Introduction

Complex behaviours and higher-order cognition rely on distributed brain systems working synergistically for both serial and parallel processing [1, 2]. Extensively studied over the past 30 years, these functional networks are investigated by measuring temporal correlations between distributed and adjacent brain areas at rest [3]. Some networks, like visual or sensorimotor networks, are implicated in sensory processing, while others, such as the salience network (SN) and the default-mode network (DMN), are crucial for higher-order cognitive tasks like detecting salient stimuli or mind wandering. Examining functional networks through resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) to measure functional connectivity (FC) between regions [3, 4] is thus valuable to understand how neurodegeneration affects the brain and consequently, cognitive and behavioural functions.

Pioneering studies in resting-state FC have suggested that brain networks show both inter-network correlations and anticorrelations, meaning that cognitive and behavioural functions are not simply due to the activation of certain networks but also to an interplay between networks involving the simultaneous decrease and increase of activity within different networks [5]. These activities play a crucial role in the brain’s functional architecture, emerging during brain development [5, 6]. Similarly, changes in FC during aging and neurodegeneration involve not only abnormalities within networks but also a change of the interactions between large-scale networks [7, 8].

With this in mind, cortical gradient mapping offers a lens through which to characterise the relationship between connectivity patterns of macroscale functional networks in low-dimensional space. Applied to a large group of healthy individuals, this method describes a principal gradient of connectivity differentiation along the cortical surface, with sensory cortices presenting maximal FC pattern differences from regions involved in transmodal association processing (Fig. 1) [9]. In psychiatric and neurological disorders, gradient mapping identifies a dedifferentiation between sensory and transmodal networks such as ischaemic stroke [10], autism spectrum disorders [11], generalised epilepsy [12], depression [13, 14] and schizophrenia [15]. In such clinical conditions, the FC space in which the networks operate in contracts and networks become dedifferentiated. Dedifferentiation means that the neural representations of perceptual and conceptual information are less distinctive [16]. Thus, networks will have more similar patterns of connectivity and their activity may be less specific. This points towards an alteration of the interplay between networks, whereby there is a smaller difference between the activity elicited by a brain region’s preferred and less preferred stimuli. Gradient mapping presents advantages over other methods by minimising prior, not assuming sharp boundaries between functional networks, and therefore enabling the investigation of the interrelationships between functional networks within a continuous FC space. It has been suggested that low-dimensional gradients provide a realistic model of brain functioning and could better predict behaviour and cognition than conventional edge-based analyses [17].

Fig. 1: Schematic representation of spatial relationships of canonical resting-state networks and corresponding brain areas.
figure 1

Cortical connectivity gradients reflecting processing hierarchies spanning sensory and transmodal areas and the seven resting-state network parcellation on the cortical surface, colour coded according to previous work (Yeo et al. [31], adapted from Margulies et al. [9].

Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a heterogeneous group of neurodegenerative conditions presenting with distinct deterioration of behaviour, language and/or motor functions involving frontotemporal brain regions. Patients with behavioural-variant of FTD (bvFTD) show behavioural changes including apathy, disinhibition and impaired social cognition [18]. These patients show grey matter atrophy within anterior frontal, temporal, cingulate and insula cortices. In the language variants of primary progressive aphasia (PPA), patients with nonfluent variant (nfvPPA) present with speech apraxia and agrammatism while patients with semantic variant (svPPA) show anomia and impaired word comprehension [19]. While patients with nfvPPA show atrophy throughout the frontotemporal regions, particularly the motor and supplementary motor cortices, patients with svPPA have focal anterior temporal lobe damage. FTD has been proposed as a ‘molecular nexopathy’, where a specific conjunction of pathogenetic protein and neural circuit characteristics leads to the disease’s manifestation [20]. Nexopathies target particular types of network connections, transcending canonical macro-network boundaries. There is a growing body of evidence on FC changes across this FTD spectrum [21,22,23]. However, reported findings remain variable and their contribution to better understanding brain dynamics, effects of neurodegeneration and clinical consequences is debated.

The aim of the present study was to apply cortical gradient mapping to investigate FC changes in patients affected by bvFTD, svPPA and nfvPPA. We aimed to find a principal gradient of macroscale functional network organisation, spanning from the DMN to primary sensory networks in controls and anticipated that this would be broadly maintained in the FTD patient groups. However, our first hypothesis was that all patients would show evidence of an overall constriction of FC space compared with controls (i.e. sensory and transmodal networks would be dedifferentiated). Secondly, in view of the clinical symptoms and associated patterns of atrophy, we expected that patients with bvFTD would show the most widespread alterations of macroscale functional network organisation. Finally, we expected such network changes to be functionally relevant and relate to these patients’ clinical symptoms.

