Abstract
The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene SLC6A4 has previously been dubbed a plasticity marker. Within the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism, a SNP (rs25531) in the L-allele in the promoter region, affects the transcription efficacy of the SCL6A4, leading to functionally important differences related to serotonin transporter availability in the synapses. 5-HTTLPR has been implicated in magnitude of negative attentional bias, a causal risk factor for depression, and the modifiability of attentional biases both in positive and negative directions. Hence, this genotype may moderate the outcomes of attention bias modification (ABM) targeted at reducing depressive symptoms. We conducted a registered randomized sham-controlled trial of ABM in a sample of 301 participants with a history of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) who had residual symptoms. They were randomized and underwent 14 days of two daily session of either ABM or sham at home. Of these, 264 provided genetic samples for determining the functionally important variant of the gene SLC6A4. We investigated if the SNP (rs25531) moderated the effect of ABM on symptoms of depression (HDRS, BDI-II) and anxiety (BAI), and attention bias post-intervention. None of the outcomes were moderated by the allelic variation in the promoter region of 5-HTTLPR. Limitations include low level of depressive symptoms, lack of data on ethnicity, current and prenatal level of stress, and early traumatic experiences. The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism did not moderate the effect of ABM on the symptom scales, nor attentional bias. Combination or interactions with other genes may be required for prescribing personalized interventions.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Attentional bias modification (ABM) is a digital intervention designed to modify negative attentional bias (AB [1]). AB is causally related to depressive symptoms [2] in all phases of Major depressive disorder (MDD), and modifying attentional bias through ABM has been shown to reduce residual symptoms of depression [3, 4].While trial outcomes are promising, the effect sizes tend to be small [5] or even non-existent [6], with the effect seemingly dependent on actual modification of attentional bias [7]. Some studies suggest that certain patient characteristics, such as levels of comorbid anxiety [8] and lower levels of inhibitory control [9], may modify the intervention efficacy. Another approach could be to explore whether genetic factors moderate the effects of ABM. The potential impact of genetic variations related to plasticity, affecting the individual’s sensitivity to both favorable and unfavorable environments [10] may be of relevance in the context of a targeted psychological intervention like ABM. The serotonin transporter polymorphism 5-HTTLPR, is proposed to hold implications for personalized treatment [11], potentially supporting the broader goal of personalizing in depression interventions [12]. Therefore, ABM could serve as a potential intervention for reducing depressive symptoms by implicitly targeting the underlying attentional bias in specific subgroups of patients.
The human serotonin transporter is encoded by a single gene (SLC6A4). 5-HTTLPR is a polymorphic sequence in the promotor region of SLC6A4. It is an indel, also containing a single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the insertion form only. From its regional location close to the promoter, the SNP (rs25531) influences the transcription of SCL6A4 [13, 14]. rs25531 affects the L allele and has two variants: LA and LG. The low expressive short allele (S’: S and LG) is associated with low genetic expression and thereby less protein (fewer transporters), which implies higher concentrations of serotonin in the synaptic cleft. In contrast, the high expressive long allele (L; LA) implies higher concentrations of transporters, and thereby lower level of serotonin in the synaptic cleft. Based on being either heterozygote or homozygote carrier of these alleles, three levels of transcription efficiency can be differentiated, which are found to be functionally important for serotonin transmission in the brain [14]. While variants of the 5-HTTLPR have been implicated in the magnitude of attentional bias [15], evidence also suggests that the magnitude of response to experimental manipulations is influenced by genetic makeup [16]. This metaanalysis suggests that experimental manipulations are more effective when participants have a susceptible genotype compared to a non-susceptible genotype, and they discuss that important main effects of interventions can be hidden by Gene x Environment (GxE) – interactions. In this context, ABM as a standardized and well-controlled intervention stands out as an ideal context for investigating GxE-interactions. Accordingly, this was studied by Fox and colleagues [10] who demonstrated that individuals without mental health disorders, having the low expressing, short variant of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism responded more profoundly to modification of attentional biases in both positive and negative directions compared to those with the high-expressing long variants. This finding indicates that 5-HTTLPR should be considered a plasticity marker rather than related to susceptibility only, thereby holding implications for treatment outcomes.
