Abstract
Neuronal activity in the hippocampus is critical for many types of memory acquisition and retrieval and influences an animal’s response to stress. Moreover, the molecularly distinct principal neurons of hippocampal area CA2 are required for social recognition memory and aggression in mice. To interrogate the effects of stress on CA2-dependent behaviors, we chemogenetically manipulated neuronal activity in vivo during an acute, socially derived stressor and tested whether memory for the defeat was influenced. One day after an acute social defeat (aSD), defeated mice spent significantly less time investigating another mouse when compared to non-defeated control mice. We found that this avoidant phenotype persisted for up to one month following a single defeat encounter. When CA2 pyramidal neuron activity was inhibited with Gi-DREADD receptors during the defeat, subject mice exhibited a significantly higher amount of social avoidance one day later when compared to defeated littermates not expressing DREADDs. Moreover, CA2 inhibition during defeat caused a reduction in submissive defense behaviors in response to aggression. In vitro electrophysiology and tracing experiments revealed a circuit wherein CA2 neurons connect to caudal CA1 projection neurons that, in turn, project to corticolimbic regions including the anterior cingulate cortex. Finally, socially avoidant, defeated mice exhibited significant reductions in cFos expression in caudal hippocampal and limbic brain areas during a social investigation task 24 h after aSD. Taken together, these results indicate that CA2 neuronal activity is required to support behavioral resilience following an acute social stressor and that submissive defensive behavior during the defeat (vs. fleeing) is a predictor of future resilience to social stress. Furthermore, CA2 preferentially targets a population of caudal CA1 projection neurons that contact cortical brain regions where activity is modulated by an acute social stressor.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The acute stress response is conserved across species and allows an organism to learn from and adapt to a myriad of potentially threatening environmental stimuli [1,2,3,4]. Recent studies have highlighted variability in physiological and behavioral effects following exposure to a stressor. In humans, for example, a traumatic stressor can cause post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), which can manifest as hyperarousal, social deficits or aggression with patients often exhibiting comorbid disorders like major depression [5, 6]. However, only a subset of humans develop PTSD [7]. Similarly, only a subset of mice exposed to the chronic social defeat stress (CSDS) behavioral paradigm, in which subject mice are challenged with multiple stressful encounters with an aggressor mouse over consecutive days, develop social avoidance [8,9,10]. In the vulnerable, “susceptible”, mice, this chronic stress has been shown to cause long lasting changes in brain regions like the hippocampus [8, 11], striatum, and hypothalamus [12] and alter defensive behaviors in susceptible animals, while a more “resilient” population of mice exhibit minimal effects of the chronic social stressor [13]. Additionally, chronic stressors like CSDS, restraint and early-life social stress can lead to gross morphological changes in limbic regions including the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate [13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20]. Recent work has highlighted the transcriptomic and structural differences between vulnerable and resilient groups in an effort to better understand the mechanisms underlying stress susceptibility. For example, both the behavioral responses in a light/dark place preference assay and glucocorticoid-dependent modulation of excitatory signaling have to been shown to be predictive of which mice will go on to exhibit deficits following CSDS [10, 21]. Interestingly, it is the mice that exhibit stronger defensive behaviors over repeated encounters with an aggressive mouse, that go on to show social investigative levels similar to non-defeated control mice [13].
Within the hippocampus, the morphologically and functionally distinct hippocampal area CA2 has been shown to be required for social behaviors like social recognition memory [22,23,24,25,26,27], allowing animals to form and maintain social partnerships and discern a safe, familiar animal from a potential threat. This ability to discriminate between a familiar and a novel conspecific, or at least show a preference for novel mice, engages downstream targets of CA2, such as caudal CA1 projection neurons, and results in activation of corticolimbic regions like the nucleus accumbens and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) [28,29,30]. Importantly, inhibition of CA2 during a social discrimination task does not affect overall sociability (social exploration) [22, 28]. CA2 activity has also been shown to positively regulate aggression towards a conspecific via direct, excitatory projections to the dorsal lateral septum, and chemogenetic disruption of CA2 activity adversely affects context dependent fear memory in female mice [31, 32]. Furthermore, area CA2 expresses a higher density of the mineralocorticoid stress hormone receptor (MRs) when compared to the surrounding hippocampal subfields of dentate gyrus (DG), CA3 and CA1 [33, 34]. Conditionally knocking out MRs in CA2 neurons not only alters the unique molecular profile of CA2 neurons, but is also sufficient to disrupt social behavior and promote hyperarousal in response to a novel object, behaviors which are also seen in vulnerable animals following restraint stress and the CSDS paradigm [35].
Although a stressful memory may play a crucial role in an animal’s ability to predict a future threat, cognitive flexibility is also important when adapting to a changing environment. Hippocampal regions like the DG and ventral hippocampus, as well as cortical areas like the prefrontal cortex, are required for reversal learning which, in turn, may underlie a more resilient phenotype when animals are faced with a stressor [36,37,38]. In fact, some research suggests that stress may facilitate this adaptive flexibility; although the underlying mechanisms are unclear, it is likely that hippocampal activity during reversal learning plays a role in the variable responses observed in a given population following either acute or chronic stress [39].
Given hippocampal area CA2’s established role in adaptive behaviors like social processing, aggression and contextual fear memory, and the functional role of mineralocorticoid stress hormone receptors in maintaining CA2’s molecular identity and associated behaviors, we sought to better understand the role of CA2 in stress resilience and susceptibility using an acute model of social defeat stress (aSD). Although this model has been well established in Syrian Hamsters [40, 41], recent evidence has highlighted social investigative deficits in mice following a single bout of social defeat by an aggressor [42, 43]. Using a combination of electrophysiological, immunohistochemical, behavioral, and in vivo chemogenetic techniques, we investigated whether the hippocampus, specifically area CA2, plays a role in social stress resilience and susceptibility in response to an acute social defeat stress. Lastly, given the lack of direct cortical projections from rostrodorsal hippocampus, we investigated how information from CA2 is routed to corticolimbic brain regions required for social processing.
Materials and methods
Animals and handling
All procedures were approved by the NIEHS Animal Care and Use Committee and were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines for care and use of animals. Male and female Amigo2-icreERT2 mice were bred in house on a B6 background as described in [44] CD1 male mice were received from Charles River laboratories at 1–2 months of age. C57BL/6 J (Jackson Laboratories) adult male mice (11–25 weeks of age) were used for some behavioral assays. Male and female subject mice were group housed in the same room on a 12:12 light dark cycle. CD1 male (aggressor) mice were single housed in a separate room. All behavioral assays were performed during the light cycle, between 8 AM and 3 PM. Male and female mice were used for electrophysiological and circuit tracing experiments and behavioral testing was conducted using adult male mice.
Intracranial injections of viral vectors and tamoxifen dosing
Adult mice were temporarily anesthetized using isoflurane (0.5% at 0.8 L/min) and then received an intraperitoneal injection of a ketamine/xylazine cocktail (10 mg/ml and 0.7 mg/ml, respectively) at 0.1cc/10 g body weight. Ophthalmic ointment was then applied to the eyes and the scalp was shaved and treated with two applications of betadine. Lidocaine (0.05 cc of 0.5% Marcaine) was injected subcutaneously at the incision site. After mice were secured in the stereotaxic instrument, an incision was made along the midline exposing the skull surface and the dura was removed.
For Gi-DREADD studies, 500 nl of AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry virus (Addgene #44362-AAV5) was infused bilaterally at 100 nl/min. into rostral CA2 (rCA2 :: ML: ± 2.4, AP: −2.3, DV: −1.9) in 26 adult (18 cre- and 18 cre + , 15–19 weeks of age) Amigo2-icreERT2 mice using a Hamilton syringe positioned in a syringe pump (KD Scientific) and tube-fed cannulas made in house from blunted 26 G needles (BD). The cannula was left in position for a minimum of 8 min before a slow retraction. The scalp was then sutured, and Buprenorphine-SR was administered subcutaneously (SQ) (0.005 mg/ml, 0.02cc/10 g body weight) followed by the antisedant Atipamezole (0.2 mg/kg, 0.1cc dosed SQ). Animals were allowed to recover on a warming pad until regaining consciousness and returned to the home cage. Two weeks after viral infusion, Amigo2-icreERT2 mice received daily IP injections of tamoxifen (100 mg/kg dissolved in warmed corn oil) for a maximum of 7 days. Two weeks later, cre- and cre+ mice received a single dose of clozapine-n-oxide (CNO, 5 mg/kg dissolved in sterile saline) 45 min prior to the aSD paradigm on day 1. Following the completion of the study, expression, or lack thereof, of the mCherry tagged Gi-DREADD construct was verified in 40 µm sections from cre+ and cre- mice, respectively. Using this pre-determined criteria, 2 cre+ Amigo2-icreERT2 mice were excluded from this study when no mCherry signal was observable in the hippocampus. No cre- Amigo2-icreERT2 mice were excluded from analysis.
For circuit tracing studies involving light-evoked stimulation of rCA2 axons, 500 nl of AAV5-EF1a-DIO-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry virus (Addgene #44361-AAV5) was injected unilaterally at 100 nl/min. in rostral area CA2 (ML: −2.4, AP: −2.3, DV: −1.9) in cre+ Amigo2-icreERT2 mice (N = 20 mice). Two weeks later, infused mice received tamoxifen injections as outlined above. Five weeks after viral infusion, 300 µm coronal hippocampal sections were cut as described below and electrophysiological recordings conducted in caudal CA1 ipsilateral to the viral injection site.