Methods

Subjects

Participants were recruited through two independent research studies within two research centres. Data collection from site 1 was part of the ECOCAPTURE study, sponsored by the French national institute for biomedical research (INSERM, C16-87), based at the Paris Brain Institute (more details here: https://www.clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03272230). Data collection from site 2 was part of the Longitudinal Investigation of FTD (LIFTD) study which took place at the Dementia Research Centre within University College London (more details here: https://www.ucl.ac.uk/drc/research/frontotemporal-dementia). A total of 129 participants were included in this study; 52 healthy control subjects and 42 patients with bvFTD across both sites, as well as 17 patients with svPPA and 18 with nfvPPA from site 2 (Table 1). Diagnoses were established based on current internationally recognised diagnostic criteria [18, 19]. Five patients with bvFTD from site 1 and one patient with bvFTD from site 2 had a pathological expansion in the C9 open reading frame 72 gene, four patients with nfvPPA were carriers of a progranulin C31fs mutation. There were no significant differences in age, education level, gender and MMSE scores between the two sites for each participant group (p > 0.05), thus these independently acquired datasets were merged for subsequent analyses.

Table 1 Demographic details for controls and each FTD patient group.

Cognitive assessments

Participants carried out extensive clinical, cognitive and behavioural assessments as described previously in these datasets [24, 25]. Tests in common to both sites and used for the purpose of this study included the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), to assess global cognitive status and disease severity, and the mini-Social cognition & Emotional Assessment battery [26] to evaluate deficits in social cognition (miniSEA). Socioemotional deficits due to impaired social cognition are a hallmark of bvFTD clinical phenotype. Mean scores, standard deviations and sample sizes are presented in Table 1. Patient groups’ and controls’ performance on these cognitive tests were compared using t-tests.

Imaging data acquisition and preprocessing

Volumetric T1 scans and resting-state fMRI scans were acquired at the neuroimaging core facility (CENIR) of the Paris Brain Institute and at University College London Hospital (UCLH). Sites were respectively equipped with a 3 T Siemens Prisma and Trio whole-body scanner and a 12-channel head coil. T1-weighted images were acquired using a magnetisation prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo pulse sequence (MPRAGE). Site 1 anatomical protocol involved TR = 2.4 s TE = 2.17 ms; TE = 2.17 ms; flip angle = 8°; voxel size=1 mm isotropic; slice thickness = 0.7 mm. Site 2 anatomical protocol involved TR = 2.4 s TE = 2 s; TE = 2.93 ms; flip angle = 8°; voxel size = 1 mm isotropic; slice thickness = 1.1 mm. Functional data based on the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal were acquired using a T2*-weighted echo-planar image (EPI) pulse sequence. Site 1 functional protocol involved TR = 2050 ms, TE = 25 ms, flip angle = 80°, oblique axial slices of the brain were acquired at 290 or 436 time points with a voxel resolution of 2 mm (with two types of acquisition of different timeseries lengths). Site 2 functional protocol involved TR = 2500 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle=80°, oblique axial slices of the brain were acquired at 200 time points with a voxel resolution of 2 mm. Participants were asked to lie with their eyes closed, without falling asleep during the resting-state acquisition run.

T1 scans and fMRI resting-state time series for all participants were preprocessed using fMRIprep 21.0.1 [27], an automated Nipype-based preprocessing pipeline for fMRI data implemented in Python, which uses tools from software packages including FSL, ANTs, FreeSurfer and AFNI. Briefly, the pipeline included bias field correction, skull stripping, brain tissue segmentation, slice time correction, correction for head motion parameters, co-registration to corresponding structural image, and non-linear spatial normalisation to MNI space. Further details on anatomical and functional data preprocessing can be found in Supplementary Methods.

Imaging data analysis

Anatomical scans

Measures of cortical thickness were obtained using FreeSurfer’s automated anatomical statistics extraction pipeline for each participant and for each parcel of the Schaefer atlas (400 parcels). To transfer the Schaefer parcellation volume to subject space, we used the Multi Atlas Transfer Tool [28]. To show structural grey matter differences in patient groups compared to controls, we averaged cortical thickness for each parcel within each patient group and presented these as percentage cortical thickness of control mean.