In this preregistered study, we examined data from a randomized controlled trial that demonstrated an effect of ABM on clinician-rated depression symptoms [4]. We hypothesized that genetic variants would modify the effect of ABM compared to sham on four outcomes: reductions in-clinician rated and self-reported severity of depression, reductions in self-reported anxiety, and reductions in negative attentional bias - thought to be the mechanism underlying ABM.
Materials & methods
This study is registered at Clinicaltrials.gov #NCT02658682.
Participants
The parent trial included 301 participants fulfilling the criteria of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) and having at least two prior episodes. Participants had primarily been treated as outpatients at a clinic in the Oslo-area. Individuals diagnosed with current- or former neurological disorders, psychosis, bipolar spectrum disorders, substance use disorders, attention deficit disorder, and head trauma were excluded. Details on the sample is provided in [4]. Of the 301, 264 were currently in remission and 37 had an ongoing depressive episode. Of the total sample, 263 consented to provide genetic data in the form of a swab sample to the Biobank “Genes in cognition and emotion” (REK Nord, t6/2006).
Genetic analysis
To extract DNA from buccal epithelial cells, participants were instructed to gently rub an Isohelix SK-1S DNA Buccal Swab against the inner cheek for 1 min. The collected DNA samples were stored at room temperature until analysis. DNA isolation was conducted using either the DNeasy Blood and Tissue kit from Qiagen or the BFK-50 kit from Isohelix, according to their respective standard procedures.
Genotypes were reclassified into a functional model based on the 5-HTTLPR-directed level of transcriptional activity of SLC6A4 as follows: LG/S, LG/LG and S/S genotypes were classified as SS (low leveled RNA transcription); LA/S and LA/LG genotypes were classified as LS (intermediate leveled); and LA/LA genotype was classified as LL (high leveled).
Intervention
All participants were randomized to receive either two weeks of ABM or a sham procedure. The specification of the intervention is detailed in [3, 4]. In short, through 28 5–7 min-sessions, twice daily for 14 days, they performed a 96-trials dot-probe task that included vertically presented pairs of facial stimuli (neutral, positive (happy) and negative (fearful and angry)) in equal numbers (negative-neutral, positive-neutral, and negative-positive). Sadness plays central role in depression; however, our ABM paradigm did not include sad stimuli. That said, angry and fearful faces also elicit amygdala activation, which is thought underlie attentional biases [17], thereby engaging neural circuits relevant for modification. After stimuli presentation (500 or 1 000 ms), participants identified the number of dots (∙or ∙∙ (the probe)) displayed in the same locations as one of the facial stimuli by pressing one of two designated keys on the keyboard as quickly and accurately as possible. In the ABM procedure, the probe was presented in the location of the relative more positive stimuli 87% of the time, whereas in the sham condition there were no contingency between type of stimuli and probe. The intervention was conducted at home on preprogrammed laptop computers provided by the research team.
Attention bias (AB)
AB was assessed by one session of sham ABM the day before (pre) and the day after the intervention (post), including another set of stimuli than the one used during the interventional period. Hence, the participants in the sham condition underwent a maximum of 30 sessions, while the participants in the ABM condition were given two sessions of sham, one before and one after a maximum of 28 session of ABM. Pre and post AB was calculated based on the difference in reaction time to the probe in the location of the relatively more positive stimuli compared to reaction time to the probe when it was in the location of the relatively more negative stimuli [4].
Randomization and blinding
Participants were told about the randomization procedure (ratio 1:1, based on a computerized random number generator) and that the trial aimed to explore the association between attention and mood yet remained unaware of their allocation and the distinctions between conditions. A separate laboratory technician programmed the laptops for intervention delivery and randomized participant based on a predetermined list. All evaluators remained unaware of participants’ treatment allocation, ensuring a double-blinded study design. Only after the completion of data collection was the randomization list disclosed.
Measures
At baseline, to assess patients based on DSM-IV criteria, we used a semi-structured clinical interview, the MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview PLUS 6.0.0 (M.I.N.I). The interviews were conducted by trained professionals or psychology students under supervision, all blind to study allocation. During the inclusion period, biweekly meetings to discuss cases and to uphold inter-rater reliability were performed.
