Abstract
Background
Low maternal cognitive empathy and higher affective empathy have been linked to increased emotional-behavioral problems (EBPs) in young children, but it remains unclear whether the associations are distinct according to maternal depression. This study aims to explore the moderating role of maternal depression in the association between maternal empathy and EBPs in preschoolers.
Methods
Cross-sectional and representative data were from 19,965 Chinese preschoolers. Maternal cognitive and affective empathy and depression were evaluated with Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy and World Health Organization Five Well-Being Index, respectively. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire was used to assess child EBPs.
Results
Lower maternal cognitive empathy was associated with increased child EBPs (aOR: 0.97, 95% CI: 0.96–0.97) with moderation of maternal depression (p = 0.002), and was slightly stronger in mothers at low risk for depression (aOR: 0.96, 95% CI: 0.95–0.97). Higher maternal affective empathy was associated with increased child EBPs (aOR: 1.03, 95% CI: 1.02–1.04), without significant moderation (p = 0.79).
Conclusions
Lower maternal cognitive empathy and higher affective empathy were associated with more EBPs in preschoolers. Maternal depression moderated only the cognitive empathy-EBPs association. Tailored strategies targeting maternal empathy according to various depression levels should be considered in clinical practices.
Impact
-
We found lower maternal cognitive empathy and higher maternal affective empathy were associated with more emotional-behavioral problems (EBPs) in a large-scale and representative sample of preschoolers in Shanghai.
-
We demonstrated the moderating role of maternal depression in the association between maternal cognitive empathy and EBPs in preschoolers, with the association being slightly stronger in mothers at low risk for depression than in mothers with depressive symptoms.
-
The study highlights that, aside from maternal depression, promoting interventions on inappropriate maternal empathy may confer significant benefits on the psychological well-being of preschool children.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Children’s emotional-behavioral problems (EBPs) merit keen attention due to their high prevalence and wide range of detrimental outcomes that span childhood to later life.1,2,3 These problems often emerge as early as the start of preschool, a critical transition period from a family-predominant environment to a richer social world. The first critical step to prevent and intervene in EBPs in preschoolers is to determine the most proximal and amendable factors. Bronfenbrenner’s socioecological framework4 posits that children’s socio-economic status, family environment, and parental factors form the microsystem with the most significant impact on their emotional-behavioral development.5,6,7 According to prior empirical studies informed by Bronfenbrenner’s framework,8,9 maternal depression has been identified as a priority predisposing children to EBPs. However, it remains unclear how maternal depression could moderate the association of other maternal psychological traits, cognitive and affective empathy in particular, with preschoolers’ EBPs.10,11 Addressing this research gap will inform tailored and integrated strategies to improve mental health of children with mothers at either low or high risk for depression.
Maternal empathy is recognized as an important psychological trait associated with child EBPs, with a notable difference between cognitive and affective components.12,13 Cognitive empathy reflects maternal ability to understand social situations, emotions and feelings of their children, and thus facilitates attuned parenting. Affective empathy refers to the ability to be sensitive to and vicariously experience children’s emotion, which can result in maternal distress when the mother becomes overwhelmed by a child’s distress.14,15 Maternal cognitive and affective empathy may be differently associated with child’s EBPs, according to maternal depression status. For example, in children having mothers with a clinical diagnosis of depression,16 a previous study showed that higher maternal affective empathy but not cognitive empathy was associated with increased child’s EBPs. However, in community children whose mothers were at low risk for depression, lower maternal cognitive empathy17 and higher affective empathy have been linked to more child EBPs.18 Thus, higher maternal affective empathy served as a consistent risky factor for child EBPs. Furthermore, both cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence suggested the moderating roles of maternal depression in the association of maternal parenting with child emotional expression, stress reactivity, and social competence.19,20,21 However, it is unclear whether the moderating role of maternal depression holds for maternal empathy and child EBPs.
