performance-related judgments. Child. Dev. 51, 912–914 (1980).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Stipek, D. J., Roberts, T. A. & Sanborn, M. E. Preschool-age children’s performance expectations for themselves and another child as a function of the incentive value of success and the salience of past performance. Child. Dev. 55, 1983–1989 (1984).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Xia, M., Poorthuis, A. M. G., Zhou, Q. & Thomaes, S. Young children’s overestimation of performance: a cross‐cultural comparison. Child. Dev. 93, e207–e221 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Bernard, S., Clément, F. & Mercier, H. Wishful thinking in preschoolers. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 141, 267–274 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Leonard, J. A., Bennett-Pierre, G. & Gweon, H. Who is better? Preschoolers infer relative competence based on efficiency of process and quality of outcome. Proc. 41st Annu. Conf. Cognit. Sci. Soc. 639–645 (2019).

  56. Muradoglu, M. & Cimpian, A. Children’s intuitive theories of academic performance. Child. Dev. 91, e902–e918 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Bamford, C. & Lagattuta, K. H. Optimism and wishful thinking: consistency across populations in children’s expectations for the future. Child. Dev. 91, 1116–1134 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Habicht, J., Bowler, A., Moses-Payne, M. E., & Hauser, T. U. Children are full of optimism, but those rose-tinted glasses are fading-Reduced learning from negative outcomes drives hyperoptimism in children. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 151, 1843–1853 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Dohmen, T., Quercia, S. & Willrodt, J. On the psychology of the relation between optimism and risk taking. J. Risk Uncertain. 67, 193–214 (2023).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Sharot, T. The optimism bias. Curr. Biol. 21, R941–R945 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Lefebvre, G., Lebreton, M., Meyniel, F., Bourgeois-Gironde, S. & Palminteri, S. Behavioural and neural characterization of optimistic reinforcement learning. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 1–9 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Sharot, T., Korn, C. W. & Dolan, R. J. How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1475–1479 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Sharot, T., Guitart-Masip, M., Korn, C. W., Chowdhury, R. & Dolan, R. J. How dopamine enhances an optimism bias in humans. Curr. Biol. 16, 1477–1481 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Williams, S. M. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Widespread origin of the primate mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb. Cortex 8, 321–345 (1998).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Hoops, D. & Flores, C. Making dopamine connections in adolescence. Trends Neurosci. 40, 709–719 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Braams, B. R., van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Peper, J. S. & Crone, E. A. Longitudinal changes in adolescent risk-taking: a comprehensive study of neural responses to rewards, pubertal development, and risk-taking behavior. J. Neurosci. 35, 7226–7238 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M. & Casey, B. J. A time of change: behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cogn. 72, 124–133 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Somerville, L. H. & Casey, B. Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 236–241 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Nussenbaum, K. & Hartley, C. A. Developmental change in prefrontal cortex recruitment supports the emergence of value-guided memory. eLife 10, e69796 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Insel, C., Kastman, E. K., Glenn, C. R. & Somerville, L. H. Development of corticostriatal connectivity constrains goal-directed behavior during adolescence. Nat. Commun. 8, 1605 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Silvers, J. A. & Guassi Moreira, J. F. Capacity and tendency: a neuroscientific framework for the study of emotion regulation. Neurosci. Lett. 693, 35–39 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Park, A. T. et al. Early childhood stress is associated with blunted development of ventral tegmental area functional connectivity. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 47, 100909 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Diamond, A. in Principles of Frontal lobe Function Vol. 466 (eds Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T.) 201–225 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2002).

  74. Fiske, A. & Holmboe, K. Neural substrates of early executive function development. Dev. Rev. 52, 42–62 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Sjåstad, H. & Baumeister, R. Fast optimism, slow realism? Causal evidence for a two-step model of future thinking. Cognition 236, 105447 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Nussenbaum, K., Velez, J. A., Washington, B. T., Hamling, H. E. & Hartley, C. A. Flexibility in valenced reinforcement learning computations across development. Child. Dev. 93, 1601–1615 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Mai, X. et al. Brain activity elicited by positive and negative feedback in preschool-aged children. PLoS ONE 6, e18774 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W. & Kaplan, H. S. in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: Foundations 2nd edn (ed. Buss, D. M.) Vol. 1 88–114 (Wiley, 2016).