We used Beck’s Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II [18]) to evaluate self-reported depression. Clinician-rated depression was assessed through the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS [19]) by experienced clinical psychologists and psychology students trained on case exemplars. Bi-weekly supervision meetings were conducted to ensure consistent rating criteria and reach consensus if deviation occurred. Self-reported anxiety was measured using the Beck’s Anxiety Inventory (BAI; [20]). Participants self-reported on current use of SSRI/SNRI (0 = no, 1 = yes). Educational level is evaluated based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED 2011).
Statistical analysis
ANOVA and Chi-tests were used for investigating differences between demographic variables, symptom level, and use of medication between the genetic groups and for investigating the distribution of the genetic variant between the two conditions.
The Process macro [21] was used for investigating whether the functionally reclassified 5-HTTLPR polymorphism (0 = SS’, 1 = LS’, 2 = LL’) moderated the effect of ABM (0 = sham, 1 = ABM) on the outcomes (change in HDRS, BDI-II, BAI and AB, operationalized as post-level – pre-level). The Process macro applies a linear regression model to the data, and we investigated if the relationship between the focal predictor (ABM) and the outcomes was dependent on the multicategorial moderator (dummy coded with SS as indicator). Both main effects and interaction effects were included in the model. Assumptions for the statistical tests were investigated. Leven’s test indicated homogeneity of variance between genotypes and residuals were normally distributed in all analysis. Significance level was set at p < 0.05. Tests were two-sided, and no corrections for multiple comparisons were made. Calculations conducted with G*Power 3.1.9.2, indicate that the current sample size could reveal medium sized moderator effects (f = 0.25), when Type-II error-probability was set to 0.15, with 5 predictor variables and 2 groups (ABM/sham).
Ethical considerations
The study was approved by the Regional Committee for Health Research for South-Eastern Norway (REK Sør-Øst 2014/217), and all participants provided written informed consent. The biobank was approved by (REK Nord t6/2006). The authors assert that this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
Results
Participants
The sample consisted primarily of women with higher education (see Table 1). A minority were currently using SSRI/SNRIs, and most had subclinical levels of anxiety and depressive symptoms and limited negative attentional bias pre-treatment. There were no significant differences on variables of interest between the three genetic groups (see Table 2). In our sample, the genetic distribution was as follows: SS – 40 (13.3%); SLG − 19 (6.3%); SLA − 123 (40.9%); LG LG – 1 (0.3%); LGLA - 25 (8.3%); LALA – 55 (18.3%). Hence, 42.2% of the participants had an S-allele, which is comparable to an expected ratio of S-alleles in a Caucasian sample (~40%). The genotype distribution was in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium, χ2(1, N = 263) = 2.99, p > 0.05, Cramér’s V = 0.067.
5-HTTLPR as moderator of ABM effect on depressive symptoms
HDRS - clinician rated depressive symptoms
With regards to change in HDRS as outcome variable, the overall model was non-significant [F (5, 257) = 1.401, p = 0.22, η2p = 0.027]. ABM condition had a non-significant main effect on reductions in HDRS levels from pre- to post-intervention, b = −2.197, t = −1.703, p = 0.090, CI [−4.737, 0.344]. As SS’ was the indicator used for the analysis, these results represents the non-significant difference of having either ABM or sham if you have this genetic variant. LS’ had no main effect, b = 0.111 t = 0.100, p = 0.92, CI [−2.08, 2.30], Cohen’s d = 0.006, nor did LL’, b = −0.326, t = −0.249, p = 0.80, CI [−2.91, 2.25], Cohen’s d = 0.015. The overall interaction effect between 5-HTTLPR and ABM on change in HDRS non-significant, ∆R2 = 0.0044, F (2, 257) = 0.563, p = 0.56, η2p = 0.004. There was not a significant interaction effect between ABM condition and LS’, b = 1.58, t = 1.032, p = 0.303, CI [−1.43, 4.58], Cohen’s d = 0.064, nor was there a significant interaction effect between ABM condition and LL’, b = 0.687, t = 0.372, p = 0.71, CI [−2.95, 4.33], Cohen’s d = 0.023. See Fig. 1 for bar chart visualizing the mean change in HDRS-levels dependent on ABM and 5-HTTLPR. Excluding participants currently using SSRI/SNRIs, did not affect the overall results.