Resting-state scans

We used mean framewise-displacement [29] as a quality assurance parameter for each subject’s resting-state timeseries. Thus, subjects were included in subsequent analyses if their mean framewise head displacement in the MRI was below the threshold of 0.55 mm, as used in previous work with similar patient populations. Six patients with bvFTD (three from each site) and two with svPPA did not meet these criteria and were therefore excluded from subsequent analyses.

To remove physiological and other sources of noise from the fMRI timeseries, fMRI confounds generated with fMRIprep were loaded using the load_confound (v. 0.6.4.) Python package. Six motion parameters, signals estimated from cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and white matter (WM), their derivatives, quadratic terms, and squares of derivatives were regressed out from functional data separately for each run. The rs-fMRI data from each subject was smoothed with a full width at half maximum 6 mm Gaussian kernel, temporally bandpass filtered in the 0.01–0.1 Hz frequency range and spatially parcellated (400 parcels) according to the Schaefer atlas [30]. The Schaefer atlas was chosen for its basis in resting-state functional networks and its frequent use in previous functional connectome gradient studies.

Linear methods have been shown to offer better gradient reliability than non-linear alternatives [17]. For this reason, to estimate connectivity gradients, we applied generalised Canonical Correlation Analysis (gCCA) to all our subjects’ timeseries using a Python implemented package mvlearn (https://mvlearn.github.io/references/embed.html#generalized-canonical-correlation-analysis-gcca). This decomposes the functional connectome into primary components, referred to as gradients, with each gradient explaining varying levels of variance in connectivity. These gradients discriminate across levels of the cortical hierarchy (i.e., sensory processing vs. higher-order cognition), whereas region specific values along the gradient, referred to as embedding values, reflect the similarity in connectivity along the sensory-transmodal axis. Further details on the connectome gradient mapping specific pipeline can be found in Supplementary Methods.

Gradient mapping

We investigated principal and secondary gradient differences between FTD groups and controls. Each of the 400 brain parcels for which we extracted embedding values along the gradients belongs to a canonical functional network within the partition scheme described by Yeo and colleagues (Fig. 1) [31]. The present work focused on these two first functional gradients which each explaining a substantial portion of the data variance.

We performed a mixed effects model to compare principal and secondary gradient values for each parcel allocated to a given functional network between controls and each FTD group. Thus, gradient scores for each parcel were included in the model as the dependent variable, while network label as well as group were entered as fixed effects. Subject, parcel label and acquisition protocol (accounting for site but also protocol differences) were entered as random effects. Finally, age and sex were also included in the model as fixed effects. We then investigated the Group x Network interaction and performed post-hoc pairwise tests, comparing each network between controls and each FTD group. Resulting p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons including the three group comparisons and the 400 parcels using the Benjamini–Hochberg FDR correction.

Assessing relationships between gradients, atrophy and disease severity

For each patient group, we also computed linear mixed effects models to assess the effect of atrophy on gradient values for each parcel, including age, sex and protocol as covariates into the model. We then investigated if the effect was significant for each network.

For each subject, networks’ parcels’ thickness and gradient values were averaged. To assess whether functional gradients or cortical atrophy was more correlated with disease severity (assessed by the MMSE), these average network gradient and atrophy scores were then correlated with MMSE scores using Spearman’s correlations.

Correlation of gradient changes with cognitive measures

To investigate the clinical relevance of altered connectome gradients in patients with bvFTD, we computed a summary metric of each gradient corresponding to the range between both extremes of the spectrum. The distance between the DMN and sensorimotor network along the principal gradient and the distance between the SN and visual network along the secondary gradient are a measure of differentiation/dedifferentiation of networks, whereby the shorter the distance (constriction of connectivity space) the less differentiated the networks.

We computed partial correlations between the miniSEA with these summary metrics in patients with bvFTD, accounting for disease duration (number of years since first symptoms) and severity (MMSE). As the direction of change of the secondary gradient extremes were different, with SN constricting and visual network expanding the axis in all patient groups, we also specifically correlated the SN and visual network embedding values with miniSEA scores in patients with bvFTD, also accounting for disease duration and severity. Due to the cognitive protocols across the sites not being harmonised and as the miniSEA task can be lengthy to administer, only a subset of patients were included in these analyses (Table 1).

All statistical tests were conducted in RStudio (v 4.2.0). Results on the cortical surface are presented using the opensource python package ‘visbrain’.