Therefore, using cross-sectional data from a large-scale and representative sample of preschoolers in Shanghai, China, this study aimed to explore the moderating role of maternal depression in the association of maternal cognitive and affective empathy with child EBPs. Based on aforementioned evidence,16,17,18 we hypothesized that lower cognitive empathy and higher affective empathy of mothers would both be associated with increased child EBPs, with the associations being slightly stronger in mothers at low risk for depression than those in mothers with depressive symptoms.
Method
Study design and participants
This study used cross-sectional data collected in June 2020, as a part of the Shanghai Children’s Health, Education and Lifestyle Evaluation-Preschool (SCHEDULE-P) study.22 Briefly, a representative sample of children newly enrolled in Shanghai kindergartens in September 2019 was selected using a stratified random sampling design. All 16 districts in Shanghai were set as primary sampling units, and we defined nine secondary sampling units (SSU) based on the kindergarten’s ownership (public or private) and quality level (from highest to lowest is demonstration, first, second, and third level). In each SSU, one or two kindergartens were randomly selected, and all the newly enrolled children were invited to this survey. Finally, 25,354 children in 213 kindergartens were sampled. All parents gave informed consent on participating in this survey. We used cross-sectional data from the follow-up survey in June 2020, including 23,958 children (95.4% of the baseline participants; Fig. 1). We further excluded 713 samples deemed as careless responses, since they responded with the same answer towards all items of the questionnaire on maternal empathy.23 In the end, 19,965 (83.3% of participants in the follow-up visit; Fig. 1) children and their caregivers were included for the final analysis. The included sample (N = 19,965), compared with those who were excluded (n = 3993), was less likely to have low maternal education, low household income per year, maternal depressive symptoms, and divorced status (Table S1). The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Shanghai Children’s Medical Center, School of Medicine, Shanghai Jiao Tong University (SCMCIRB-K2016022-01).
Measures
Maternal cognitive and affective empathy
Maternal empathy was evaluated with the Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy (QCAE).12 The QCAE is a measurement for trait empathy, consisting of 31 items. Participants are required to assess their agreement with a series of descriptions about feelings without specific time reference on a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). This instrument defines cognitive empathy as the ability to construct a working model of the emotional state of others and is indexed by summing scores on the Perspective Taking (11 items) and Online Simulation (9 items) subscales (scores ranging from 20 to 80). Affective empathy is defined as the ability to be sensitive to and vicariously experience the feelings of others and indexed by summing scores on the Emotion Contagion (4 items), Proximal Responsivity (3 items) and Peripheral Responsivity (4 items) subscales (range from 11 to 44). A higher score indicates higher empathy. We used raw scores of cognitive empathy and affective empathy for main analyses. The QCAE possesses good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, convergent validity, and acceptable criterion validity.24 In the current sample, the Cronbach’s α is 0.92 for cognitive empathy, and 0.75 for affective empathy.
Emotional-behavioral problems
The 25-item Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) was developed to screen EBPs among children and adolescents during the past 6 months.25 Parents rate each item according to the children’s behavior in the last 6 months on a 3-point Likert scale (not true = 0, somewhat true = 1, and certainly true = 2). The total difficulties score can be computed by adding up the 20 items for full subscales of emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity, and peer problems (0–40), with higher scores indicating more EBPs. The total difficulties score was categorized into normal (0–14), borderline (15–16), and clinical (≥17) groups. In this study, the children in clinical group were identified as with EBPs. We also used the combination of borderline and clinical groups as a broader definition of EBPs for sensitivity analyses. The Chinese version of SDQ had good convergent and discriminant validity, with validated cutoff values for children aged 3–16 years in China.26,27 For the current sample, the Cronbach’s α is 0.76.
Maternal depression
The widely used World Health Organization Five Well-Being Index (WHO-5) is a short, generic rating scale that measures subjective well-being over the past 2 weeks and has adequate validity as a screening tool for depression in primary care.28,29 Five items are rated on a 6-point Likert scale that ranges from “at no time” (0) to “all the time” (5), resulting in a maximum score of 25. A total score <13 indicates poor well-being and warrants clinical evaluation for potential depression,30 which was used to distinguish mothers with depressive symptoms from those at low risk for depression, and explore the moderating role of maternal depression in the association of maternal cognitive and affective empathy with child EBPs in our study. The raw score of WHO-5 was also treated as a confounder in regression models. For the current sample, the Cronbach’s α is 0.82.