BDI-II – self-reported depressive symptoms
With regards to change in BDI-II as outcome variable, there were no statistically significant main effects or interaction effects to report. Therefore, the findings are presented in a condensed format only. None of the effects approached significance; all p’s > 0.41, all F’s < 1.1, and all t’s < +/- 0.8. Exclusion of participants currently undergoing psychopharmacological treatment, did not change the results substantially. See Fig. 2 for bar chart visualizing the mean change in BDI-II-levels dependent on ABM and 5-HTTLPR.
5-HTTLPR as moderator of ABM effect on anxiety symptoms
Also, with regards to change in BAI as outcome variable, no moderating effect of 5-HTTLPR was detected, and neither did any of the main effects approach significance; all p’s > 0.27, all F’s < 0.57, and all t’s < +/−1.2. Exclusion of participants currently undergoing psychopharmacological treatment, did not affect the results. See Fig. 3 for bar chart visualizing the mean change in BAI.
5-HTTLPR as moderator of ABM effect on attentional bias
Finally, with regards to change in negative AB as outcome variable, no significant results were found. All results were far from significant; all p’s >0.50, all t’s < +/−0.8, and all F’s < 0.82. When excluding participants currently using SSRI/SNRIs, the overall results were not affected. See Fig. 4 for a bar chart visualizing the mean change in attentional bias.
Discussion
The main finding from this registered clinical trial suggests that the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism did not moderate the effect of ABM compared to the sham procedure. This was observed when investigating change in self-reported or clinician-rated depressive symptoms, change in anxiety symptoms, and attentional bias away from negative material the proposed mechanisms underlying the procedure. These findings indicate that efficacy of the intervention is not influenced by genetic variation in the serotonin transporter polymorphism. Therefore, based on this study, 5-HTTLPR does not appear to useful for personalizing ABM-treatment.
The field of therapygenetics, which explores the impact of genetic variations on psychological therapy outcomes, has provided some support for the use of single candidate genes (i.e., 5-HTTLPR) to predict treatment response [22]. However, growing evidence suggests that treatment outcomes – particularly in patients with treatment resistant depression and Major Depressive Disorder - may be better predicted by a combination [23] or an interaction [24] of multiple genetic polymorphisms rather than a single variant. Supporting this, studies have shown that a higher number of risk alleles moderate the negative effect of stress on proxies of depression (i.e., rumination [25]). Therefore, future studies should incorporate broader genetic datasets when investigating interventions to reduce residual symptoms in individuals with a history of MDD.
Our finding partly contradicts the findings in Fox, Zougkou [10] who reported that participants with the low expressive variant of 5-HTTLPR were more sensitive to experimental manipulation of attentional bias than those with intermediate and high expressive participants. Their results suggested that individuals with the low expressive variant exhibit greater plasticity in response to both positive and negative environments. However, in our study, we found no association between 5-HTTLPR and malleability of attentional biases towards more positive stimuli. Notably, we did not attempt to induce a negative attentional bias, which limits direct comparisons.
A key difference between the studies is the sample composition. First, our sample was more than twice as large, reducing the likelihood of extreme observations skewing the results. Second, our sample consisted of participants with a history of MDD compared to the sample without mental health disorders in Fox’s study. Since depression is associated with reduced plasticity [26], this may have constrained modification of bias. Consequently, the overall magnitude of bias change in our sample was smaller, and it was also in the opposite direction. Among the participants with the high-expressive genotype, the magnitude of change was the largest - though not statistically significant - compared to the other genetic variants.
We also did not find an association between 5-HTTLPR and AB at baseline, which has previously been reported [15]. Several factors may explain this discrepancy. First, the reliability of the dot probe paradigm for assessing attentional bias has been repeatedly questioned [27], and in our overall sample, reliability estimates were low [28]. Since [15] also relied on the dot-probe paradigm, its poor reliability may have contributed to inconsistent findings. Second, differences in how studies classify genetic variants, particularly the inconsequent consideration of the LGLA allelic variant, may have further contributed to the divergent results. Third, residual depressive symptoms in our sample might have affected reaction time estimates beyond the variation attributed to genetic factors. This suggest that future research should explore alternative methods for assessing AB.