For retrograde tracing experiments, 250 nl of AAVrg-CAG-mCherry virus (Addgene #59462) were intracranially injected unilaterally at 50 nl/min in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC: ML: −0.5, AP: +0.80, DV: −1.75) of Amigo2-icreERT2 mice. A subset of Amigo2-icreERT2 mice (N = 5) also received a 500 nl injection of the channelrhoposin construct (AAV5-EF1a-DIO-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry), allowing for light evoked stimulation of rCA2 axons in cCA1 cells with ACC as a known projection target.
For Gq-DREADD mediated activation of rostral CA2 neurons in vivo, 500 nl of AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM3Dq-mCherry (Addgene #44361-AAV5) was bilaterally injected into rostral CA2 (ML: ± 2.4, AP: −2.3, DV: −1.9) in 7 adult male and female Amigo2-icreERT2 mice (4 cre- mice and 3 cre+ mice). Tamoxifen (100 mg/kg) was delivered IP two weeks later and a single dose of CNO (1 mg/kg) was given three weeks after tamoxifen. Ninety minutes following CNO, mice were transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde and the brains were sectioned for immunofluorescent staining for the immediate early gene cFos.
Behavior and analysis
Group housed, male mice (wildtype C57BL/6 J, Jackson Laboratories or GiDREADD-injected animals), 11–25 weeks of age, were placed in the home cage of adult male CD1 mice (3–10 months of age, Charles River), which had been prescreened for aggression prior to encountering the subject mice. We defined aggression as low latency ( < 20 s) to attack a novel, C57 adult male mouse over successive days. Subject mice remained in the CD1 home cage for a single, 5-min bout of aSD and were then returned to their homecage. Twenty-four hours after the aSD, non-defeated control mice and defeated subject mice were placed in a novel arena that contained a littermate of the CD1 aggressor housed in a wire cup and allowed to explore the arena for five minutes before being returned to their homecage. In order to determine the effect of aSD over time, mice were again placed in the day 2 arena at one week, 4 weeks and, in a subset of mice, 12 weeks, that again contained a littermate of the CD1 aggressor and allowed to explore the context for five minutes. The CD1 littermate mice presented during the day 2 avoidance test were novel to the subject mouse at the 24-h, one week and four-week time points. At the three-month time point, subject mice were presented a CD1 mouse from an early timepoint do to CD1 litter size limitations. Non-defeat control mice were age matched to experimental mice, received no viral injections, no CNO, and had no previous exposure to CD1 mice. Moreover, non-defeat controls mice remained in their home cage prior to the day 2 avoidance test and were not exposed to a novel C57 mouse during day 1 of our paradigm. Video recordings were taken during both the defeat and the investigative encounters for behavioral analysis. Defeat videos were analyzed by an experienced individual, blinded to genotype as applicable, and the following behaviors were scored: amount of direct aggression a subject mouse received (seconds); passive submission (freezing) during aggression + subject flee reactions when facing aggression (seconds); defensive submission, defined as a subject mouse taking a defensive, often upright posture and standing its ground when facing aggression (seconds). Passive and defensive submission scores were later added together in order to calculate total submission during defeat. For day 2 videos, the novel area was divided in half with the half furthest from the contained CD1 mouse defined as the “far zone” and the immediate area (16 cm diameter) surrounding the wire cup defined as the “interaction zone” (Fig. 1a). EthoVision video tracking software (Noldus) was used to score the following subject behaviors during the day 2 avoidance/investigation assay; total distance traveled (cm), time spent in the far zone (seconds), time spent in the interaction zone (duration, seconds), and number of distinct entries in the interaction zone. Avoidant mice were defined as mice that spent more than 200 s of the 300 s screen in the far zone of the arena. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Experimenters were blinded to the genotype of subject mice during the both the defeat and avoidance assays and subsequent analysis.
A subset of adult male C57 mice (N = 11) underwent a two-day fear conditioning protocol. During day one of fear conditioning, mice were individually housed in fear conditioning chambers (Med Associates) for a three-minute habituation period followed by five tone (75db)/shock (0.6 mA) pairings separated by three-minute intervals with the shock delivered immediately following the tone. Freezing behavior during the habituation period and five post-shock intervals was calculated using the Video Freeze program. On day 2, ~24 h after the fear acquisition task, mice were returned to the day 1 context and freezing behavior was monitored across eight minutes, averaged at one-minute intervals, and calculated using Video Freeze. The chambers were cleaned with 75% isopropyl alcohol and five sprays of Simple Green cleaning solution between every trial [45].
Five weeks following a CA2 targeted, intracranial injection of the Gi-DREADD construct (and 3 weeks following the first of the IP injections of tamoxifen) Amigo2-icreERT2 mice received an IP injection of clozapine n-oxide (CNO) at 5 mg/kg 45 min prior to or immediately following a 5-min bout of aSD and then tested for avoidance/investigation 24 h later. Following the day 2 assay, a subset of non-defeated and defeated Amigo2-icreERT2 mice underwent cardiac perfusion with a 5% paraformaldehyde (PFA) solution 45 min after the assay in order to preserve the brains for immunostaining.
Patch clamp recordings in acute hippocampal slices
Adult Amigo2-icreERT2 mice (N = 20, male and female) received an IP dose of Fatal Plus (sodium pentobarbital, 50 mg/mL, ≥100 mg/kg) and were transcardially perfused with 10 ml of ice-cold ACSF solution containing the following (in mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 glucose, 2 Na-pyruvate, 0.5 CaCl2, and 7 MgCl2, 100 picrotoxin (Abcam), saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 per a protocol developed for recording from adult mouse slices [46]. The brain was removed from the skull in ice-cold ACSF solution. Coronal slices at 300 µm thickness were cut using a vibratome (Leica VT-1200), stored for ~60 min at 32 °C, and allowed to recover at room temperature (22–24 °C) for at least 30 min in ACSF (saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2). For recordings, slices were transferred to a recording chamber and continuously superfused with ACSF containing the following (in mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 25 glucose, 2 CaCl2, and 1 MgCl2, saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. All recordings were performed using an Axopatch 200B amplifier. Signals were filtered at 10 kHz and sampled at 20 kHz for current-clamp recordings and filtered at 5 kHz and sampled at 10 kHz for voltage-clamp recordings. Pipettes were pulled from WPI glass (PG10–165) using a horizontal puller (P97; Sutter Instruments) and were filled with an internal solution consisting of 140 K-gluconate, 10 HEPES, 0.1 EGTA, 8 NaCl, 4 MgCl2, 2 ATP, and 0.3 GTP, along with biocytin (1 mg/ml), pH 7.3 with KOH. Pipette resistances in the working solutions ranged from 3 to 6 MΩ resulting in series resistances of 10–30 MΩ. rCA2 axons were stimulated using 1-ms blue light pulses generated by a 455 nm LED (Prizmatix UHP-T-455-EP) mounted on a Zeiss Examiner Z.1 microscope and directed through a Zeiss 40x objective.
Tissue processing and immunohistochemistry
Following the conclusion of a given experiment, mice were deeply anesthetized with Fatal Plus (sodium pentobarbital, 50 mg/mL: >100 mg/kg, IP). Immediately after transcardial perfusion with 4% paraformaldehyde (pH 7.4) in phosphate buffered saline (PBS), brains were removed and post-fixed in the same fixative overnight at 4 °C. Brains were coronally sectioned at 40 µm on a Leica VT 1200S vibratome. Brain sections were then stored free floating in PBS with 0.02% sodium azide at 4 °C until staining.
Unless otherwise noted, all free-floating sections were washed in 1xPBS for 15 min and incubated overnight at 4 °C in 3% neuronal goat serum (NGS) in 0.3% PBS-Triton followed by a 2-day incubation in primary antibody in 3% NGS/0.3% PBS-Triton at 4 °C. Sections were then washed for 20 min in 0.3% PBS-Triton and then 3 times 0.3% PBS-Triton for 10 min each before being incubated overnight at 4 °C in secondary antibody in 3% NGS/0.3% PBS-Triton. For identification of biocytin filled CA1 neurons in the caudal hippocampus, sections were stained with a fluorescence-tagged streptavidin secondary antibody. Finally, sections were washed once in 0.3% PBS-Triton for 30 min and three time in 1xPBS for 15 min each at room temp before being cover slipped with Vectashield Hardset Mounting Medium with DAPI (Vector, H-1500-10).
The following antibodies were used to stain CA2 axons (1⁰ chicken anti-GFP (Abcam #ab13970) 1:1000 + 2⁰ anti-chicken 488 (Invitrogen #A11039) 1:500) and biocytin filled cells (2⁰ Streptavidin-568 (Invitrogen #S11226) 1:500) in caudal hippocampus. A subset of sections was stained for calbindin+ cells (1⁰ rabbit anti-calbindin (Swant #CB38) 1:500 + 2⁰ anti-rabbit-633 (Invitrogen #A21071) 1:500). To visualize Gi-DREADD-mCherry expression in rCA2 of injected mice sections were stained with 1⁰ rat anti-mCherry (Life Technology #M11217) 1:500 + 2⁰ anti-rat 568 (Invitrogen #A11077) at 1:250. In order to visualize AAV-retro positive cells sections were stained for mCherry (1⁰ rat anti-mCherry (Life Technology #M11217) 1:500 + 2⁰ anti-rat 568 (Invitrogen #A11077) at 1:250) paired with (2⁰ Streptavidin-633 (Invitrogen #S21375) 1:500).