Confounding variables
Potential confounders for the association between maternal empathy and EBPs were identified using a directed acyclic graph (DAG) using DAGitty online software.31,32 We included children’s age, sex, maternal socioeconomic statues (maternal education level and household annual income), parents’ marital status and child empathy to build DAG.5,6,7,33,34 The Bivariate correlation matrix was also provided in supplemental material (Table S2). According to the developed DAG (Fig. S1), child empathy was considered as mediator between maternal empathy and child EBPs.35,36 Thus, we excluded child empathy from the minimally sufficient adjustment set to measure the total effect, instead of direct effect between our key variables in the multivariate regression models.31 Among those variables, maternal education was divided into junior high school and below, high school and junior college, undergraduate, master’s and above, and unknown and refused, while household annual income was categorized as <100,000 CNY, 100,000–300,000 CNY, >300,000 CNY, and unknown and refused. Parents’ marital status was recognized as divorced or not.
Statistical analysis
Descriptive statistics were employed to characterize the sample with respect to percentages and means with standard deviations across child EBPs. Differences in categorical variables were assessed using the Chi-square test, and continuous variables were analyzed with the t-test. We did a 3-step statistical analysis to explore the association between maternal cognitive/affective empathy and child’s EBPs. Firstly, logistic regression was used to examine the association between maternal cognitive/affective empathy and child EBPs, respectively. Adjusted models were generated with children’s age, sex, maternal education level, household annual income, parents’ marital status, and WHO-5 raw score. Secondly, to examine the moderating role of maternal empathy in the association of maternal cognitive/affective empathy with child’s EBPs, we included an interaction term (maternal cognitive/affective empathy × dichotomized maternal depression) in the logistic models. ΔR2 after adding the interaction term was used to assess the effect size of moderation.37 Thirdly, the unadjusted and adjusted regression models were replicated within maternal depression strata, dichotomized by the WHO-5. A 2-tailed Bonferroni-corrected threshold of 0.017 (0.05/3) was used to indicate regression significance. Several sensitivity analyses were used to evaluate the robustness of our findings. To minimize the effect of missing data, we performed multiple imputations on maternal cognitive and affective empathy, depression, and confounders for the sample who had completed the SDQ (n = 23,937). To address the representability of our sample, we weighted the sample by selection probability and sex ratio. To rule out potential bias from varied data sources, we adjusted the logistic model, including the role of respondent (mother, father, grandparents, or others). To examine the association between maternal empathy and more general EBPs, we used a broader definition (a borderline group also identified as children with EBPs) and raw scores of SDQ total difficulties as outcomes (Details in Supplementary Materials). All analyses were conducted using Stata v16 (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas) from September 2021 to December 2022. The study adhered to the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline.
Results
Study sample
The results were drawn from the complete-case dataset (N = 19,965; Fig. 1). The mean age of the children was 4.3 ± 0.3 years. Of the children, 51.3% were boys, and 34.1% were from a high-income family (household annual income >300,000 CNY). In terms of maternal education, 64.0% had at least an undergraduate degree. The prevalence of EBPs in preschoolers was 10.2% (n = 2,037), remaining 76.8% normal and 13.0% borderline in SDQ categories. Children with EBPs had higher level of maternal cognitive empathy, but lower level of maternal affective empathy. More children without EBPs had mothers at low risk for depression (86.2% vs 74.0%). Maternal cognitive empathy ranged from 20 to 80, and 11 to 44 for affective empathy. Both maternal empathy variables were slightly left-skewed with higher peaks and heavier tails (Skewness/Kurtosis: cognitive empathy −0.52/3.85; affective empathy −0.12/3.56) (Table 1).