Results

Demographics and cognition

As demographic details did not significantly differ between the two sites for each participant group (p > 0.05), these independently acquired datasets were merged for subsequent analyses. There were no significant demographic differences between patient groups and controls (p > 0.05), except for patients with nfvPPA being older than the other patient groups (p < 0.02) (Table 1). As expected, patient groups all had a significantly lower MMSE score than controls (p < 0.001) (Table 1). Moreover, compared to controls, patients with bvFTD showed significantly reduced scores on the miniSEA (p < 0.0001) (Table 1). Violin plots of patients with bvFTD and controls performance on the miniSEA are reported in Supplementary Fig. 2.

Cortical thickness

Each clinical FTD group showed reduced cortical thickness compared to controls in expected regions. Thus, patients with bvFTD showed an average of around 10% reduction of cortical thickness compared to controls in bilateral medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex and middle temporal gyrus as well as around a 20% reduction in the bilateral anterior temporal lobes and the frontoinsula region. Patients with svPPA showed reduced cortical thickness by up to 30% in left anterior temporal lobe, particularly the temporal pole, as well as up to around 25% in right temporal pole compared to controls. In nfvPPA, the pattern of reduced cortical thickness compared to controls was more widespread within the frontal and parietal lobes, involving up to 20% reductions within the supplementary motor area, particularly on the left, as well as up to 15% reduction in medial and inferior frontal cortex and left superior temporal gyrus (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2: Grey matter atrophy.
figure 2

Cortical thickness for each FTD group expressed as percentages of control means for each parcel. bvFTD behavioural variant FTD, svPPA semantic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia, nfvPPA non-fluent variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia.

Cortical gradients

In controls, the first two gradients explained a total of 48% of the variance; 29% and 19% respectively (Supplementary Fig. 3). There was no significant difference in the variance explained by each gradient between controls and the patient groups (Mann–Whitney-U, p < 0.05). The principal gradient anchored sensorimotor areas at its positive extreme and DMN at its negative extreme (Fig. 3a), with a gradual transition from sensory to transmodal association networks similar to what has been reported in previous work [9]. Along our secondary gradient, the visual network occupied the negative extreme, while areas from the SN populated the positive end of this gradient (Fig. 3b). Brain regions with the highest or lowest embedding values are at the extremes of the axis, contributing most to the latent component and having the most differentiated functional connectivity. Regions with similar embedding values have similar connectivity patterns, and those near the centre (embedding value around 0) contribute less to the latent component.

Fig. 3: Principal and secondary gradients in controls.
figure 3

Distribution of embedding values along the principal and secondary gradients for each canonical resting-state network in healthy controls, colour-coded according to Yeo et al., 2018 partition scheme. Below, the cortical surface is presented according to each parcel’s average embedding value along the gradients. Along the principal gradient, the sensorimotor network is anchored on one end of the spectrum in dark blue/purple (precentral and postcentral gyri) and on the opposite end lies the default-mode network in bright red (posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus, medial prefrontal cortex, inferior parietal lobule). Along the secondary gradient, the salience network is anchored on one end of the spectrum in dark blue/purple (anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex) and on the opposite end lies the visual network in bright red (occipital lobe).

Local alterations were notable, particularly widespread in patients with bvFTD and more focal in the language variants. Figure 4 reports results in patient populations. The distribution of patients for each network and along each gradient is presented in Supplementary Fig. 1.

Fig. 4: Gradients in the different FTD variants.
figure 4

The cortical surface is presented according to each parcel’s average embedding value along the principal (panel a) and secondary (panel b) gradient in each FTD variant group. bvFTD behavioural variant FTD, svPPA semantic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia, nfvPPA non-fluent variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia.

Principal and secondary gradients group comparisons

The mixed models comparing groups on the principal and secondary gradient embedding values for each of the 400 parcels identified main effects of Network (p < 0.0001) as well as significant Group x Network interactions (p < 0.0001). All p-values reported are corrected for multiple comparisons.

Pairwise comparisons at each network level found that patients with bvFTD showed significantly different principal gradient values within the DMN, the SN and visual network compared to controls (p < 0.0001), their embedding values shifting towards the centre of the spectrum. Similarly, patients with nfvPPA showed significantly different principal gradient values within the DMN and sensorimotor network compared with controls (p < 0.0001), with both extreme-end networks’ embedding values shifting towards the centre of the spectrum. Finally, patients with svPPA also showed significantly different principal gradient values within the limbic network (p < 0.0001) and the sensorimotor compared to controls (p = 0.04), again these networks’ embedding values shifted towards the centre of the spectrum. Figure 5 (left) shows the adjusted embedding value means along the principal gradient for each functional network, grouped for each FTD variant. Controls’ means are presented on the left for comparison. Details from the statistical model are presented in Supplementary Table 1.