For immunofluorescent staining of the IEG cFOS, free-floating sections were washed twice in 1x PBS for five minutes followed by a permeabilization wash in 0.3% PBS-Triton for 20 min and a 5-min wash in 1x PBS. Cells were incubated at room temperature (RT) in 10% normal goat serum (NGS) in 0.1% PBS-Triton for 1 h. Sections were then incubated overnight at 4 °C on a plate rocker in a 5% NGS/0.1% PBS-Triton solution containing rabbit anti-cFos antibody (Abcam, ab190289) at 1:500 dilution. On day 2 sections were allowed to incubate at RT for ~2 h before 2 5-min washes in 0.1% PBS-Triton, a third 5-min wash in 1xPBS and 2 h in Goat anti-rabbit 488 (Thermo A11034) at a 1:500 dilution in 5% NGS/0.1% PBS-Triton at RT. Following secondary incubation, sections were washed twice in 0.1% PBS-Triton for 10 min, and once in PBS for a minimum of 20 min. Lastly, sections were given a final 5-min wash in 1xPBS, and cover slipped with Vectashield Hardset Mounting Medium with DAPI (Vector, H-1500-10).
Microscopy and analysis
All images were acquired with an LSM 880 Zeiss confocal microscope. For images that were later used for quantification, imaging parameters were kept constant for sections of a given brain region. All quantifications were made on maximum intensity z-projected images with ImageJ [47]. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.
Determining the location of a biocytin filled caudal hippocampal neuron along the dorsal-ventral axis
Following the completion of a whole-cell electrophysiological recording, the recording pipette was slowly retracted allowing the soma to reseal in order to preserve the biocytin labeling. Sections were then transferred to 4% PFA overnight at 4 °C before being transferred to PBS. Next, sections were immunostained with Streptavidin-568 (Invitrogen #S11226) and, in a subset of sections, calbindin (1⁰ rabbit anti-calbindin (Swant #CB38) + 2⁰ anti-rabbit-633 (Invitrogen #A21071)). Prior to imaging, the 400 µm sections were incubated at room temperature in a thiodiethanol solution (30% in PBS, Sigma-Aldrich #88561) for a minimum of 30 min rendering the sections translucent. Confocal images of the caudal hippocampus were taken and the distance from the dorsal to the ventral end of the CA1 cell body layer was measured using Image J. This measurement was then divided into three equal parts in order to discriminate between the dorsal, intermediate, and ventral subregions of caudal CA1. Imaged biocytin labeled neurons were then categorized as residing in one of these three subregions.
Quantification of cFos+ cells following the social avoidance assay
Sections containing the hippocampus, lateral septum and frontal cortical brain regions used in the quantification of cFos+ cells were immunostained using the protocol detailed above. 20x tile scan images of our regions of interest were taken at 512×512 resolution at a z-section depth of 4.04 µm, 4–5 z-sections per stack. Laser power, pinhole size, and exposure settings were kept consistent for a given brain region and set to a level that maximized the fluorescent signal while minimizing background for a given region. Notably, while all brain sections used in the Gq-DREADD experiments underwent immunohistochemical processing and slide mounting at the same time, cHPC sections were imaged at a later time period than ACC and mPFC and as such, a higher post-processing threshold was applied to minimize background. Prior to quantification 3 z-sections were max projected for a total quantifiable depth of 12 µm per section. Regions of interest (ROIs) were drawn in Image J to further subdivide brain regions to be quantified based on landmarks and measures from The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinated, 2nd edition [48]. cFos+ cell counts were achieved using the Image J Image Processing analysis plugin. Grayscale images were first thresholded consistently across sections for a given brain region and particles (“Analyze Particles”) were analyzed with the following constraints: size µm2 = 400–1000, circularity = 0.5–1.0. A minimum of 2 hemispheres were quantified for a given brain region and cell counts were averaged per animal. cFos quantification data presented as mean ± SEM.
Results
Acute social defeat results in long lasting social avoidance
Repeated, stressful exposure to an aggressive mouse over multiple days results in behavioral and physiological changes in subject animals, but less is known about how an acute aggressive defeat alters mouse investigative behavior. To better understand these potential effects, we subjected adult male, wildtype mice (C57BL/6 J) to a 5-min bout of aSD in the home cage of a male CD1 aggressor mouse. One day after this stressful encounter, subject mice were placed in a novel arena that contained a male littermate of the CD1 aggressor and were allowed to explore the context for 5 min. We then calculated the amount time a subject mouse spent in the far zone of the area (avoidance) and the number of discrete entries into the interaction zone surrounding the CD1 mouse (investigation) (Fig. 1a). When compared to non-defeated control mice, defeated mice spent significantly more time in the far zone of the arena (Fig. 1b–d). Additionally, they exhibited significantly fewer entries into the interaction zone (Fig. 1e). Not only did aSD result in a reduction in social investigation, but defeated mice also covered significantly less total distance over the course of the 5-min avoidance assay (Supplementary Fig. 1c). We next asked if any metrics observed during the day 1 defeat would predict the degree of social avoidance a defeated subject mouse would exhibit 24 h later. We observed no significant correlation between either the amount of aggression a subject received and the amount of time of spent in the far zone (Supplementary Fig. 1a) or total observable submissive behaviors such as freezing, flee, and defensive posturing and avoidance 24 h post defeat (Supplementary Fig. 1b). Surprisingly, this avoidant phenotype persisted in the more “susceptible” population of defeated mice: when re-exposed to the day 2 context, containing a novel CD1 mouse, one month after the initial acute defeat exposure, defeated mice that spent >200 s in the far zone of the arena on day 2 (Fig. 1f, red) continued to exhibit significantly higher levels of avoidance when compared to more investigative cohort of defeated mice (Fig. 1f, blue, Supplementary Fig. 1d). Moreover, this avoidant phenotype is observable for up to 3 months in the most susceptible mice following a single bout of social aggression (Supplementary Fig. 1d).
a In the acute social defeat model (aSD) a male subject mouse (C57BL/6 J, 11–25 weeks of age) was placed in the home cage of an adult male CD1 for a five-minute bout of defeat then returned to the home cage. Twenty-four hours later the subject mouse was allowed five-minutes to explore a novel environment containing a littermate of the CD1 aggressor housed within a wire cup. Warm colors represent more time spent. b Heatmap illustrating a non-defeated subject’s preference investigation of the novel CD1 mouse. c Heatmap illustrating a defeated subject’s preference for the “far zone” opposite the location of the CD1. d Defeated mice spent significantly more time in the “far zone” of the arena when compared to non-defeat mice 24 h after aSD (No Defeat: 97.98 ± 3.6 s vs. Defeat: 190.49 ± 25.0 s, N = 6 & 12 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p = 0.02). e Non-defeated control mice interacted with the CD1 mouse at a significantly higher frequency, measured as discrete entries into the interaction zone, than the defeated mice 24 h after aSD (No Defeat: 91.17 ± 16.1 entries vs. Defeat: 40.25 ± 9.4 entries, N = 6 & 12 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p = 0.01). f One month following a single bout of defeat the two groups, as a whole, no longer exhibited significantly different measures of avoidance (No Defeat: 127.80 ± 20.6 s vs. Defeat: 180.75 ± 30.5 s, N = 6 & 11 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p = 0.25). Notably, though, a more avoidant subpopulation ( >200 s spent in the far zone) of defeated mice emerged as distinct from the more investigative mice (Defeat-Avoidant: 265.81 ± 14.2 s vs. Defeat-Investigative: 78.68 ± 6.7 s, N = 6 & 5 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p < 0.0001). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.
Chemogenetic inactivation of CA2 pyramidal neurons increases avoidance after an acute social defeat
Given the now well-established role of hippocampal area CA2 in behaviors such as social discrimination memory and aggression, we next asked whether inhibition of neuronal activity in this region during aSD would alter the avoidant/investigative phenotype 24 h following defeat. To that end, we injected intracranially a cre-dependent inhibitory Gi-DREADD AAV into area CA2 of cre+ and cre- Amigo2-icre-ERT2 male mice. Forty-five minutes prior to the defeat, CNO was delivered intraperitoneally to both populations of subject mice, effectively silencing CA2 neuronal output in subject mice (cre+ defeat group, expressing Gi-DREADD) during the stressful defeat encounter [32]. Cre- and cre+ mice were then subjected to the day 2 avoidance test as previously outlined (Fig. 2a, b). Similar to what we observed in male C57 wildtype, defeated cre- mice (no DREADD-expressing control mice), with CA2 activity left unperturbed, spent significantly more time in the far zone of the arena (Fig. 2c) with significantly fewer entries into the interaction zone surrounding the CD1 mouse (Fig. 2d) than non-defeated control mice. Surprisingly, cre+ mice, with CA2 neuronal activity inhibited during defeat, exhibited an even stronger avoidance phenotype when compared to cre- defeat counterparts as measured by time spent in zone opposite the CD1 mouse (Fig. 2c, d) and number of entries in the surrounding interaction zone (Fig. 2c, d). The significant increase in social avoidance in mice where CA2 activity was inhibited during the defeat was driven by the loss of the more investigative/”resilient” population of cre+ mice. When comparing the two defeated populations, 61.1% of cre- mice exhibited an investigative phenotype versus 16.7% of cre+ mice. Conversely, although 38.9% of cre- defeats showed an avoidant phenotype, this avoidant population significantly increased to 83.3% in cre+ defeated mice (Chi-square 40.69, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 2e). As CNO-driven activation of an inhibitory Gi-DREADD is known to exert its effects for up 6 h following administration, we next wondered if the investigative deficits we observed in cre+ mice were due to CA2 neuronal inhibition during acquisition (during the defeat) or as a result of Gi-DREADD mediated disruption of consolidation of the stress memory which occurs on the order of hours following acquisition. To better understand the role of these distinct phases of memory processing we ran a second cohort of Gi-DREADD expressing mice through the aSD paradigm with CNO being delivered to both cre- and cre+ defeat groups immediately following the defeat (Supplementary Fig. 2a). Chemogenetic inactivation of area CA2 immediately following the defeat encounter had no statistically significant effect on avoidance levels (Supplementary Fig. 2b), whereas both groups showed significantly less avoidance levels relative to defeated cre+ animals that had CA2 activity inhibited during defeat (Supplementary Fig. 2b).