  79. Nettle, D. & Frankenhuis, W. E. The evolution of life-history theory: a bibliometric analysis of an interdisciplinary research area. Proc. R. Soc. B. 286, 20190040 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Frankenhuis, W. E. & Gopnik, A. Early adversity and the development of explore–exploit tradeoffs. Trends Cognit. Sci. 27, 616–630 (2023).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Eckstein, M. K. et al. The interpretation of computational model parameters depends on the context. eLife 11, e75474 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Nussenbaum, K. & Hartley, C. A. Reinforcement learning across development: what insights can we draw from a decade of research? Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 40, 100733 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Dorfman, H. M., Bhui, R., Hughes, B. L. & Gershman, S. J. Causal inference about good and bad outcomes. Psychol. Sci. 30, 516–525 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Bjorklund, D. F. Why Youth Is Not Wasted on the Young: Immaturity in Human Development (Wiley, 2009).

  85. Tooley, U. A., Bassett, D. S. & Mackey, A. P. Environmental influences on the pace of brain development. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 372–384 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. McDermott, C. L. et al. Early life stress is associated with earlier emergence of permanent molars. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2105304118 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Belsky, J. Early-life adversity accelerates child and adolescent development. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 241–246 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Colich, N. L., Rosen, M. L., Williams, E. S. & McLaughlin, K. A. Biological aging in childhood and adolescence following experiences of threat and deprivation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 146, 721–764 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Heinonen, K. et al. Socioeconomic status in childhood and adulthood: associations with dispositional optimism and pessimism over a 21‐year follow‐up. J. Pers. 74, 1111–1126 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Robb, K. A., Simon, A. E. & Wardle, J. Socioeconomic disparities in optimism and pessimism. Int.J. Behav. Med. 16, 331–338 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Decker, A. L. et al. Striatal and behavioral responses to reward vary by socioeconomic status in adolescents. J. Neurosci. 44, e1633232023 (2024).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  92. Heathcote, A., Brown, S. & Mewhort, D. J. K. The power law repealed: the case for an exponential law of practice. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 7, 185–207 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Thorndike, E. L. The Psychology of Learning Vol. 2 (Teachers College, Columbia Univ., 1913).

  94. Newell, A. & Rosenbloom, P. S. in Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition (ed. Anderson, J. R.) 1–55 (Psychology Press, 1981).

  95. Son, L. K. & Sethi, R. Metacognitive control and optimal learning. Cognit. Sci. 30, 759–774 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Stadler, M. A., & Frensch, P. A. (eds) Handbook of implicit learning (Sage, 1998).

  97. Szulewski, A., Roth, N. & Howes, D. The use of task-evoked pupillary response as an objective measure of cognitive load in novices and trained physicians: a new tool for the assessment of expertise. Acad. Med. 90, 981–987 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Bian, L. & Leslie, S.-J. Gender stereotypes about intellectual ability emerge early and influence children’s interests. Science 355, 389–391 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., McAdoo, H. P. & García Coll, C. The home environments of children in the United States part I: variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child. Dev. 72, 1844–1867 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Bradley, R. H. & Corwyn, R. F. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 371–399 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Goudeau, S. et al. Unequal opportunities from the start: socioeconomic disparities in classroom participation in preschool. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 152, 3135–3152 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Hasan, N. & Power, T. G. Optimism and pessimism in children: a study of parenting correlates. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 185–191 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S. in Development of Achievement Motivation Ch. 4 (eds Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S.) 91–120 (Academic Press, 2002).

  104. Häfner, I. et al. STEM motivation and course-taking: bidirectional relationships between parents and adolescents from middle school to college. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA) (2016).