Fig. 5: Network differences between groups along each gradient.
figure 5

Bar plot with error bars of average principal (left) and secondary (right) gradients for each group and for each network, colour-coded according to their position along each gradient axis. * indicate significant differences for each network, between the patient group and controls who are presented on the left in both graphs (p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons). bvFTD behavioural variant FTD, svPPA semantic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia, nfvPPA non-fluent variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia.

All networks along the secondary gradient in patients with bvFTD showed significant differences compared to controls (p < 0.01), with the largest changes occurring within the networks on either end of the spectrum, the SN and visual network (p < 0.0001). Though the SN showed convergence towards the centre of the spectrum, the visual network expanded the axis by shifting away from the SN. Moreover, the middle networks’ embedding values mostly shifted towards the SN end of the spectrum, apart from the DMN which shifted towards the visual end. Similarly, patients with nfvPPA showed significant changes along the secondary gradient with the same direction of changes of the SN and visual network as in patients with bvFTD compared to controls (p < 0.0001). Moreover, patients with nfvPPA also showed a significant shift of the limbic (p = 0.03) and sensorimotor network (p < 0.0001) towards the SN and thus away from the visual network. Finally, patients with svPPA showed an expansion of the spectrum with visual network shifting away from the SN (p < 0.0001) whilst the limbic (p = 0.010) and sensorimotor (p < 0.0001) networks’ embedding values significantly shifted along the secondary gradient, towards the SN. Figure 5 (right) shows the adjusted embedding value means along the principal gradient for each functional network, grouped for each FTD variant. Controls’ means are presented on the left for comparison.

Figure 6 presents an overview of the distribution of the embedding values along the principal (left) and secondary (right) gradient, grouped according to functional network with a distribution plot for each FTD variant and controls. More detailed results from the statistical comparisons are presented in Supplementary Table 1.

Fig. 6: Distribution of gradient embedding values for each network and for each group.
figure 6

Controls’ density plot colour-coded according to Yeo et al., 2018 partition scheme. Arrows indicate significant shift along the gradient for each group. bvFTD patients’ density plot/arrow in solid black line, nfvPPA patients’ density plot/arrow in dashed black line and svPPA patients’ density plot/arrow in dotted black line. bvFTD behavioural variant FTD, svPPA semantic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia, nfvPPA non-fluent variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia.

To ensure that within-group heterogeneity did not impact the results, we performed the same mixed model analyses a) excluding the genetic cases from the bvFTD and nfvPPA groups and b) comparing the PPA groups with only controls from site 2 as these patients were only from site 2. These control analyses did not show major differences (see Supplementary Tables 2 and 3).

Relationships between gradients, atrophy and disease severity

Supplementary Table 4 summarises the significant effects of cortical thickness on both the principal and secondary gradients for each network and within each patient group. In patients with bvFTD, there was a significant effect of atrophy within the limbic network and SN principal gradient (p = 0.001; p < 0.001) and within the DMN secondary gradient (p = 0.015). In patients with svPPA, there was a significant effect of atrophy within the SN principal gradient (p < 0.001) and marginally significant effects within the limbic and visual networks (p = 0.05). In patients with nfvPPA, there was a significant effect of atrophy within the dorsal attentional network, frontoparietal network, SN and visual network principal gradient (p = 0.04; p = 0.006; p = 0.05; p = 0.001) and within the dorsal attentional network, DMN and limbic network secondary gradient (p = 0.002; p < 0.001; p < 0.001).

In patients with bvFTD, only the DMN principal gradient was significantly negatively correlated with MMSE scores (rho = −0.40; p = 0.03). In patients with svPPA, only frontoparietal network cortical thickness was significantly positively correlated with MMSE scores (rho = 0.55; p = 0.04). Finally, in patients with nfvPPA, MMSE was significantly positively correlated with the cortical thickness within the DMN (rho = 0.56; p = 0.03), limbic network (rho = 0.65; p = 0.009) and frontoparietal network (rho = 0.59; p = 0.02) as well as with secondary gradient embedding values of dorsal attentional network (rho = 0.56; p = 0.03), DMN (rho = −0.66; p = 0.007), and visual network (rho = 0.59; p = 0.03). Supplementary Table 5 summarises these results.