a Acute social defeat experimental timeline. Five mg/kg of clozapine-N-oxide (CNO) was delivered on day 1, 45 min prior to defeat by a CD1 aggressor mouse via IP injection in Amigo2-icreERT2 male mice (15–19wks of age) that either did (Cre + ) or did not (Cre-) express cre-recombinase in area CA2 neurons. Twenty-four hours after defeat, day 2, mice were tested for avoidance/investigation in a novel context containing a littermate of the CD1 aggressor mouse. b (right) Amigo2-icreERT2-Cre+ mice show CA2 specific expression of the intracranially injected Gi-DREADD-mCherry construct 3+ weeks following injection (left). No expression of the Gi-DREADD construct was observed in Cre- mice. c Day 2, 24 h after defeat, both defeat populations spent significantly more time in the far zone of the novel arena, opposite the CD1 mouse (i.e. avoidance behavior), when compared to no defeat control mice. Cre+ defeated males, with CA2 activity inhibited, spent significantly more time in the far zone than Cre- defeated mice (No Defeat controls: 119.44 ± 5.7 s vs. Defeat Cre-: 188.60 ± 16.4 s vs. Defeat Cre + : 241.64 ± 12.7 s, N = 22, 18, 18, respectively, one-way ANOVA F = 28.19 p < 0.0001; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre-, Holm-Sidak multiple comparisons test p = 0.0.0002, No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre + , Holm-Sidak multiple comparisons test p < 0.0001, Defeat Cre- vs. Defeat Cre + , Holm-Sidak multiple comparisons test p = 0.003). d Day 2, 24 h after defeat, Cre+ mice entered the interaction zone, surrounding the CD1 mouse, at a significantly lower frequency than both Defeat Cre- and No Defeat control mice (No Defeat controls: 73.09 ± 6.5 entries vs. Defeat Cre-: 32.5 ± 6.5 entries vs. Defeat Cre + : 16.17 ± 3.4 entries, N = 22, 18, 18 mice, respectively, one-way ANOVA F = 26.34 p < 0.0001; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre-, Holm-Sidak p < 0.0001, No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre + , Holm-Sidak p < 0.0001, Defeat Cre- vs. Defeat Cre + , Holm-Sidak p = 0.062, unpaired t-test p = 0.034). e The increase in avoidance behavior (red) in Defeat Cre+ mice was driven by a loss of the more investigative phenotype (green) when CA2 activity was inhibited during aSD (Cre-: 61.1% investigative and 38.9% avoidance versus Cre + : 16.7% investigative and 83.3% avoidant, N = 18 & 18 mice respectively, Chi-square 40.69, z = 6.38, p < 0.0001). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001.
Chemogenetic inactivation of CA2 alters defensive behaviors during defeat
A single bout of received aggression is sufficient to drive social avoidance in a subset of defeated mice. Given our observation that CA2 activity during defeat supports later investigative behaviors, we next sought to determine whether a subject mouse’s behavior during the defeat is predictive of future investigation/”resilience” or avoidance/”susceptibility”. On day 1 of the aSD paradigm with CNO delivered 45 min prior to defeat (Fig. 3a), we observed no significant correlation between the amount of aggression received and future avoidance in cre- and cre+ defeated mice (Fig. 3b). Similarly, we observed no significant correlation between the amount of total submissive behaviors expressed by mice during the defeat encounter and time spent in the far zone on day 2 of the paradigm (Fig. 3c): (N = 36 mice, R2 = 0.0297, p > 0.005). Because mice are capable of a range of submissive behaviors when faced with threat or aggression, we next subdivided total submission into either submissive flee (time spent freezing or fleeing the aggression) or a more defensive response where the subject would raise up on their hindlegs and stand ground in response to CD1 aggression (defensive submission). Mice that showed high levels of submissive flee during defeat went on to display higher measures of avoidance on day 2 as evidenced by a positive correlation between these two measures (Fig. 3d), though we found no significant difference in absolute amount of submissive flee between defeated cre- and cre+ mice (Fig. 3e). When CA2 was inhibited during the defeat, cre+ mice spent significantly less time in defensive postures (Fig. 3f) and showed a significant reduction in defensive submission as a percentage of total submissive behaviors (Fig. 3g). Interestingly, we observed a positive correlation between the amount of aggression a subject mouse received, and the amount of time spent in a defensive submissive posture (Fig. 3h). As expected, we observed no difference in submissive defensive behaviors between cre- and cre+ mice in response to the aggressor when CNO was instead delivered immediately following the defeat (Supplementary Fig. 2c).
a Acute social defeat experimental timeline. b No significant correlation was observed between the amount of aggression a subject mouse received during the day 1 defeat and the amount of time spent in the “far zone” of the arena (i.e. avoidance) 24 h after defeat (N = 36 mice, R2 = 0.0052, p > 0.005). c No significant correlation was observed between the amount of total submission (combined flee/freezing + defensive behaviors) exhibited by subject mice during defeat and avoidance 24 h following the defeat (N = 35 mice, R2 = 0.0297, p > 0.005). d During the 5-min defeat, the amount of submissive flee/freezing behaviors exhibited by subject mice positively correlates with an increase in CD1 avoidance 24 h after defeat (N = 36 mice, R2 = 0.1196, p = 0.039). e The absolute amount of submissive fleeing/freezing did not significantly differ between the Cre- and Cre+ populations of defeated mice (Defeat Cre-: 39.11 ± 6.1 s vs. Defeat Cre + : 58.39 ± 9.0 s, N = 18, 18 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p = 0.08). f Cre+ mice, with CA2 activity inhibited, spend significantly less time exhibiting defensive behaviors during defeat when compared to Cre- mice (Defeat Cre-: 44.06 ± 6.3 s vs. Defeat Cre + : 24.50 ± 5.2 s, N = 18, 18 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p = 0.02). g Similarly, Cre- mice, on average, spent a significantly higher percent of total submission engaging in more defensive behaviors when compared to Cre+ defeat mice (Defeat Cre-: 52 ± 7.0% vs. Defeat Cre + : 30 ± 4.0%, N = 18,18, unpaired t-test p = 0.01). h The amount of aggression a subject mouse receives positively correlates with submissive defensive behaviors during the acute defeat (N = 36 mice, R2 = 0.143, p = 0.023). *p < 0.05.
The avoidance phenotype is specific to the acute social defeat experience
Given the understanding that a specific stressor can result in a more generalized stress response [49], we implemented two final behavioral tests using adult, male mice to determine whether or not avoidance of the CD1 aggressor littermate was specific to the aSD stressor. In the first test, the day 1 social defeat stress was replaced with a stressful fear conditioning assay, consisting of five tone-shock pairings followed by the day 2 avoidance test and re-exposure to the footshock context (Supplementary Fig. 3a). As expected, five tone-shock (0.6 mA) pairings delivered at 3 min intervals resulted in mice displaying increased freezing behavior over the course of the fear conditioning followed by high levels of freezing when the mice were re-exposed to the conditioning context ~24 h later (Supplementary Fig. 3b, c). Furthermore, when shock-conditioned mice were placed in the arena containing a novel CD1 mouse, we observed avoidance levels similar to those of the non-defeated wild type mice and significantly lower than defeated wildtype (Supplementary Fig. 3d). In a second test, a separate cohort of adult male Amigo2-cre- mice were defeated following our standard aSD protocol and returned to their home cage. Then, 24 h later, they were placed in the arena containing a novel conspecific (C57BL/6 J) mouse and allowed to explore the context. Interestingly, we found that non-defeat control and socially defeated mice spent an equivalent amount of time in the far zone of the arena, opposite the novel mouse (Supplementary Fig. 3f). From these data we concluded that the socially avoidant phenotype we observe in our defeated population is specific to the aSD experience and not merely a generalized stress response.
Rostrodorsal CA2 pyramidal neurons preferentially target intermediate CA1 in the caudal hippocampus
Stress responses following exposure to threat or aggression have been shown to affect circuitry within the hippocampus as well as neuronal physiology and signaling in extrahippocampal and limbic brain regions, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and lateral septum [50, 51]. Although CA2 afferents to dorsal lateral septum (dLS) have been shown to underlie aggression towards a conspecific, these pyramidal neurons, like those in dorsal CA1, are not believed to directly project to higher level brain areas like the mPFC. Within the hippocampus, it is the projection neurons located in caudal CA1 (cCA1) that form excitatory connections with such regions [52, 53]. To better understand how CA2 activity may be transmitted to these cortical areas we performed whole cell, patch-clamp experiments in cCA1 of cre+ Amigo2-icreERT2 mice that expressed channelrhodopsin (AAV5-DIO-ChR2-EYFP) in rostrodorsal CA2 (Fig. 4a, b). This allowed us to record light-evoked excitatory synaptic responses in CA1 pyramidal neurons of coronal slices along the dorsal-intermediate-ventral (DV) axis of the caudal hippocampus originating from rostrodorsal CA2 (Fig. 4c, d). We observed strong rostrodorsal CA2 (rCA2) axonal labeling in the dorsal and intermediate regions of caudal hippocampal slices (Fig. 4c, Supplementary Fig. 4). Notably, we saw limited EYFP+ labeling in the more ventral region of the cCA1 cell body layer. Consistent with this observation, optical stimulation of rCA2 axons resulted in excitatory synaptic currents in both dorsal and intermediate caudal CA1 cells, with the largest response amplitudes observed in intermediate cCA1 pyramidal neurons, but minimal to no synaptic responses in CA1 cells in the more ventral region of the caudal DV axis (Fig. 4d, e). The evoked responses we did observe were of a significantly lower amplitude than those recorded at intermediate levels. Interestingly, rCA2 → cCA1 synapses in the dorsal and intermediate areas exhibited paired pulse ratios (PPR) of less than one (i.e., depressing synapses) in response to optical stimulation at a 500 ms interval, with significantly stronger evoked depression occurring in more dorsal CA1 neurons (Fig. 4f, g). This finding is different than the well-studied CA3 → CA1 Schaffer collateral synapses, which exhibit paired pulse ratios closer to 1 following a 500 ms interval [54, 55].