Association between maternal cognitive and affective empathy and child EBPs
As shown in Table 2 and Fig. 2, adjusted by children’s age, sex, maternal education, household annual income, parents’ marital status and WHO-5 raw score, higher maternal cognitive empathy was associated with lower risk for child EBPs (aOR: 0.97, 95% CI: 0.96–0.97). The association was significantly moderated by maternal depression (p = 0.002), and was slightly stronger in mothers at low risk for depression (aOR: 0.96, 95% CI: 0.95–0.97) than that in mothers with depressive symptoms (aOR: 0.98, 95% CI: 0.97–0.99). The effect size of moderation was relatively small (ΔR2 = 0.07). Higher maternal affective empathy was associated with higher risk for child EBPs (aOR: 1.03, 95% CI: 1.02–1.04), without significant moderation effect of maternal depression (p = 0.79).
Sensitivity analysis
The results for the association between maternal cognitive empathy and child EBPs, and moderation of maternal depression were substantively the same after multiple imputation or inclusion of the respondent in the logistic model. When weighted by sampling probability and sex ratio, the association was not significant in mothers with depressive symptoms. Using a broader definition of child EBPs or considering the raw score of SDQ total difficulties as an outcome, the association between maternal cognitive empathy and child EBPs remained significant, without noting significant moderation of maternal depression (Fig. S2). For maternal affective empathy, all results were robust notwithstanding the weighting of the sample by sampling probability and sex ratio, resulting in a non-significant association in mothers with depressive symptoms (Fig. S3).
Discussion
Lower maternal cognitive empathy was associated with increased risk for EBPs in preschoolers. More importantly, we found that the association was moderated by maternal depression, and was slightly stronger in mothers at low risk for depression than in mothers with depressive symptoms. We also found that higher maternal affective empathy was associated with more EBPs in preschoolers, but not moderated by maternal depression. Furthermore, a series of sensitivity analyses endorsed the robustness of our findings.
Similar to previous research conducted with mothers at high risk for mental health problems,16,38 our study showed a reduced association between maternal cognitive empathy and EBPs in preschoolers in mothers with depressive symptoms, compared to those at low risk for depression. While the underlying mechanism warrants further investigation, it may reside in a period of low mood and loss of interest among mothers with depressive symptoms. This state of “emotional disinvolvement” can cause maternal cognitive empathy, the ability to “see things from the child’s point of view”, to become dysfunctional, thereby weakening the effect of maternal cognitive empathy in mothers with depressive symptoms.39 Even when depressed mothers have the appropriate ability to understand the reasons behind their children’s emotions, they still have difficulties providing appropriate parenting behaviors that align with their understanding.40
In mothers at low risk for depression, our study represents one of the first to reveal that lower maternal cognitive empathy was associated with higher risk for EBPs in preschoolers. Children of mothers with the lowest quantile of cognitive empathy had nearly two-fold increase in EBPs compared to those with the highest quantile (Fig. S4 and Table S3). Those mothers and children are less likely to be identified as potentially having difficulties since these mothers seem to be in positive well-being. Our results highlight that particular attention should be paid to preschoolers whose mothers have lower cognitive empathy to reduce their EBPs, even when their mothers are at low risk for depression. One of the potential mechanisms between maternal cognitive empathy and child EBPs is parenting behavior. Mothers with adequate cognitive empathy are able to recognize and respond to their children’s needs in a more responsive and supportive way (positive parenting).41,42,43 In contrast, mothers who lack cognitive empathy may struggle to understand their children’s perspectives when the children are not complying with their requests, which may result in hostility and overcontrol parenting behavior (negative parenting), even if they are at low risk for depression.35,44 Those different parenting patterns may impact early childhood social-emotional development, including parent-child attachment.45 An alternative explanation for these results is the limited use of emotion words by mothers when labeling children’s action or affect. According to the constructionist view of emotional development, mothers play a crucial role in children’s emotional development by continuous descriptions of their emotions.46 The use of emotion words helps children to learn context-dependent emotional conception for perceiving and expressing emotions, thereby reducing their risk for EBPs.47 While mothers with low cognitive empathy may struggle to fulfill this role.48,49
Furthermore, maternal cognitive empathy plays a role in not only mother-child interactions, but also family functioning. The existing literature has shown that a couple’s empathic ability and a partner’s perceived empathic concern are closely related to family functioning.50,51 Therefore, mothers’ low level of cognitive empathy also may affect children’s EBPs via problematic family dynamics (e.g., more family conflicts and fewer effective coping strategies). Genetic effects also may serve as a mechanism. Twin studies have found that approximately one-third of cognitive empathy variance is heritable.52,53 It has been well recognized that maternal psychological trait, cognitive empathy in particular is a primary and proximal contributor to child mental health according to the socioecological framework.4 However, theoretical and empirical evidence also supports the reciprocal relationship between maternal psychological trait and child EBPs.54,55 We are more interested in how maternal cognitive empathy can influence EBPs of preschoolers. Maternal cognitive empathy is considered as a more stable trait than the parenting behaviors and is less susceptible to the influence from the child’s behaviors, suggesting a more enduring and potentially influential role in the developmental process. Therefore, low levels of maternal cognitive empathy could be a potential target for reducing EBPs in young children.