Clinical relevance of principal and secondary gradients

We found a significant positive correlation (r = 0.40, p = 0.05) between the principal gradient range and social cognition performances (miniSEA) in patients with bvFTD. Similarly, the secondary gradient range showed a trend towards a positive correlation (r = 0.42, p = 0.08). In both cases, the more constricted the gradient, the worse the score on the miniSEA (Fig. 7a). Visual network was significantly negatively correlated with social cognition scores (r = −0.58, p = 0.011). In this case, the more the visual network embedding values were expanding the axis (smaller values), the better the score on the miniSEA (Fig. 7b). Figure 7 presents the simple correlation plots for interpretability. The partial correlation plots are reported in Supplementary Fig. 4.

Fig. 7: Correlations between social cognition and gradients.
figure 7

a Partial correlations between mini-Social cognition & Emotional Assessment battery (miniSEA) scores with the principal gradient (left; r = 0.40, p = 0.05, uncorrected) and secondary gradient (right; r = 0.42, p = 0.080, uncorrected) range, accounting for disease severity (MMSE) and duration (number of years since first symptoms) in patients with bvFTD. The smaller the gradients’ range (constriction), the worse the miniSEA scores. b Partial correlation between miniSEA scores and the secondary gradient visual network (r = −0.42; p = 0.011, uncorrected). The smaller the visual network embedding values, expanding the axis, the better the score on the miniSEA. DMN default-mode network, miniSEA mini-Social cognition & Emotional Assessment.

Discussion

In the present study, we used connectome gradient mapping to investigate macroscale functional network organisation in low dimensional space in patients affected by FTD. Healthy control subjects exhibited a typical hierarchy and differentiation of functional network connectivity patterns. The principal gradient captured a progressive hierarchy between sensorimotor regions (processing external stimuli) on one side and transmodal association regions (internal processes, like mind-wandering) on the other, consistent with previous findings [9]. This differentiation supports efficient external vs. internal processing. The secondary gradient sharply distinguished the SN and visual network, reflecting a separation between regions attending to external visual cues and those interpreting their social relevance, thus dissociating observed vs. predicted states. This aligns with recent studies showing a SN-visual axis in healthy controls [32] and a transmodal network, like the DMN, opposite the visual network along the secondary gradient [14]. These principal and secondary gradients explained 48% of the variance within the data, capturing a healthy hierarchy that enables the transition from concrete perception to abstract cognition, the basis of the evolutionary transition from apes to humans and of higher-order cognitive and behavioural functions [33].

In all patients with FTD, although broad gradients were maintained, network organisation was perturbed. Along the principal gradient, extreme-end networks shifted towards the centre, causing a constriction of FC space across the FTD spectrum, with focal changes in sensorimotor and limbic networks in patients with PPA. While the secondary gradient also showed these specific alterations in patients with PPA, it presented notable widespread changes in patients with bvFTD. Interestingly, the secondary gradient also revealed a common pattern in all FTD patient groups with the visual network expanding the axis.

Principal gradient – marker of neurodegeneration

Across all patient groups, the principal gradient was significantly constricted, whereby networks positioned at the extreme-ends of the spectrum (DMN and sensorimotor) shifted towards the centre compared to controls. This suggests that these networks’ FC patterns become more similar to other networks’, losing their specificity, a process known as dedifferentiation. Consequently, these networks may activate in situations where they normally would not or fail to activate where they should. Previous work in psychiatric or non-neurodegenerative neurological patients have shown such principal gradient constrictions [12, 15]. We are the first to demonstrate this dedifferentiation in neurodegenerative diseases. Dedifferentiation is most likely a mechanism common to many brain dysfunctions related to neurological conditions.

In this paper, we measured network dedifferentiation as the distance between the extreme-ends of the gradient representing overall constriction of FC space. In our patients with bvFTD, principal gradient constriction mainly depended on the shift of the DMN towards the centre and correlated with poorer social cognition scores (miniSEA). Social cognition dysfunction, one of the hallmark clinical disturbances in bvFTD, relies on maintaining a clear dissociation between external (sensorimotor) and internal (DMN) processing. As with the miniSEA, DMN FC changes in bvFTD significantly correlated also with a lower MMSE. Surprisingly, atrophy was not correlated with MMSE, highlighting the particular contribution of altered FC to bvFTD clinical picture.

Widespread alterations in bvFTD

In patients with bvFTD, along both gradients, we observed the SN shifting towards the DMN, thus reducing the differentiation between these networks’ respective activity. A decrease of SN FC in bvFTD is one of the most replicated findings in the field [7, 34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44]. It is believed the frontoinsula and anterior cingulate cortex, SN hubs, are rich of von Economo neurons and fork cells and, for this reason, particularly vulnerable to FTD pathology [45, 46]. Anatomically, these regions are the most vulnerable to grey matter degeneration. This explains why SN atrophy also contributed significantly to gradient values. Looking beyond regions of atrophy, changes in the interplay between the DMN and SN have been linked to symptoms in patients with bvFTD [34, 37, 47, 48]. Our findings of reduced differentiation between these networks support a model where the SN and DMN are anticorrelated, exerting inhibitory influence on each other, crucial for responding to prevailing goals and conditions [34, 49].