a Intracranial injection of 500ul of a Cre recombinase dependent virus containing channelrhodopsin and an EYFP tag were delivered in the rostral CA2 of an Amigo2-icreERT2 mouse. b Image of the resulting channelrhoposin expression pattern in rostral CA2 of in a cre+ Amigo2-icreERT2 mouse (green). c (left) Channelrhodopsin-positive axons of rostral CA2 neurons extended caudally and were the densest in the dorsal and intermediate areas of cCA1 (scale bar = 500 µm). (inset) A biocytin filled pyramidal neuron in dorsal, cCA1 of the hippocampus (red, scale bar = 100 µm). d A 1 ms pulse of laser generated blue light evoked an inward, excitatory current in a cCA1 pyramidal neuron (scale bar = 50 pA x 50 ms). e Light-evoked synaptic responses from CA2 axons were significantly larger in amplitude in neurons found in dorsal and intermediate cCA1 neurons when compared to more ventral locations with the largest response generated in intermediate, cCA1 (Fig. 4d, e: Dorsal: −77.9 ± 14.0 pA vs. Intermediate: −199.42 ± 58.0 pA vs. Ventral −12.6 ± 14.4 pA, n = 18, 41 and 12 cells, Welch’s ANOVA W = 14.67 p < 0.0001; Dorsal vs. Intermediate unpaired t-test with Welch’s correction p = 0.048, Dorsal vs. Ventral unpaired t-test with Welch’s correction p = 0.0002, Intermediate vs. Ventral unpaired t-test with Welch’s correction p = 0.0026). f Two light-evoked pulses, delivered with a 500 ms interval, were used to evoke synaptic response in a cCA1 pyramidal neuron (scale bar = 25 pA x 50 ms). g Both dorsal and intermediate cCA1 neurons showed paired pulse ratios (PPR) of less than 1 indicating these are likely depressing synaptic connections. Dorsal CA1 neurons had a significantly lower PPR than intermediate neurons (Dorsal: 0.52 ± 0.031 vs. Intermediate: 0.65 ± 0.046, n = 12 and 25 cells, from 8 and 14 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p = 0.012). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.
Intrinsic properties of CA1 pyramidal neurons along the dorsal-intermediate-ventral axis of the caudal hippocampus were largely similar with one notable exception: the resting membrane of more ventral CA1 neurons was significantly more depolarized (less negative) when compared to dorsal and intermediate CA1 neurons (Supplementary Fig. 5a). Action potentials were evoked using 500 ms stepwise current injections and although we observed a trend towards more robust firing in dorsal CA1 cells, we found no significant difference in firing rates between groups at 400 pA current injections (Supplementary Fig. 5b). The sag ratio elicited by a single hyperpolarizing current injection, a measure of Ih current activation, was also similar across neurons from these three regions (Supplementary Fig. 5c). Finally, it has been proposed that rostrodorsal CA2 neurons preferentially target neighboring calbindin+ CA1 neurons located in the deep, versus superficial, cell layer [25]. In cCA1, we found that the majority of cells that receive synaptic input from rCA2 were, in fact, calbindin negative (Supplementary Fig. 5d).
CA2-targeted caudal CA1 pyramidal cells project to corticolimbic brain regions
Our electrophysiological experiments provided evidence that cCA1 neurons in the dorsal and intermediate regions along the DV axis receive excitatory synaptic input from pyramidal neurons in rostrodorsal CA2. As these CA1 neurons have previously been shown to project to extrahippocampal, cortical targets, we next investigated whether cCA1 could be acting as an intermediary in a disynaptic circuit routing CA2 information to higher order limbic brain areas.
We first asked whether activation of CA2 neurons resulted in increased activation of cortical neurons as approximated by cFos staining. To do this, we expressed cre-dependent, excitatory Gq-DREADDs in rCA2 neurons of cre+ and cre- Amigo2-icreERT2 mice via bilateral intracranial injections. After allowing for expression of the Gq-DREADD construct, rCA2 neurons were activated following a single IP injection of CNO (1 mg/kg) and the mice were euthanized 90-min later. Brain sections from these mice were immunohistochemically labeled for the immediate early gene (IEG) cFos, one marker for neuronal activity. Although cFos was detectable in cortical areas of cre- control animals, chemogenetic activation of rCA2 drove a significant increase in the number of cFos+ neurons in the ACC of cre+ mice when compared to control mice (Fig. 5a, b). Similarly, Gq-DREADD activation resulted in a marked increase in the number of cFos+ cells in both the prelimbic and infralimbic subregions of the mPFC of cre+ mice when compared to cre- controls (Fig. 5c, d). Furthermore, Gq-DREADD activation of rCA2 neurons resulted in a significant increase in cFos+ cells within the caudal CA1 region when compared to cre- control mice (Supplementary Fig. 6a, b). Given our finding that (a) rCA2 neurons synaptically activate dorsal and intermediate cCA1 neurons, and (b) GqDREADD activation of CA2 results in increased cFos expression in ACC, mPFC and caudal CA1, we wondered if dorsal or intermediate cCA1 neurons project to ACC and mPFC. To address this question, we unilaterally infused retrogradely transported AAVs (rAAV-mCherry) into either the prelimbic mPFC or the ACC to label CA1 neurons targeting these areas. In coronal sections of the caudal hippocampus, mCherry+ neurons were found labeled along the dorsal-ventral axis of the CA1 cell body layer and specifically in the dorsal and intermediate regions that receive direct, excitatory input from CA2 (Fig. 5e). Next, we recorded from acutely prepared caudal hippocampal slices from cre+ Amigo2-icreERT2 mice that expressed ChR2-EYFP in rCA2 neurons and mCherry+ cCA1 neurons with ACC as a known projection target (Fig. 5f). Optical stimulation of rCA2 axons resulted in excitatory synaptic responses in mCherry+ cCA1 neurons that were significantly larger in amplitude than responses in mCherry- cells that presumably target regions other than the ACC (Fig. 5g). Taken together, these experiments demonstrate that chemogenetic stimulation of rCA2 is sufficient to drive activation of cells in both the mPFC and the ACC, and that excitatory synaptic information originating in rCA2 pyramidal neurons can be routed to corticolimbic brain areas via cCA1 projection neurons in the dorsal and intermediate regions of the caudal hippocampus. Furthermore, CA2 may preferentially target cCA1 neurons that, in turn, project directly to the ACC.
a Coronal sections from the anterior cingulate cortex of cre- (top) and cre+ Amigo2-icreERT2 (bottom) mice immunohistochemically stained for the IEG cFos (scale bar = 500 µm). b in vivo activation of area CA2 via bilateral expression of Gq-DREADDs causes a significant increase in cFos expression in the ACC of cre+ mice compared to cre- mice (cre-: 72.3 ± 15.1 cFos+ cells vs. cre + : 239.1 ± 8.5 cFos+ cells, 8 hemispheres from 4 animals and 6 hemispheres from 3 animals, respectively, unpaired t-test p < 0.0001). c Sections from the prelimbic cortex (mPFC) of cre- Amigo2-icreERT2 (top) and cre+ Amigo2-icreERT2 (bottom) mice immunohistochemically stained for the IEG cFos (dashes outline the more dorsal prelimbic and the more ventral infralimbic cortices, scale bar = 500 µm). d In vivo activation of area CA2 via bilateral expression Gq-DREADDs drives a significant increase in cFos expression in both prelimbic (cre-: 6.1 ± 1.5 cFos+ cells/area−5 µm vs. cre + : 18.7 ± 1.8 cFos+ cells/area−5 µm, 7 sections from 3 mice and 5 sections from 3 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p = 0.0004) and infralimbic (cre-: 3.9 ± 1.2 cFos+ cells/area−5 µm vs. cre + : 18.7 ± 5.8 cFos+ cells/area−5 µm, 7 sections from 3 mice and 5 sections from 3 mice, respectively, unpaired t-test p = 0.0004) cortex of Cre+ mice compared to Cre- mice. e (left, top) 250–300 µl intracranial injection of AAV-retro-mCherry virus injected into the prelimbic cortex of a wildtype mouse resulted in mCherry expression in neurons along the dorsal-ventral axis of the ipsilateral, cHPC (left, bottom). (right, top) 250–300 µl intracranial injection of AAV-retro-mCherry virus injected into the ACC of a wildtype mouse results in mCherry expression in neurons along the dorsal-ventral axis of the ipsilateral cHPC (right, bottom) (scale bars = 500 µm). f Dual intracranial viral injections result in rostral CA2 specific expression of a channelrhodopsin-EYFP (left) and AAV-retro-mCherry labeled neurons in the cHPC neurons that sent axonal projections to the ACC (right). g Rostral CA2 > cCA1 light evoked, excitatory synaptic responses in cHPC neurons that project to the ACC. Responses from neurons that send axonal projections to ACC (mCherry + ) were significantly larger in amplitude than the synaptic responses from mCherry- neurons that likely project elsewhere (non-labeled cells: −10.28 ± 4.3 pA vs. −75.8 ± 25.7 pA, n = 5 and 10 cells, respectively, p = 0.034, unpaired t-test with Welch’s correction, scale bars = 50 pA x 50 ms). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001.