  105. Wang, S., Rubie-Davies, C. M. & Meissel, K. A systematic review of the teacher expectation literature over the past 30 years. Educ. Res. Eval. 24, 124–179 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Poon, K. The impact of socioeconomic status on parental factors in promoting academic achievement in Chinese children. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 75, 102175 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Harvey, D. G. & Slatin, G. T. The relationship between child’s SES and teacher expectations: a test of the middle-class bias hypothesis. Soc. Forces 54, 140–159 (1975).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Speybroeck, S. et al. The role of teachers’ expectations in the association between children’s SES and performance in kindergarten: a moderated mediation analysis. PLoS ONE 7, e34502 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Matheson, S. M., Asher, L. & Bateson, M. Larger, enriched cages are associated with ‘optimistic’ response biases in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 109, 374–383 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Wolfensohn, S. et al. Assessment of welfare in zoo animals: towards optimum quality of life. Animals 8, 110 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. Harding, E. J., Paul, E. S. & Mendl, M. Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature 427, 312–312 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Brydges, N. M., Leach, M., Nicol, K., Wright, R. & Bateson, M. Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive bias in rats. Anim. Behav. 81, 169–175 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Douglas, C., Bateson, M., Walsh, C., Bédué, A. & Edwards, S. A. Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive biases in pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 139, 65–73 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Xia, M., Poorthuis, A. M. G. & Thomaes, S. Why do young children overestimate their task performance? A cross-cultural experiment. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 226, 105551 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Luo, R., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Song, L. Chinese parents’ goals and practices in early childhood. Early Child. Res. Q. 28, 843–857 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. in The Self: Interdisciplinary Approaches (eds Strauss, J. & Goethals, G. R.) 18–48 (Springer, 1991).

  117. Sedikides, C. & Brewer, M. B. Individual Self, Relational Self, Collective Self (Psychology Press, 2015).

  118. Wang, Y. & Ollendick, T. H. A cross-cultural and developmental analysis of self-esteem in Chinese and Western children. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 4, 253–271 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Wu, P. et al. Similarities and differences in mothers’ parenting of preschoolers in China and the United States. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 481–491 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  120. Fang, Z., Grant, L. W., Xu, X., Stronge, J. H. & Ward, T. J. An international comparison investigating the relationship between national culture and student achievement. Educ. Asse Eval. Acc. 25, 159–177 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Doepke, M. & Zilibotti, F. Love, Money, and Parenting: How Economics Explains the Way We Raise Our Kids (Princeton Univ. Press, 2019).

  122. Rogoff, B. Learning by observing and pitching in to family and community endeavors: an orientation. Hum. Dev. 57, 69–81 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  123. Gaskins, S. in Children’s Engagement in the World: Sociocultural Perspectives (ed. Goncu, A.) 25–60 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1999).

  124. Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J. & Ellis-Davies, K. How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? Hum. Nat. 28, 367–394 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  125. Ames, P. Learning to be responsible: young children transitions outside school. Learn. Cult. Soc. Interact. 2, 143–154 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Bateson, M., Desire, S., Gartside, S. E. & Wright, G. A. Agitated honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Curr. Biol. 21, 1070–1073 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  127. Rygula, R., Golebiowska, J., Kregiel, J., Kubik, J. & Popik, P. Effects of optimism on motivation in rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 32 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  128. Bethell, E. J., Holmes, A., MacLarnon, A. & Semple, S. Cognitive bias in a non-human primate: husbandry procedures influence cognitive indicators of psychological well-being in captive rhesus macaques. Anim. Welf. 21, 185–195 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  129. Lipko-Speed, A. R. Can young children be more accurate predictors of their recall performance? J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 114, 357–363 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  130. Forsberg, A., Blume, C. & Cowan, N. The development of metacognitive accuracy in working memory across childhood. Dev. Psychol. 57, 1297–1317 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  131. Scheier, M. F. & Carver, C. S. Optimism, coping, and health: assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol. 4, 219–247 (1985).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  132. Shepperd, J. A., Waters, E., Weinstein, N. D. & Klein, W. M. P. A primer on unrealistic optimism. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 232–237 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  133. Lockhart, K. L., Chang, B. & Story, T. Young children’s beliefs about the stability of traits: protective optimism? Child. Dev. 73, 1408–1430 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  134. Bateson, M. Optimistic and pessimistic biases: a primer for behavioural ecologists. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 12, 115–121 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  135. Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M. & Phelps, E. A. Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature 450, 102–105 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  136. Segerstrom, S. C. & Sephton, S. E. Optimistic expectancies and cell-mediated immunity: the role of positive affect. Psychol. Sci. 21, 448–455 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  137. Xia, M., Poorthuis, A. M. G. & Thomaes, S. Children’s overestimation of performance across age, task, and historical time: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 95, 1001–1022 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  138. Rhodes, M., Leslie, S.-J., Yee, K. M. & Saunders, K. Subtle linguistic cues increase girls’ engagement in science. Psychol. Sci. 30, 455–466 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  139. Miller, D. I., Nolla, K. M., Eagly, A. H. & Uttal, D. H. The development of children’s gender-science stereotypes: a meta-analysis of 5 decades of U.S. draw-a-scientist studies. Child. Dev. 89, 1943–1955 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  140. Peterson, C. & Seligman, M. E. P. Values in action (VIA) classification of strengths in A Life Worth Living: Contributions to Positive Psychology (eds Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Selega, I.) 29–48 (2006).