Some previous work has pointed towards FC changes extending beyond DMN/SN showing the interplay between many other networks is affected in bvFTD [7]. In line with this, along the secondary gradient, we observe that all seven functional networks showed significant shifts in bvFTD, even at relatively early stages of the disease (average disease duration of 3–6 years). Conversely, PPA patient groups did not show such striking widespread changes. It is likely that for patients with PPA, the interplay between networks plays a less important role, related to the more focal nature of brain damage, and associated proteinopathies.

Focal alterations in PPA

FC studies in patients with PPA are sparse, mostly reporting specific local frontotemporal or subcortical structure disconnections [50,51,52,53] with few investigating macroscale functional network changes. Along both gradients, PPA variants exhibited specific alterations within sensorimotor and limbic networks, with larger changes of the former in patients with nfvPPA and of the latter in svPPA. These functional changes are likely to be related to key areas of structural changes, such as motor language related cortices in nfvPPA [54,55,56,57] and limbic structures in svPPA [39]. Importantly, however, we highlight that atrophy within sensorimotor and limbic networks did not contribute significantly to these networks’ gradient values, supporting a specific role for functional changes in these patient groups. The specific contribution of FC to svPPA clinical picture has already been reported for the limbic network [58, 59].

When investigating the changes more thoroughly, we noticed that in our controls both sensorimotor and limbic networks were bimodally distributed along the secondary gradient, thus dissociating regions with differing patterns of FC. This bimodal distribution is lost in patients with PPA, in sensorimotor network for nfvPPA and in limbic network for svPPA. The 17-network parcellation described by Yeo and colleagues divides the sensorimotor strip into dorsal and ventral subnetworks, with the boundary between these roughly positioned between the hand and tongue representations [31]. The bimodal distribution of sensorimotor network may dissociate sets of body part representations, and the high kurtosis found in patients with nfvPPA could represent a blurring of this boundary. The loss of this bimodal representation might in our case correspond to an example of dedifferentiation at a more local level, within sensorimotor network, and potentially relating to the specific oro-facial symptoms characteristic of this patient group [19]. Similarly, Yeo’s 17-network parcellation dissociates the limbic network into two subnetworks, orbitofrontal cortex and temporal pole, which may underlie the bimodal distribution observed in controls. As atrophy in svPPA is particularly pronounced in temporal pole and less so in orbitofrontal cortex, the smoothing of the bimodal distribution may also be a result of this pattern of atrophy. Thus, our findings of specific FC alterations in sensorimotor and limbic network in patients with nfvPPA and svPPA are novel and warrant further investigation. Understanding the specific patterns of these networks, their relationship to function and their potential use as a functional signature in these patients is crucial.

Visual network functional compensation?

In all three patient groups, the visual network shifted outwards along the secondary gradient, expanding the axis. As functional networks’ activities are correlated and anticorrelated with one another, this change in visual network FC could reflect overexcitation caused by reduced inhibitory control from other networks as they accrue pathology and undergo atrophy, such as SN [36, 37]. In line with this, the SN’s FC pattern was significantly altered in both patients with bvFTD and nfvPPA and previous work has alluded to the SN’s reduced inhibitory control contributing to aberrant judgment and behaviour in bvFTD patients [60]. Another hypothesis is that visual network FC changes are a compensatory mechanism to maintain neural differentiation between predicted vs. observed processes. This is supported by visual network secondary gradient values significantly correlating with miniSEA scores in patients with bvFTD. A previous study demonstrated that patients with bvFTD increased fixations towards emotional faces to facilitate the contextualisation of emotional information [61]. This behavioural compensatory mechanism would presumably lead to visual network FC changes, such as those reported in our work.

Atrophy and functional connectivity

Atrophy and FC are undeniably closely related [45]. Our findings of DMN and SN changes are consistent with grey matter hubs of atrophy in bvFTD [62] with up to 20–30% grey matter volume reductions in regions within these networks in patients with bvFTD compared to controls. Similarly, our observed changes within sensorimotor and limbic network in nfvPPA and svPPA align with their respective atrophy patterns within sensorimotor cortices and orbitofrontal cortex/temporal poles [19]. However, our study highlights that atrophy patterns do not perfectly overlap with functional network changes [63]. This is further emphasised by finding that atrophy did not contribute specifically to the networks which showed gradient differences compared to controls. For example, in patients with bvFTD, along the secondary gradient, all seven networks were impacted but atrophy only within the DMN contributed. Thus, functional network changes do not seem to be fully explained by atrophy alone in any FTD subtype [64]. The finding that functional changes are more extensive than structural suggests that functional alterations may possibly be at the origin of symptoms, only later followed by structural changes.