Acute social defeat alters cFos+ expression in frontocortical and hippocampal brain regions during social investigation
In a final set of experiments, we aimed to better understand how the aSD stressor may affect cellular activity in hippocampal and extrahippocampal brain areas during the day 2 avoidance test when subject mice were allowed to investigate an arena containing a littermate of the CD1 aggressor. In this experiment, we examined cFos staining in non-defeated controls and cre- and cre+ defeated mice (given CNO prior to defeat). Forty-five minutes following the avoidance test, animals were euthanized, and coronal brain sections cut and immunostained for cFos. We asked whether cFos would be differentially expressed in the highly avoidant cre+ group, where CA2 activity was inhibited during the defeat (Fig. 6a, c, e). We found that 24 h after aSD, the number of cFos+ cells was significantly reduced in the mPFC of cre+ mice when compared to non-defeat controls during social investigation. This effect was largely driven by significantly lower cFos positivity in the prelimbic subregion (Fig. 6b). Moreover, we observed significantly lower cFos+ neuron counts in highly avoidant cre+ mice in the ACC when compared to non-defeat control mice (Fig. 6b). Notably, cFos+ cell counts from the more heterogeneous cre- defeat population, which contains mice that were either investigative or avoidant fell, on average, between our control mice and our cre+ defeated mice. Somewhat surprisingly, cFos+ was unaffected in the dorsal lateral septum, a region that receives direct input from rCA2 and modulates aggressive behaviors (Fig. 6b). In the caudal hippocampus (Fig. 6c) the number of cFos+ cells in the cCA1 cell body layer was significantly reduced in both cre- and cre+ defeat groups when compared to non-defeat control mice. Interestingly, this effect is mainly driven by a subregion specific loss of cFos positivity in ventral cCA1, a region that our electrophysiological data suggests receives little to no direct synaptic input from rCA2 (Fig. 6d). These reductions, along with significantly lower cFos expression in caudal CA3 in cre+ mice, result in significantly lower total cFos+ counts in the caudal hippocampus of both cre- and cre+ socially defeated mice. In the rostral hippocampus (Fig. 6e), a region that underlies social recognition memory [22, 28, 56] and also the region most directly affected by Gi-DREADD mediated inhibition in cre+ mice during defeat, we observed a general trend towards reduced cFos expression in defeated animals. The number of cFos+ neurons was only significantly reduced in the rCA1 of cre- defeated mice when compared to non-defeated control mice (Fig. 6f). Together these data support the idea that inactivation of CA2 during defeat on day 1 can result in decreased activation of ACC that occurs when animals are investigating another animal on day 2. In contrast, cFos staining in hippocampal areas appeared to best reflect whether an animal had experienced defeat, irrespective of CA2 inactivation status the day before.
a Confocal image of mouse anterior cingulate cortex stained for the immediate early gene (IEG) cFos (green) 45 min after the day 2 avoidance test, 20x, scale bar = 100 µm. b Quantification of cFos+ cells in extrahippocampal brain regions active during the investigation of a CD1 mouse 24 h after aSD. The number of cFos+ neurons in the mPFC, in total, and the prelimbic (PL) subregion were significantly lower in Cre+ mice that had been defeated when compared to No Defeat control animals (mPFC: No Defeat: 116.4 ± 13.3 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 76.3 ± 10.2, N = 5 and 7 mice, respectively, one-way ANOVA F = 3.44 p = 0.059, unpaired t-test p = 0.035:: PL: No Defeat: 90.3 ± 5.6 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 64.0 ± 8.7 cells, N = 5 and 7 mice, respectively, one-way ANOVA F = 2.50 p = 0.115, unpaired t-test p = 0.044). Similarly, cFos+ cells were significantly lower in number in the ACC of Cre+ defeated mice compared to No Defeat controls (No Defeat: 269.6 ± 63.0 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 102.1 ± 48.8 cells, N = 11 and 11 mice, respectively, one-way ANOVA F = 2.44 p = 0.95, unpaired t-test p = 0.048). aSD had no effect on cFos expression in either the dorsal lateral septum (dLS: No Defeat: 12.5 ± 1.3 cFos+ cells vs. Defeat Cre-: 10.4 ± 1.3 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 13.7 ± 2.2 cells, N = 11, 12 and 11 mice, respectively) or ventral lateral septum (vLS: No Defeat: 91.7 ± 9.0 cells vs. Defeat Cre-: 107.7 ± 14.1 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 109.3 ± 8.8 cells, N = 11, 12 and 11 mice) during investigation of a novel CD1 mouse. c. Confocal image of mouse cHPC stained for cFOS (green) following the day 2 avoidance test, 20x, scale bar = 500 µm. d cFOS+ neurons counts were significantly lower in cCA3, cCA1 total, ventral cCA1 and the cHPC in total in defeated mice when compared to No Defeat controls (cCA3: one-way ANOVA F = 3.32 p = 0.052, No Defeat: 20.4 ± 2.6 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 12.9 ± 1.1 cells, N = 9 and 10 mice, unpaired t-test p = 0.014 :: cCA1: one-way ANOVA F = 4.14 p = 0.027, No Defeat: 67.7 ± 9.0 cells vs. Defeat Cre-: 43.1 ± 5.2 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 43.4 ± 6.2 cells, N = 9, 10 and 10 mice; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre-, unpaired t-test p = 0.026; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre + , unpaired t-test p = 0.037:: cCA1v: one-way ANOVA F = 5.59 p = 0.0095, No Defeat: 38.3 ± 5.0 cells vs. Defeat Cre-: 23.9 ± 3.1 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 20.1 ± 3.8 cells, N = 9, 10 and 10 mice; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre-, unpaired t-test p = 0.024; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre + , unpaired t-test p = 0.001) :: cHPC total: one-way ANOVA F = 3.95 p = 0.032, No Defeat: 106.0 ± 12.0 cells vs. Defeat Cre-: 74.8 ± 8.5 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 71.8 ± 7.5 cells, N = 9, 10 and 10 mice; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre-, unpaired t-test p = 0.046; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre + , unpaired t-test p = 0.023. e Confocal image of mouse rostral hippocampus stained for cFOS (green) following the day 2 avoidance test, 20x, scale bar = 100 µm. f Although the number of cFos+ cells trended lower in the rCA3 subregion and for total positive cells, this reduction is only significant when comparing control mice with Cre- defeated animals (rCA3: one-way ANOVA F = 2.60 p = 0.090, No Defeat: 50.0 ± 7.4 cells vs. Defeat Cre-: 33.7 ± 5.3 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 31.7 ± 5.7 cells, N = 11, 13 and 11 mice :: rCA1: one-way ANOVA F = 4.16 p = 0.025, No Defeat: 42.4 ± 10.7 cells vs. Defeat Cre-: 15.0 ± 3.9 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 19.4 ± 6.1 cells, N = 11, 13 and 11 mice; No Defeat vs. Defeat Cre-, unpaired t-test p = 0.018 :: rHPC total: one-way ANOVA F = 1.82 p = 0.18, No Defeat: 113.0 ± 20.6 cells vs. Defeat Cre-: 92.0 ± 13.5 cells vs. Defeat Cre + : 94.6 ± 16.8 cells, N = 11, 13 and 11 mice, respectively). *p < 0.05.
Discussion
Here we present evidence that activation of rCA2 neurons is sufficient to drive neuronal activity in corticolimbic targets like the mPFC and ACC. Axons from rCA2 form excitatory synaptic connections with CA1 pyramidal neurons in the dorsal and intermediate regions of the caudal hippocampus (cHPC) and may preferentially target caudal CA1 (cCA1) neurons that project to the ACC. Inhibition of CA2 during an acute social defeat stressor resulted in highly avoidant mice that exhibited significant reductions in defensive behaviors when faced with aggression. Moreover, cFos staining was significantly lower in the rCA2 → cCA1 → ACC circuit in highly avoidant, defeated mice following (the opportunity for) social exploration. Our findings contribute to the increasing understanding of the role that CA2 plays in social behaviors and aggression. Alongside previous work from our lab describing the effects of CA2 inhibition on fear conditioning [32], these data support a role for area CA2 in the processing of stressful stimuli. We also report that the acute social defeat paradigm (aSD) is sufficient to drive social avoidance in a subset of defeated mice, not only one day after the defeat, but also weeks later while the more investigative/resilient mice return to avoidance levels similar to those of non-defeated controls.
Previous studies have identified behavioral and physiological markers that can predict whether an animal would exhibit a more “resilient” or “susceptible” phenotype following chronic social defeat stress [10, 57, 58]. Although much of this work has focused on identifiable variability prior to the stressor, Willmore, et al., observed that a defensive, “fighting back” phenotype emerged over the 10-day CSDS paradigm in mice that go on to exhibit resilience following the chronic stress [13]. Consistent with their finding, our results indicate that inhibition of area CA2 during the aSD can significantly reduce the amount of defensive submission in mice that show higher levels of avoidance (low investigation) when compared to defeated mice with unperturbed CA2 activity (Fig. 3f, g). Moreover, and counter to what one may assume, the more time a mouse spends in a defensive posture the more aggression it receives. If chemogenetic inhibition of CA2 activity is disrupting the CA2 to dorsal lateral septum (dLS) excitatory projections known to positively regulate aggressive behaviors [31], it follows that neuronal activity along this pathway may promote defensive aggression, versus freezing or fleeing, thus leading to a more investigative phenotype following aSD. One question that arises is whether the upright defensive posturing we describe as defensive submission can be considered on the spectrum of aggressive behavior. Our reasoning for categorizing this behavior as submission is two-fold; for one, we only observed this posture in response to aggression and never as active or instigative and two, we did not observe subject mice biting or lashing out towards the aggressor. If defensive submission is akin to aggression, we might expect to see changes in cFos expression in the dLS of defeated mice. Surprisingly, when defeated mice were exposed to a littermate of the CD1 aggressor 24 h after defeat, we observed no changes in cFos staining in either the dorsal or lateral septum, regardless of whether CA2 was previously active or inhibited (Fig. 6b, cre- and cre + , respectively). This could be because while active during the defeat, the rCA2 → dLS circuitry may not be reactivated during the day 2 investigation/avoidance test. To better understand the response of brain circuitry during social stress, future work will focus on cFos activation patterns in our regions of interest during the defeat itself. If defensive submission during defeat promotes a more resilient phenotype, as our data suggests, we would expect to see differential patterns of cFos activation along the rHPC-cCA1-ACC pathway when comparing cre- and cre+ mice. This would also further our understanding of a potential role of the ACC in defensive behaviors during received aggression. Interestingly, we also found that CA2 itself has the lowest levels of cFos staining of all the hippocampal areas. However, we know from our in vivo and in vitro work, CA2 neurons are active during social interactions, but that they have unique molecular features that would be predicted to suppress IEG expression [24, 56, 59, 60]. Future work will help us to better understand the role rostral CA2 may playing in promoting a more defensive response to aggression to support a more investigative/resilient phenotype following an aggressive encounter.