Our results also indicated that higher maternal affective empathy was associated with more child EBPs, whether in mothers at low risk for depression or mothers with depressive symptoms. Children of mothers with the highest quantile of affective empathy in our sample had a 19 to 38 percent higher risk for EBPs compared to those with the lowest quantile (Fig. S4 and Table S4). This finding was in accordance with previous research in community sample and clinical high-risk sample.16,18 The affective empathy measured by the QCAE highlights negative emotion responses to unpleasant and neutral stimuli (e.g., I get very upset when I see someone cry; I am inclined to get nervous when others around me seem to be nervous.).56 Therefore, it is possible that maternal affective empathy is positively associated with their negative emotions elicited by children’s distress, which may lead to less support from mothers and more EBPs in children.57 Furthermore, these negative parenting behaviors could impede the acquisition of valuable emotional regulation strategies, resulting in more child EBPs.58 Taking the above findings together, the current study indicates that the moderating role of maternal depression is distinct in the associations of maternal cognitive and affective empathy with child EBPs. The disparity may be attributed to the fact that cognitive empathy, a top-down procedure requiring cognitive efforts, differs from affective empathy and operates as an automatic bottom-up process.59 Depressive symptoms may disturb the responsive parenting behavior motivated by high cognitive empathy. However, those symptoms may not affect the increased negative emotions elicited by children’s distress due to higher affective empathy.60
To be noted that, our study did not intend to negate the well-recognized significance of maternal depression in pediatric clinical practice. Instead, it focused on the broader community population, particularly the majority of mothers at low risk for depression. Maternal psychological traits (either depression or empathy) distribute as continuum, wherein some individuals may not manifest symptoms of mental health problems but could still increase children’s risk of EBPs. Thus, the coverage of maternal cognitive empathy advocacy, such as teaching them to recognize children’s emotion cues, in a population-wide health promotion strategy could be worthy of consideration.
Several limitations should be considered to interpret our findings. First, due to the population-based nature of the study, our data were collected via an online, self-reported questionnaire. Mothers’ subjective bias may under- or overestimate their empathy and children’s EBPs in the same direction and, thus, enhance the association. Besides, the same respondents providing information for all the key variables may inflate the association. However, the questionnaires we used were all validated in Chinese population, which ensure the validity to assess the key variables in our study. Concerning the respondent effect, we have conducted another regression model adjusting the role of respondent and duplicated the results (Figs. S2 and S3). Second, the cross-sectional design restricted the exploration of causal effect. Maternal cognitive empathy could be elevated during satisfying interactions with a well-behaved child. In addition, mothers could express less cognitive empathy and higher affective empathy when they become frustrated and overwhelmed in dealing with difficult children. The bidirectional association between these variables warrants a cohort or intervention study. Third, we did not measure maternal mental health problems (e.g., anxiety, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia) other than depression. Thus, the low risk mothers in our study may not be entirely at low risk for a mental health disorder. Future studies could control for additional maternal mental health risks and clarify the association between maternal empathy and child EBPs, and moderating role of maternal depression in the general population. Fourth, the SCHEDULE-P study is representative of the preschool population of Shanghai, one of China’s most developed cities. As in most cohort studies, attrition may have occurred over time, so as our studies, leading to potential selection bias. The overall high socioeconomic status and selection bias of the included dataset may limit generalization to other populations in low socioeconomic groups and also in other age stages.