We conducted additional analyses to evaluate the correlation of gradient and cortical thickness to disease severity, as measured by MMSE. In patients with bvFTD, FC measured within the DMN correlated inversely with MMSE. In nfvPPA both cortical thickness and FC correlated with MMSE. Finally, in svPPA only cortical thickness correlated with MMSE. These results suggest that within the spectrum of FTD, underlying proteinopathy, which differs among clinical phenotypes, seems to preferentially affect different brain features (grey matter and/or FC).

Interestingly, recent work has suggested that the longitudinal spread of atrophy can be predicted using an individual’s functional connectome [65]. Brown et al. found that the shortest path length in FC space to the epicenter, combined with quantifying atrophy within a region’s network neighbours, accurately estimated the spatial pattern of subsequent atrophy in patients with bvFTD and svPPA. This highlights the utility of FC profiles in predicting atrophy progression. However, the onset, evolution and relationship of pathological structural and functional changes in FTD are not fully understood, even at the group level, as studies have lacked multimodal longitudinal data [66].

Limitations

The present work has some limitations. Firstly, the associations of our functional gradient results with clinical and cognitive measures warrant further validation. Due to our limited sample size, particularly in the PPA variants, we were not able to extensively investigate the relationships between cognition/behaviour and functional gradients. Moreover, the miniSEA was unfortunately only available for half our bvFTD patients, limiting our interpretations. Finally, due to combining data from two sites, we were limited to cognitive and clinical tests which were in common across both cohorts. Thus, we chose the MMSE for assessing disease severity, however, we acknowledge that is not the ideal test for patients with FTD.

A second limitation is that the secondary gradient we present did not follow the “prototypical” secondary gradient first described by Margulies et al. [9]. Moreover, healthy controls showed large variability of embedding values within one same network. Others have reported similar gradients to ours [14, 32] and there are several factors which may explain these differences. Firstly, our sample’s average age is older than in most studies (controls were 67 years old on average), and gradients have been shown to change across the lifespan [15, 67]. Additionally, our method of extracting gradients using a linear approach is not the same as in previous work [9]. Linear methods have the advantage of producing better gradient reliability and by applying the dimension reduction directly to the timeseries, we directly address group-level consistency and do not require thresholding of functional connectivity matrices [17]. As this is the first study to explore functional gradients in patients with FTD (and one of the first in neurodegenerative diseases), and in view of our interesting secondary gradient findings, it will be important for these to be replicated in other samples.

A final limitation of the study is that the subcortical structures or the basal ganglia are not included in resting-state functional networks described in this work, making our results limited to cortical networks.

Concluding remarks and perspectives

In conclusion, gradient mapping offers a lens through which to characterise macroscale functional network organisation and relationships, essential for brain function. While specific network changes occur, this whole-brain, low-dimensional approach revealed widespread disruptions of the evolutionarily derived global network hierarchy in each FTD variant. Dissociations between internal/external processes, as well as between predicted/observed processes were significantly altered in these patients. Our secondary gradient may be a specific marker for bvFTD because it was so majorly impacted. As molecular nexopathies, correlating such FC profiles with underlying molecular pathologies is crucial to understanding the clinico-anatomical heterogeneity found amongst patients with FTD [68]. Refining these findings could help identify the specific role FC can play in disease detection, differential diagnosis and measuring disease progression in FTD.

Future research should focus on developing reliable FC profiles using connectome gradient mapping, particularly through longitudinal studies. Although methodological parameters require further optimisation, existing research indicates that low-dimensional connectivity gradients may outperform other approaches in predicting clinical scores and could serve as reproducible biomarkers. This potential, however, needs further investigation in the context of FTD.

Finally, if FC changes indeed precede brain atrophy, as some studies suggest, FC measures could serve as effective tools for patient stratification and for assessing the efficacy of disease-modifying therapies within a dynamic biomarker framework. Given that up to 30% of FTD cases are attributed to autosomal dominant genetic mutations [69], assessing FC gradients in presymptomatic individuals with these mutations would provide a valuable opportunity for early intervention and monitoring.