CA1 cell bodies span the length of hippocampus along the dorsal-ventral axis in the mouse brain, and cCA1 excitatory projections to the lateral septum, amygdala and cortex have been shown to regulate and modulate anxiety-related behaviors [61,62,63]. Conversely, the rostrodorsal hippocampus is believed to make few if any afferent connections to higher brain regions that underlie “emotional” behaviors, but more likely send intrahippocampal projections to cCA1 projection neurons that, in turn, project to limbic centers [63,64,65]. Although much of the literature on hippocampal circuits has focused on the dense concentration of ventral cCA1 projection neurons [28, 30], we found that rCA2 axons preferentially target CA1 neurons in the more dorsal/intermediate regions versus the more ventral region of the cHPC. Consistent with our results, Raam & Sahay demonstrated that optogenetic inhibition of rCA2 afferents terminating in the more dorsal/intermediate region of cHPC was sufficient to disrupt social discrimination during a three-chamber social memory task [26]. Conversely, Meira et al. disrupted social discrimination memory by chemogenetically inhibiting CA2 axons in ventral CA1 (direct injection of CNO). Though we did not observe direct synaptic input from rCA2 onto these more ventral neurons, we did see that reductions in cCA1 cFos expression was primarily found in neurons in vCA1. A possible explanation for the results observed by Meira, et al. is that CNO diffused from the ventral injection location and modulated CA2 axon in more intermediate regions. Moreover, previous work has detailed the reciprocal connectivity both within cCA1 and between cCA1 regions and the subiculum, another important, cortically projecting output region of the hippocampal formation [66]. As we provide evidence that rCA2 sends excitatory signals to dorsal and intermediate cCA1, the afferent connections to either more ventral or subicular projection neurons could further modify hippocampal information before subsequent transmission to corticolimbic regions implicated in stress and anxiety. Future work will be required to better understand not only how rostral hippocampal information is routed to extrahippocampal, cortical regions, but also how reciprocal connectivity within caudal projection neurons alters or modulates outbound transmission.
Activity in the ACC is modulated by stress. For example, in humans with post-traumatic stress disorder, exposure to emotionally relevant stimuli results in hypoactivity of the ACC when compared to healthy counterparts [67]. In mice, an early-life stressor, such as maternal separation, reduces the activation of ACC pyramidal neurons [68, 69]. Furthermore, repeated social defeat changes ACC IEG level in a manner consistent with the social hierarchy position (Basil, Gross et al. 2018). Caudal hippocampus is known to project to ACC [70], and we found that rostrodorsal CA2 neurons form excitatory synaptic connections onto cCA1 neurons that project to limbic regions such as the mPFC and anterior cingulate cortices. We also found that inhibition of CA2 during aSD (cre+ group, Fig. 2c, d) significantly reduces social investigation (i.e., increases “susceptibility”) in defeated mice that also had reduced cFOS+ cells in the ACC. Thus, these findings support the idea that a CA2→cCA1→ACC pathway may at least partially control an animal’s response to social stress acutely, as well as the memory of social stress. Given our results that defeated mice show reduced cFos levels along this pathway, and specifically in the ACC of the more avoidant, cre+ mice, future experiments could use Gq-DREADD mediated activation of either dCA2 or the ACC to effectively counter this deficit and restore the investigative deficits of susceptible mice.
Pyramidal neurons in CA2 express high levels of mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs), but not glucocorticoid receptors (GRs), which prompted this study to examine the role of CA2 in processing information related to a stressful social stimulus [33,34,35]. Even given the high levels of MRs in CA2, our experiments demonstrate that mice undergoing the fear conditioning (shock) protocol showed minimal avoidance behavior, showing that avoidance is not a generalized response to stress or stress hormones. Rather, the nature of the stressor does seem to be an important factor for later avoidance. We note however, that, in keeping with the mouse aSD experimental design first reported by Rosenhauer et al., subject mice were not presented with an empty wire cup prior to interacting with the CD1 littermate. For that reason, we are unable rule out the idea that subject mice are reacting to a novel object versus a novel social stimulus. Our data supports the idea that an animal’s behavior during the defeat (flee vs. defensive behavior) is likely to be a major contributor to later avoidance. Note that the two scenarios, either (a) CA2 activity is central for ‘learning’ the identity of an aggressor, or (b) CA2 activity is critical for defensive submission, which could be required for the resilient/investigative phenotype, would be exceedingly difficult to disentangle. Also still unknown is whether the resilient/investigative (CA2 activity intact) mice were simply better able to “learn” during the defeat the exact identity of the aggressor (better social discrimination during the defeat), leading them to be less likely to avoid the unfamiliar CD1 mouse. Some level of social discrimination is required for the avoidance behavior, however, in that defeat by a CD1 failed to lead to avoidance of a conspecific. Thus, CA2 activity, be it by modifying behavior in response to an aggressor, and/or by aiding in social discrimination, plays a critical role in the lasting effects of social defeat.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
McEwen BS, Bowles NP, Gray JD, Hill MN, Hunter RG, Karatsoreos IN, et al. Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1353–63.
McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med. 1998;338:171–9.
Fanselow MS. The role of learning in threat imminence and defensive behaviors. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018;24:44–49.
Romero LM, Gormally BMG. How truly conserved is the “well-conserved” vertebrate stress response? Integr Comp Biol. 2019;59:273–81.
Blake DD, Cook JD, Keane TM. Post-traumatic stress disorder and coping in veterans who are seeking medical treatment. J Clin Psychol. 1992;48:695–704.
Lewis SJ, Arseneault L, Caspi A, Fisher HL, Matthews T, Moffitt TE, et al. The epidemiology of trauma and post-traumatic stress disorder in a representative cohort of young people in England and Wales. Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6:247–56.
Yuan K, Gong YM, Liu L, Sun YK, Tian SS, Wang YJ, et al. Prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder after infectious disease pandemics in the twenty-first century, including COVID-19: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26:4982–98.
Krishnan V, Han MH, Graham DL, Berton O, Renthal W, Russo SJ, et al. Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell. 2007;131:391–404.
Bhatnagar S. Rethinking stress resilience. Trends Neurosci. 2021;44:936–45.
Nasca C, Menard C, Hodes G, Bigio B, Pena C, Lorsch Z, et al. Multidimensional predictors of susceptibility and resilience to social defeat stress. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86:483–91.
Zhang TR, Larosa A, Di Raddo ME, Wong V, Wong AS, Wong TP. Negative memory engrams in the hippocampus enhance the susceptibility to chronic social defeat stress. J Neurosci. 2019;39:7576–90.
Diaz V, Lin D. Neural circuits for coping with social defeat. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2020;60:99–107.
Willmore L, Cameron C, Yang J, Witten IB, Falkner AL. Behavioural and dopaminergic signatures of resilience. Nature. 2022;611:124–32.
Colyn L, Venzala E, Marco S, Perez-Otano I, Tordera RM. Chronic social defeat stress induces sustained synaptic structural changes in the prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Behav Brain Res. 2019;373:112079.
Cook SC, Wellman CL. Chronic stress alters dendritic morphology in rat medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurobiol. 2004;60:236–48.
Schoenfeld TJ, McCausland HC, Morris HD, Padmanaban V, Cameron HA. Stress and loss of adult neurogenesis differentially reduce hippocampal volume. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82:914–23.
Conrad CD. Chronic stress-induced hippocampal vulnerability: the glucocorticoid vulnerability hypothesis. Rev Neurosci. 2008;19:395–411.
Tse YC, Montoya I, Wong AS, Mathieu A, Lissemore J, Lagace DC, et al. A longitudinal study of stress-induced hippocampal volume changes in mice that are susceptible or resilient to chronic social defeat. Hippocampus. 2014;24:1120–8.
Misquitta KA, Miles A, Prevot TD, Knoch JK, Fee C, Newton DF, et al. Reduced anterior cingulate cortex volume induced by chronic stress correlates with increased behavioral emotionality and decreased synaptic puncta density. Neuropharmacology. 2021;190:108562.
Catale C, Martini A, Piscitelli RM, Senzasono B, Iacono LL, Mercuri NB, et al. Early-life social stress induces permanent alterations in plasticity and perineuronal nets in the mouse anterior cingulate cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2022;56:5763–83.
Nasca C, Bigio B, Zelli D, Nicoletti F, McEwen BS. Mind the gap: glucocorticoids modulate hippocampal glutamate tone underlying individual differences in stress susceptibility. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:755–63.
Hitti FL, Siegelbaum SA. The hippocampal CA2 region is essential for social memory. Nature. 2014;508:88–92.