  141. Bruininks, P. & Malle, B. F. Distinguishing hope from optimism and related affective states. Motiv. Emot. 29, 324–352 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  142. Fleur, D. S., Bredeweg, B. & van den Bos, W. Metacognition: ideas and insights from neuro- and educational sciences. NPJ Sci. Learn. 6, 1–11 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  143. Lavis, L. & Mahy, C. E. V. “I’ll remember everything no matter what!”: the role of metacognitive abilities in the development of young children’s prospective memory. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 207, 105117 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  144. Schraw, G. A conceptual analysis of five measures of metacognitive monitoring. Metacognit. Learn. 4, 33–45 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  145. Shtulman, A. & Carey, S. Improbable or impossible? How children reason about the possibility of extraordinary events. Child. Dev. 78, 1015–1032 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  146. Shtulman, A. & Phillips, J. Differentiating “could” from “should”: developmental changes in modal cognition. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 165, 161–182 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  147. Kushnir, T. How children learn to transcend limits: developmental pathways to possibility beliefs. Possibility Stud. Soc. 1, 451–460 (2023).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  148. Leonard, J. A., Lee, Y. & Schulz, L. E. Infants make more attempts to achieve a goal when they see adults persist. Science 357, 1290–1294 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  149. Leonard, J. A., Garcia, A. & Schulz, L. E. How adults’ actions, outcomes, and testimony affect preschoolers’ persistence. Child. Dev. 91, 1254–1271 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  150. Shachnai, R., Kushnir, T. & Bian, L. Walking in her shoes: pretending to be a woman role model increases young girls’ persistence in science. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1818–1827 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  151. Silver, I. M. & Shaw, A. Pint-sized public relations: the development of reputation management. Trends Cognit. Sci. 22, 277–279 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  152. Asaba, M. & Gweon, H. Young children infer and manage what others think about them. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2105642119 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  153. Ma, F., Zeng, D., Xu, F., Compton, B. J. & Heyman, G. D. Delay of gratification as reputation management. Psychol. Sci. 31, 1174–1182 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  154. Engelmann, J. M. & Rapp, D. J. The influence of reputational concerns on children’s prosociality. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 92–95 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  155. Thomaes, S., Brummelman, E. & Sedikides, C. Why most children think well of themselves. Child. Dev. 88, 1873–1884 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  156. Elliot, A. J. & Church, M. A. A motivational analysis of defensive pessimism and self-handicapping. J. Pers. 71, 369–396 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  157. Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. in Cross-Cultural Differences in Perspectives on the Self (eds Murphy-Berman, V. & Berman, J. J.) 18–74 (Univ. Nebraska Press, 2003).