Evidence suggests that recent exposure to stress and distal exposure to childhood trauma can affect vulnerability of carriers of the short allele of 5-HTTLPR through increased methylation [29]. We lack data on exposure among our participants, and we therefore cannot exclude the possibility that they are unevenly distributed among the three genotypes and therefore influence our findings. Belsky and Gaspar have suggested that the plasticity conferred by the short variant of 5-HTTLPR may be more profound during neuronal development than in adulthood [30]. Since we lack information on prenatal exposures, we cannot assess their potential role in shaping our results.
The generalizability of our findings to patients with more severe levels of depressive symptoms remains uncertain. While reduction of residual symptoms is important for reducing relapse risk [31], and presence of attentional bias in remitted phases of MDD may contribute to the relapse risk [3], the low levels of baseline symptoms also left minimal room for improvement. To enhance generalizability of our findings, future studies should also include patients with more severe symptoms.
We did not collect information on ethnicity in this study, however S-allele frequency in our sample indicates representativeness for a predominantly Caucasian population. Since previous studies have indicated that ethnicity may play a role in the relationship between genetic polymorphisms and treatment outcomes [32], the lack of such information is a limitation.
Moderation studies require a high number of participants to ensure adequate statistical power, and this study was powered to detect medium-sized moderation effects only. This indicates that smaller effect sizes would not reach statistical significance. At the same time, the effect sizes we did detect in the current study were indeed very small, raising doubts about their usefulness when personalizing treatment.
Summing up, the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism did not moderate the effect of ABM when compared to a sham condition. The negative finding may indicate that there are other factors, like interactions or combinations of polymorphisms that may be more relevant for the effect of the intervention than a single genetic polymorphism.
Data availability
The data underlying this study are not publicly available due to privacy reasons, but is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request and with necessary ethical approval.
References
Browning M, Holmes EA, Harmer CJ. The modification of attentional bias to emotional information: a review of the techniques, mechanisms, and relevance to emotional disorders. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010;10:8–20. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.10.1.8.
Wells TT, Beevers CG. Biased attention and dysphoria: manipulating selective attention reduces subsequent depressive symptoms. Cogn Emot. 2010;24:719–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802652388.
Browning M, Holmes EA, Charles M, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ. Using attentional bias modification as a cognitive vaccine against depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:572–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.014 .
Jonassen R, Harmer CJ, Hilland E, Maglanoc LA, Kraft B, Browning M, et al. Effects of Attentional Bias Modification on residual symptoms in depression: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. 2019;19:141. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-019-2105-8.
Fodor LA, Georgescu R, Cuijpers P, Szamoskozi Ş, David D, Furukawa TA, et al. Efficacy of cognitive bias modification interventions in anxiety and depressive disorders: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7:506–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2215-0366(20)30130-9.
Bø R, Kraft B, Pedersen ML, Joormann J, Jonassen R, Osnes K et al. The effect of attention bias modification on depressive symptoms in a comorbid sample: a randomized controlled trial. Psych Med. 2023;53:6389–6396. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722003956.
Grafton B, MacLeod C, Rudaizky D, Holmes EA, Salemink E, Fox E, et al. Confusing procedures with process when appraising the impact of cognitive bias modification on emotional vulnerability. Br JPsychiatry. 2017;211:266–71. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.115.176123.
Bø R, Kraft B, Jonassen R, Harmer CJ, Hilland E, Stiles TC, et al. Symptom severity moderates the outcome of attention bias modification for depression: an exploratory study. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;138:528–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.04.027.
Bø R, Kraft B, Skilbrei A, Jonassen R, Harmer CJ, Landrø NI. Inhibition moderates the effect of Attentional Bias Modification for reducing residual depressive symptoms: a randomized sham-controlled clinical trial. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2024;85:101982. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2024.101982.
Fox E, Zougkou K, Ridgewell A, Garner K. The serotonin transporter gene alters sensitivity to attention bias modification: evidence for a plasticity gene. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:1049–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.004.
Landrø NI. Towards personalized treatment of depression: a candidate gene approach. Scand J Psychol. 2014;55:219–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12124.