Conclusion
Our study revealed that lower maternal cognitive empathy was associated with increased child EBPs, with the association slightly stronger in mothers at low risk of depression than that in mother with depressive symptoms. Conversely, higher maternal affective empathy was associated with increased child EBPs, independent of maternal depression. These findings indicate that inappropriate maternal cognitive and affective empathy may disturb child emotional and behavioral adjustment, even for mothers at low risk for depression. Therefore, tailored and integrated strategies targeting inappropriate maternal cognitive and affective empathy according to different risks of maternal depression should be considered in clinical practices and public health initiatives to reduce EBPs in young children.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to shared ownership of Shanghai Municipal Education Commission but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable requests.
References
Burke, J. D., Hipwell, A. E. & Loeber, R. Dimensions of oppositional defiant disorder as predictors of depression and conduct disorder in preadolescent girls. J. Am. Acad. Child Psy 49, 484–492 (2010).
Pardini, D. A. & Fite, P. J. Symptoms of conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and callous-unemotional traits as unique predictors of psychosocial maladjustment in boys: advancing an evidence base for DSM-V. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49, 1134–1144 (2010).
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J. & Ridder, E. M. Conduct and attentional problems in childhood and adolescence and later substance use, abuse and dependence: results of a 25-year longitudinal study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 88, S14–S26 (2007).
Bronfenbrenner, U. & Morris, P. A. The Bioecological Model of Human Development. in Handbook of Child Psychology 6th edn. (John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006).
Kremer, P. et al. Normative data for the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for young children in Australia. J. Paediatr. Child Health 51, 970–975 (2015).
Council on Community Pediatrics. Poverty and child health in the United States. Pediatrics 137, e20160339 (2016).
Hall, J. A., Schmid Mast, M. & Latu, I.-M. The vertical dimension of social relations and accurate interpersonal perception: a meta-analysis. J. Nonverbal Behav. 39, 131–163 (2015).
Cummings, E. M. & Davies, P. T. Maternal depression and child development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 35, 73–112 (1994).
Elgar, F. J., McGrath, P. J., Waschbusch, D. A., Stewart, S. H. & Curtis, L. J. Mutual influences on maternal depression and child adjustment problems. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 24, 441–459 (2004).
Meaney, M. J. Perinatal maternal depressive symptoms as an issue for population health. Am. J. Psychiatry 175, 1084–1093 (2018).
Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O’Hare, E. & Neuman, G. Maternal depression and parenting behavior: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 20, 561–592 (2000).
Reniers, R. L., Corcoran, R., Drake, R., Shryane, N. M. & Völlm, B. A. The QCAE: a questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. J. Personal. Assess. 93, 84–95 (2011).
Hoffman, M. L. in Emotions, Cognition, and Behavior 103–131 (Cambridge University Press, 1985).
Abraham, E., Raz, G., Zagoory-Sharon, O. & Feldman, R. Empathy networks in the parental brain and their long-term effects on children’s stress reactivity and behavior adaptation. Neuropsychologia 116, 75–85 (2018).
Perez-Albeniz, A. & de Paul, J. Dispositional empathy in high- and low-risk parents for child physical abuse. Child Abus. Negl. 27, 769–780 (2003).
Dittrich, K. et al. Alterations of empathy in mothers with a history of early life maltreatment, depression, and borderline personality disorder and their effects on child psychopathology. Psychol. Med. 50, 1182–1190 (2020).
Ojha, A. et al. Empathy for others versus for one’s child: associations with mothers’ brain activation during a social cognitive task and with their toddlers’ functioning. Dev. Psychobiol. 64, e22313 (2022).
Psychogiou, L., Daley, D., Thompson, M. J. & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. Parenting empathy: associations with dimensions of parent and child psychopathology. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 26, 221–232 (2008).
Field, T., Diego, M. & Hernandez-Reif, M. Depressed mothers’ infants are less responsive to faces and voices. Infant Behav. Dev. 32, 239–244 (2009).
Merwin, S. M., Leppert, K. A., Smith, V. C. & Dougherty, L. R. Parental depression and parent and child stress physiology: moderation by parental hostility. Dev. Psychobiol. 59, 997–1009 (2017).
Maughan, A., Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L. & Rogosch, F. A. Early-occurring maternal depression and maternal negativity in predicting young children’s emotion regulation and socioemotional difficulties. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 35, 685–703 (2007).
Wang, X. et al. Cohort profile: the Shanghai Children’s Health, Education and Lifestyle Evaluation, Preschool (SCHEDULE-P) study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 50, 391–399 (2021).
Meade, A. W. & Craig, S. B. Identifying careless responses in survey data. Psychol. Methods 17, 437–455 (2012).
Liang, Y. S. et al. Validation and extension of the questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy in the Chinese setting. PsyCh. J. 8, 439–448 (2019).
Goodman, R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 1337–1345 (2001).
Du, Y., Kou, J. & Coghill, D. The validity, reliability and normative scores of the parent, teacher and self report versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire in China. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 2, 8 (2008).
Kou, J., Du, Y. & Xia, L. Reliability and validity of “children strengths and difficulties questionnaire” in Shanghai norm. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 17, 25–28 (2005).
Fung, S. F. et al. Validity and psychometric evaluation of the Chinese version of the 5-item WHO well-being index. Front. Public Health 10, 872436 (2022).
Aujla, N., Skinner, T. C., Khunti, K. & Davies, M. J. The prevalence of depressive symptoms in a white European and South Asian population with impaired glucose regulation and screen-detected type 2 diabetes mellitus: a comparison of two screening tools. Diabet. Med. 27, 896–905 (2010).
Staehr-Johansen, K. Well-Being Measures in Primary Health Care—the Depcare Project. (World Health Organization, Regional Office for Europe, Geneva, World Health Organization, 1998).
Lipsky, A. M. & Greenland, S. Causal directed acyclic graphs. Jama 327, 1083–1084 (2022).
Textor, J., Hardt, J. & Knüppel, S. Dagitty: a graphical tool for analyzing causal diagrams. Epidemiology 22, 745 (2011).
Cooke, J. E., Racine, N., Pador, P. & Madigan, S. Maternal adverse childhood experiences and child behavior problems: a systematic review. Pediatrics 148, e2020044131 (2021).
Dadds, M. R. et al. A measure of cognitive and affective empathy in children using parent ratings. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 39, 111–122 (2007).
Hu, Y., Emery, H. T., Ravindran, N. & McElwain, N. L. Direct and Indirect pathways from maternal and paternal empathy to young children’s socioemotional functioning. J. Fam. Psychol. 34, 825–835 (2020).
Hawk, S. T. et al. Examining the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI) among early and late adolescents and their mothers. J. Pers. Assess. 95, 96–106 (2013).
Liu, H. & Yuan, K. H. New measures of effect size in moderation analysis. Psychol. Methods 26, 680–700 (2021).
Bi, S. & Keller, P. S. Parental empathy, aggressive parenting, and child adjustment in a noncustodial high-risk sample. J. Interpers. Violence 36, Np10371–np10392 (2019).
Bremner, J. G. & Wachs, T. D. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Infant Development, Volume 2, Applied and Policy Issues 2nd edn (The Wiley-Blackwell, 2010).
Stein, A. et al. Effects of perinatal mental disorders on the fetus and child. Lancet 384, 1800–1819 (2014).
Walker, L. O. & Cheng, C. Y. Maternal empathy, self-confidence, and stress as antecedents of preschool children’s behavior problems. J. Spec. Pediatr. Nurs. 12, 93–104 (2007).
Waller, R., Shaw, D. S., Forbes, E. E. & Hyde, L. W. Understanding early contextual and parental risk factors for the development of limited prosocial emotions. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 1025–1039 (2015).
Crocetti, E. et al. The dynamic interplay among maternal empathy, quality of mother-adolescent relationship, and adolescent antisocial behaviors: new insights from a six-wave longitudinal multi-informant study. PLoS ONE 11, e0150009 (2016).