Smith AS, Williams Avram SK, Cymerblit-Sabba A, Song J, Young WS. Targeted activation of the hippocampal CA2 area strongly enhances social memory. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21:1137–44.
Zhao M, Choi YS, Obrietan K, Dudek SM. Synaptic plasticity (and the lack thereof) in hippocampal CA2 neurons. J Neurosci. 2007;27:12025–32.
Kohara K, Pignatelli M, Rivest AJ, Jung HY, Kitamura T, Suh J, et al. Cell type-specific genetic and optogenetic tools reveal hippocampal CA2 circuits. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:269–79.
Raam T, McAvoy KM, Besnard A, Veenema AH, Sahay A. Hippocampal oxytocin receptors are necessary for discrimination of social stimuli. Nat Commun. 2017;8:2001.
Dudek SM, Alexander GM, Farris S. Rediscovering area CA2: unique properties and functions. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17:89–102.
Meira T, Leroy F, Buss EW, Oliva A, Park J, Siegelbaum SA. A hippocampal circuit linking dorsal CA2 to ventral CA1 critical for social memory dynamics. Nat Commun. 2018;9:4163.
Okuyama T, Kitamura T, Roy DS, Itohara S, Tonegawa S. Ventral CA1 neurons store social memory. Science. 2016;353:1536–41.
Phillips, ML, HA Robinson, and L Pozzo-Miller, Ventral hippocampal projections to the medial prefrontal cortex regulate social memory. eLife. 2019;8:e44182.
Leroy F, Park J, Asok A, Brann DH, Meira T, Boyle LM, et al. A circuit from hippocampal CA2 to lateral septum disinhibits social aggression. Nature. 2018;564:213–8.
Alexander GM, Riddick NV, McCann KE, Lustberg D, Moy SS, Dudek SM. Modulation of CA2 neuronal activity increases behavioral responses to fear conditioning in female mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2019;163:107044.
Kalman BA, Spencer RL. Rapid corticosteroid-dependent regulation of mineralocorticoid receptor protein expression in rat brain. Endocrinology. 2002;143:4184–95.
Herman JP, Patel PD, Akil H, Watson SJ. Localization and regulation of glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptor messenger RNAs in the hippocampal formation of the rat. Mol Endocrinol. 1989;3:1886–94.
McCann KE, Lustberg DJ, Shaughnessy EK, Carstens KE, Farris S, Alexander GM, et al. Novel role for mineralocorticoid receptors in control of a neuronal phenotype. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26:350–64.
Anacker C, Luna VM, Stevens GS, Millette A, Shores R, Jimenez JC, et al. Hippocampal neurogenesis confers stress resilience by inhibiting the ventral dentate gyrus. Nature. 2018;559:98–102.
Graybeal C, Feyder M, Schulman E, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ, Brigman JL, et al. Paradoxical reversal learning enhancement by stress or prefrontal cortical damage: rescue with BDNF. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1507–9.
Bissonette GB, Powell EM. Reversal learning and attentional set-shifting in mice. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:1168–74.
Bryce CA, Howland JG. Stress facilitates late reversal learning using a touchscreen-based visual discrimination procedure in male Long Evans rats. Behav Brain Res. 2015;278:21–8.
Morrison KE, Curry DW, Cooper MA. Social status alters defeat-induced neural activation in Syrian hamsters. Neuroscience. 2012;210:168–78.
Rosenhauer AM, McCann KE, Norvelle A, Huhman KL. An acute social defeat stressor in early puberty increases susceptibility to social defeat in adulthood. Horm Behav. 2017;93:31–38.
Rosenhauer AM, Beach LQ, Jeffress EC, Thompson BM, McCann KE, Partrick KA, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor signaling mitigates the impact of acute social stress. Neuropharmacology. 2019;148:40–49.
Osakada T, Yan R, Jiang Y, Wei D, Tabuchi R, Dai B, et al., A dedicated hypothalamic oxytocin circuit controls aversive social learning. Nature, 2024;626:347–56.
Alexander GM, Brown LY, Farris S, Lustberg D, Pantazis C, Gloss B, et al. CA2 neuronal activity controls hippocampal low gamma and ripple oscillations. eLife. 2018;7:e38052.
Stevanovic KD, Fry SA, DeFilipp JMS, Wu N, Bernstein BJ, Cushman JD. Assessing the importance of sex in a hippocampus-dependent behavioral test battery in C57BL/6NTac mice. Learn Mem. 2022;29:203–15.
Chen BT, Bowers MS, Martin M, Hopf FW, Guillory AM, Carelli RM, et al. Cocaine but not natural reward self-administration nor passive cocaine infusion produces persistent LTP in the VTA. Neuron. 2008;59:288–97.
Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat Methods. 2012;9:671–5.
Paxinos G, Franklin K. Paxinos and Franklin’s the Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. 4th Edition ed. 2012: Academic Press.
Huckleberry KA, Ferguson LB, Drew MR. Behavioral mechanisms of context fear generalization in mice. Learn Mem. 2016;23:703–9.
Russo SJ, Murrough JW, Han MH, Charney DS, Nestler EJ. Neurobiology of resilience. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:1475–84.
Li L, Durand-de Cuttoli R, Aubry AV, Burnett CJ, Cathomas F, Parise LF, et al. Social trauma engages lateral septum circuitry to occlude social reward. Nature. 2023;613:696–703.
Hoover WB, Vertes RP. Anatomical analysis of afferent projections to the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Struct Funct. 2007;212:149–79.
Cenquizca LA, Swanson LW. Spatial organization of direct hippocampal field CA1 axonal projections to the rest of the cerebral cortex. Brain Res Rev. 2007;56:1–26.
Dunwiddie TV, Lynch G. The relationship between extracellular calcium concentrations and the induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Brain Res. 1979;169:103–10.
Jackman SL, Beneduce BM, Drew IR, Regehr WG. Achieving high-frequency optical control of synaptic transmission. J Neurosci. 2014;34:7704–14.
Alexander GM, Farris S, Pirone JR, Zheng C, Colgin LL, Dudek SM. Social and novel contexts modify hippocampal CA2 representations of space. Nat Commun. 2016;7:10300.
Wood SK, Walker HE, Valentino RJ, Bhatnagar S. Individual differences in reactivity to social stress predict susceptibility and resilience to a depressive phenotype: role of corticotropin-releasing factor. Endocrinology. 2010;151:1795–805.
Larrieu T, Cherix A, Duque A, Rodrigues J, Lei H, Gruetter R, et al. Hierarchical status predicts behavioral vulnerability and nucleus accumbens metabolic profile following chronic social defeat stress. Curr Biol. 2017;27:2202–10.e4.
Lee SE, Simons SB, Heldt SA, Zhao M, Schroeder JP, Vellano CP, et al. RGS14 is a natural suppressor of both synaptic plasticity in CA2 neurons and hippocampal-based learning and memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:16994–8.
Simons SB, Escobedo Y, Yasuda R, Dudek SM. Regional differences in hippocampal calcium handling provide a cellular mechanism for limiting plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:14080–4.
Parfitt GM, Nguyen R, Bang JY, Aqrabawi AJ, Tran MM, Seo DK, et al. Bidirectional Control of Anxiety-Related Behaviors in Mice: role of inputs arising from the ventral hippocampus to the lateral septum and medial prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:1715–28.
Kim WB, Cho JH. Synaptic targeting of double-projecting ventral CA1 hippocampal neurons to the medial prefrontal cortex and basal amygdala. J Neurosci. 2017;37:4868–82.
Padilla-Coreano N, Bolkan SS, Pierce GM, Blackman DR, Hardin WD, Garcia-Garcia AL, et al. Direct ventral hippocampal-prefrontal input is required for anxiety-related neural activity and behavior. Neuron. 2016;89:857–66.
Fanselow MS, Dong HW. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron. 2010;65:7–19.
Watarai A, Tao K, Wang MY, Okuyama T. Distinct functions of ventral CA1 and dorsal CA2 in social memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2021;68:29–35.
Bienkowski MS, Bowman I, Song MY, Gou L, Ard T, Cotter K, et al. Integration of gene expression and brain-wide connectivity reveals the multiscale organization of mouse hippocampal networks. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:1628–43.
Shin LM, Whalen PJ, Pitman RK, Bush G, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, et al. An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:932–42.
Luo F, Deng JY, Sun X, Zhen J, Luo XD. Anterior cingulate cortex orexin signaling mediates early-life stress-induced social impairment in females. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2023;120:e2220353120.
Nephew BC, Febo M, Huang W, Colon-Perez LM, Payne L, Poirier GL, et al. Early life social stress and resting state functional connectivity in postpartum rat anterior cingulate circuits. J Affect Disord. 2018;229:213–23.
Fillinger C, Yalcin I, Barrot M, Veinante P. Afferents to anterior cingulate areas 24a and 24b and midcingulate areas 24a’ and 24b’ in the mouse. Brain Struct Funct. 2017;222:1509–32.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, U.S. National Institutes of Health (ES 100221). We thank the expert staff the NIEHS Fluorescence Microscopy and Imaging Center for their help with imaging.
Funding
Open access funding provided by the National Institutes of Health.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
SD and DR conceived of the study. DR and GA performed experiments. DR and KM analyzed the data. DR wrote the manuscript with input from SD, GA, and KM.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethics approval
All methods were performed in accordance with the with the United States National Institutes of Health guidelines for care and use of animals. Procedures were approved by the NIEHS Animal Care and Use Committee (Animal Study Protocol numbers 2009-0023 and 2011-0009).
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Radzicki, D., McCann, K.E., Alexander, G.M. et al. Hippocampal area CA2 activity supports social investigation following an acute social stress. Mol Psychiatry 30, 2284–2296 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-024-02834-9
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-024-02834-9