Herrman H, Patel V, Kieling C, Berk M, Buchweitz C, Cuijpers P, et al. Time for united action on depression: a Lancet-World Psychiatric Association Commission. Lancet. 2022;399:957–1022. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(21)02141-3.
Lipsky RH, Hu XZ, Goldman D. Additional functional variation at the SLC6A4 gene. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B:153. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.30766.
Hu XZ, Lipsky RH, Zhu G, Akhtar LA, Taubman J, Greenberg BD, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Med Genet. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1086/503850.
Pergamin-Hight L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bar-Haim Y. Variations in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene and biased attention for emotional information: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:373–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.030.
Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. The hidden efficacy of interventions: gene × environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015;66:381–409. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015407.
Shackman AJ, Stockbridge MD, Tillman RM, Kaplan CM, Tromp DPM, Fox AS, et al. The neurobiology of dispositional negativity and attentional biases to threat: implications for understanding anxiety disorders in adults and youth. J Exp Psychopathol. 2016;7:311–42. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.054015.
Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. 1988;8:77–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5.
Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol, Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56.
Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56:893–7. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.56.6.893.
Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach (3 ed.): Guilford Press, New York; 2022.
Eley TC, Hudson JL, Creswell C, Tropeano M, Lester KJ, Cooper P, et al. Therapygenetics: the 5HTTLPR and response to psychological therapy. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17:236–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2011.132.
Kautzky A, Baldinger P, Souery D, Montgomery S, Mendlewicz J, Zohar J, et al. The combined effect of genetic polymorphisms and clinical parameters on treatment outcome in treatment-resistant depression. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25:441–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.01.001.
Chang HS, Lee H-Y, Cha J-H, Won ES, Ham B-J, Kim B, et al. Interaction of 5-HTT and HTR1A gene polymorphisms in treatment responses to mirtazapine in patients with Major Depressive Disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;34:446–54. https://doi.org/10.1097/jcp.0000000000000143.
Clasen PC, Wells TT, Knopik VS, McGeary JE, Beevers CG. 5-HTTLPR and BDNF Val66Met polymorphisms moderate effects of stress on rumination. Genes Brain Behav. 2011;10:740–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00715.x.
Price RB, Duman R. Neuroplasticity in cognitive and psychological mechanisms of depression: an integrative model. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;25:530–43. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0615-x.
Meissel EEE, Liu H, Stevens ES, Evans TC, Britton JC, Letkiewicz AM, et al. The reliability and validity of response-based measures of attention bias. Cognit Ther Res. 2022;46:146–60. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-021-10212-w.
Hagen HS, Røssberg JI, Harmer CJ, Jonassen R, Landrø NI, Bø R. Does rumination moderate the effect of attention bias modification for depressive symptoms? J Affect Disord Rep. 2024. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2024.100824.
Palma-Gudiel H, Fañanás L. An integrative review of methylation at the serotonin transporter gene and its dialogue with environmental risk factors, psychopathology and 5-HTTLPR. Neurosci Biobehavi Rev. 2017;72:190–209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.011.
Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull. 2009;135:885–908. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017376.
Paykel ES. Partial remission, residual symptoms, and relapse in depression. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2008;10:431–7. https://doi.org/10.31887/DCNS.2008.10.4/espaykel.
Porcelli S, Fabbri C, Serretti A. Meta-analysis of serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) association with antidepressant efficacy. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;22:239–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.10.003.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
RB analysed the data and wrote the first draft. RJ collected and analysed the data and reviewed the manuscript. CJH TCS VØH designed the study and reviewed the manuscript. ØØ HLN QYE LL HH conducted the genetic analysis and reviewed the manuscript. BK EH collected the data and reviewed the manuscript. NIL acquired funding, planned and supervised the study and reviewed the manuscript. All authors approved the final version to be published.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
CJH has received consultancy fees from P1vital, Lundbeck, Sage Therapeutics, Compass Pathways, Zogenix outside of this work. Other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Bø, R., Jonassen, R., Harmer, C.J. et al. 5-HTTLPR does not moderate the effect of attention bias modification for depression: a randomized sham-controlled trial. Transl Psychiatry 15, 363 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-025-03581-3
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-025-03581-3