Trentacosta, C. J. & Shaw, D. S. Maternal predictors of rejecting parenting and early adolescent antisocial behavior. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 36, 247–259 (2008).
Oppenheim, Koren-Karie, N., Abraha & David. Mothers’ empathic understanding of their preschoolers’ internal experience: relations with early attachment. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 25, 16–26 (2001).
Hoemann, K., Xu, F. & Barrett, L. F. Emotion words, emotion concepts, and emotional development in children: a constructionist hypothesis. Dev. Psychol. 55, 1830–1849 (2019).
Trentacosta, C. J. & Fine, S. E. Emotion knowledge, social competence, and behavior problems in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analytic review. Soc. Dev. 19, 1–29 (2010).
Bird, G. et al. Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain 133, 1515–1525 (2010).
Banzhaf, C. et al. Interacting and dissociable effects of alexithymia and depression on empathy. Psychiatry Res. 270, 631–638 (2018).
Kaźmierczak, M., Pawlicka, P., Łada-Maśko, A. B., van, I. M. H. & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. How well do couples care when they are expecting their first child? family and dyadic predictors of parental sensitivity in expectant couples. Front. Psychiatry 11, 562707 (2020).
Kenny, D. A. & Acitelli, L. K. Accuracy and bias in the perception of the partner in a close relationship. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 80, 439–448 (2001).
Knafo, A. & Uzefovsky, F. in The Infant Mind: Origins of the Social Brain 97–120 (The Guilford Press, 2013).
Warrier, V. et al. Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions and cognition. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 1402–1409 (2018).
Leerkes, E. M. & Crockenberg, S. C. Antecedents of mothers’ emotional and cognitive responses to infant distress: the role of family, mother, and infant characteristics. Infant Ment. Health J. 27, 405–428 (2006).
Bell, R. Q. A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychol. Rev. 75, 81–95 (1968).
Horan, W. P. et al. Structure and correlates of self-reported empathy in schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 66-67, 60–66 (2015).
Leerkes, E. M. Predictors of maternal sensitivity to infant distress. Parent. Sci. Pract. 10, 219–239 (2010).
Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K. & Teti, L. O. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: a clinical perspective. Monogr. Soc. Res Child Dev. 59, 73–100 (1994).
Cusi, A. M., Macqueen, G. M., Spreng, R. N. & McKinnon, M. C. Altered empathic responding in major depressive disorder: relation to symptom severity, illness burden, and psychosocial outcome. Psychiatry Res. 188, 231–236 (2011).
Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. & Aan Het Rot, M. Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. J. Affect. Disord. 150, 1–16 (2013).
Funding
The study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 82071493), the Shanghai Municipal Education Commission (No. 2022you1-2), the Science and Technology Commission of Shanghai Municipality (No. 2018SHZDZX05), and the Innovative Research Team of High-level Local Universities in Shanghai (No. SHSMU-ZDCX20211900, SHSMU-ZDCX20211100). Shanghai Municipal Health Commission (No. 2020CXJQ01).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Zichen Zhang conceptualized the study; contributed to the data collection, design, and conduct of data analysis; and drafted the initial manuscript. Yujiao Deng contributed to the conduct of data analysis and drafted and revised the manuscript. Tingyu Rong, Yiding Gui, Yunting Zhang, Jin Zhao, Wenjie Shan, and Qi Zhu contributed to the data collection and revision of the draft. Guanghai Wang supervised the data collection, conceptualized and designed the study, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. Fan Jiang coordinated and supervised the data collection, conceptualized and designed the study, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The study received approval from the institutional review board of the Shanghai Children’s Medical Center, Shanghai Jiao Tong University (SCMCIRB-K2016022-01). Parents of the involved children signed the informed consent before participating in the study.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Zhang, Z., Deng, Y., Rong, T. et al. Maternal cognitive and affective empathy related to preschoolers’ emotional-behavioral problems: moderation of maternal depression. Pediatr Res 98, 551–558 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-024-03770-8
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-024-03770-8