  158. Cameron, C. A., Lau, C., Fu, G. & Lee, K. Development of children’s moral evaluations of modesty and self-promotion in diverse cultural settings. J. Moral. Educ. 41, 61–78 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  159. Goupil, L., Romand-Monnier, M. & Kouider, S. Infants ask for help when they know they don’t know. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 3492–3496 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  160. Hembacher, E. & Ghetti, S. Don’t look at my answer: subjective uncertainty underlies preschoolers’ exclusion of their least accurate memories. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1768–1776 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  161. Gonzales, C. R., Merculief, A., McClelland, M. M. & Ghetti, S. The development of uncertainty monitoring during kindergarten: change and longitudinal relations with executive function and vocabulary in children from low-income backgrounds. Child. Dev. 93, 524–539 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  162. Williams, N. A., Davis, G., Hancock, M. & Phipps, S. Optimism and pessimism in children with cancer and healthy children: confirmatory factor analysis of the youth Life Orientation Test and relations with health-related quality of life. J. Pediatric Psychol. 35, 672–682 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  163. Lemola, S. et al. A new measure for dispositional optimism and pessimism in young children. Eur. J. Pers. 24, 71–84 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  164. Fischer, M. & Leitenberg, H. Optimism and pessimism in elementary school-aged children. Child. Dev. 57, 241–248 (1986).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  165. Usán, P., Salavera, C. & Quílez-Robres, A. Self-efficacy, optimism, and academic performance as psychoeducational variables: mediation approach in students. Children 9, 420 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  166. Gordeeva, T., Sheldon, K. & Sychev, O. Linking academic performance to optimistic attributional style: attributions following positive events matter most. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 35, 21–48 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  167. Ey, S. et al. A new measure of children’s optimism and pessimism: the youth Life Orientation Test. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 548–558 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  168. Seligman, M. E. P. Learned Optimism: How to Change Your Mind and Your Life (Knopf Doubleday, 2006).

  169. Kassai, R., Futo, J., Demetrovics, Z. & Takacs, Z. K. A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence on the near-and far-transfer effects among children’s executive function skills. Psychol. Bull. 145, 165 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  170. Walton, G. M. & Yeager, D. S. Seed and soil: psychological affordances in contexts help to explain where wise interventions succeed or fail. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 29, 219–226 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  171. King, L. S., Humphreys, K. L. & Gotlib, I. H. The neglect–enrichment continuum: characterizing variation in early caregiving environments. Dev. Rev. 51, 109–122 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  172. Ursache, A. & Noble, K. G. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology 53, 71–82 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  173. Lepper, M. R., Greene, D. & Nisbett, R. E. Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: a test of the ‘overjustification’ hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 28, 129–137 (1973).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  174. Wang, E., Radovanovic, M., Sommerville, J. & Leonard, J. A. Practice what you preach: consistent messages about the value of effort foster children’s persistence. Proc. 46th Annual Conf. Cognitive Science Society 46, 4091–4098 (2024).

    Google Scholar 

  175. Schonert-Reichl, K. A. et al. Enhancing cognitive and social–emotional development through a simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Psychol. 51, 52–66 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  176. Yeager, D. S. et al. A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature 573, 364–369 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  177. Yeager, D. S. et al. Teacher mindsets help explain where a growth-mindset intervention does and doesn’t work. Psychol. Sci. 33, 18–32 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  178. Munakata, Y. & Michaelson, L. E. Executive functions in social context: implications for conceptualizing, measuring, and supporting developmental trajectories. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 3, 139–163 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank members of the Leonard Learning Lab, the Toronto Early Cognition Lab and J.A.L.’s writing group, as well as A. Mackey and T. Kushnir, for helpful comments on this manuscript. This research was supported by a Jacobs Foundation Research Fellowship awarded to J.A.L.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors contributed equally to all aspects of the article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julia A. Leonard.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Lori Markson, who co-reviewed with Laura Hennefield; Gail Heyman and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Leonard, J.A., Sommerville, J.A. A unified account of why optimism declines in childhood. Nat Rev Psychol 4, 35–48 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00384-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00384-z

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing