Abstract
Despite extensive work on the evolution of cooperation, the roles of teaching and leadership in transmitting opaque cultural norms—foundations of cooperative behaviors—are underexplored. Similarly, while teaching is well-studied in the evolution of instrumental culture, little attention is given to its role in transmitting opaque culture, such as social values and norms. Transmitting opaque culture often requires teaching, and group leaders are well-positioned to facilitate this process. Using comparative ethnographic data, we explore teaching, leadership, and instrumental versus opaque culture by examining whether opaque culture is primarily transmitted via teaching, which age groups tend to learn these norms, and whether leaders are disproportionately involved in teaching. Drawing on ethnographic data from 23 egalitarian foraging societies, we find teaching is more strongly associated with transmitting cultural values and kinship knowledge than subsistence skills and is closely linked to opaque culture and leadership. Leader-directed teaching may drive cooperation, suggesting new research avenues.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The success of human societies is fundamentally linked to our ability to internalize and adhere to complex social norms1,2,3,4. These norms are often opaque—meaning they are not readily inferred through direct observation or imitation—making their learning particularly challenging. Unlike instrumental culture, which often involves observable and directly learnable skills, opaque cultural elements require deeper cognitive processing, contextual understanding, and explicit instruction for effective transmission. A central aim within the field of cultural evolution is to understand how complex, opaque cultural norms are effectively passed down through generations5,6. As a feature of our evolved life history strategy, teaching is a universal and significant aspect of human social learning7,8,9,10. Teaching typically begins in infancy11,12, but opaque culture is understood and internalized by middle childhood13,14,15. As such, the teaching of opaque culture may be primarily targeted towards older children, reflecting their more developed social cognition and abstract reasoning abilities16,17.
Like teaching, leadership is also a human universal18. Evolutionary scholars define leaders as individuals who disproportionately influence group members’ views, cognition, or behaviors to achieve shared goals19,20,21. Leaders emerge in all social groups, including families, residential communities, extended kin networks, and economic or political organizations22,23,24, and are often characterized as hypercooperators19,25. At the community level, leaders often attain their positions of influence through the respect garnered by their success in culturally valued domains, especially in more egalitarian social contexts26. Core services leaders provide to followers include advising, counseling on proper behavior, and teaching—this is true across diverse nonindustrial societies and in postindustrial contexts18,27,28. Leaders can provide such services more effectively and efficiently than non-leaders because of their disproportionate social influence, specialized knowledge, and opportunities for one-to-many transmission29. In many societies, the transmission of social norms and values is closely tied to both teaching and leadership. In a representative cross-cultural sample of largely nonindustrial societies, Barry and colleagues30 found that authority figures, including community leaders and elders, play a crucial role in the early socialization of children, imparting cultural norms through both direct instruction and structured learning environments. Being described as knowledgeable or intelligent was found to be the most common quality of leaders in a sample of the ethnographic record of 59 diverse, largely nonindustrial societies18. In another similar sample, individuals with expertise in observable motor skills (i.e., instrumental culture) were often teachers with specialized knowledge associated with providing particular services like medical care31.
A deeper understanding of how complex social norms are transmitted through teaching can yield multiple societal benefits32. Better models of norm transmission can enhance educational outcomes by aligning teaching strategies with cultural expectations33, improve public health initiatives through culturally tailored campaigns34, and facilitate economic development by driving local business practices and social contracts towards group and individually beneficial outcomes35,36. Here, we investigate the social learning of cooperative behaviors and linkages with teaching and leadership among ethnographically described egalitarian hunter–gatherers. Egalitarian hunter–gatherers can inform evolutionary theoretical models given that (1) egalitarianism is believed to have deep evolutionary roots among the genus Homo, and (2) the diversity of socio-political organization across human societies during the Pleistocene would have likely developed from cooperative norms evolved in egalitarian socio-ecological contexts. Therefore, cultural packages of opaque cooperative norms were, putatively, the foundation for the emergence of more intensified and diverse social, economic, and political structures during the Upper Pleistocene and into the Holocene37,38.
Theories associating leadership and cultural transmission have primarily focused on imitation26, with relatively limited attention paid to leader-directed teaching as a mechanism for the evolution of opaque culture. Much of the empirical work on cultural transmission has also predominantly focused on imitation as a mechanism for learning, with less attention paid to the role of teaching39,40,41,42. When social learning scholars have examined teaching, most empirical and theoretical work has concentrated mainly on instrumental culture, with less emphasis on the teaching of opaque culture12,43,44. A refined understanding of these features of human sociality can provide new insights into our species’ cultural and ecological success. In what follows, we discuss the role of leaders in promoting cooperation-enhancing social norms, emphasize the cooperative nature of teaching45, and outline the distinction between instrumental and opaque culture. We then present results from text analyses, penalized regression analyses, and Bayesian multi-level modeling using comparative ethnographic data from a sample of egalitarian hunter–gatherers. Our results collectively suggest new paths for research on teaching, social influence, and opaque cooperative norms, which we frame as the leader-directed teaching hypothesis.
Given their strong emphasis on personal autonomy, egalitarian hunter–gatherer community members are not necessarily obligated to cooperate, and generally, leaders lack coercive capacity, and there is limited social stratification46,47,48,49,50,51. The transmission and maintenance of cooperative norms from generation to generation are collective action problems egalitarian societies must overcome via social and normative behavioral strategies, rather than institutional or coercive control. Among egalitarian hunter–gatherers, leaders are often respected, skilled in culturally valued domains, accomplished, have reputations for sound decision-making, strong oratory skills, and embody cultural ideals for proper behavior22,52. Recurrent descriptions of leaders as knowledgeable, intelligent, experienced, and accomplished indicate high levels of neural capital18,53,54,55, or the components of embodied capital56 associated with cognition and learning. Within groups, an important function of leaders includes using their expert knowledge and intelligence to provide counsel and direction to followers–essential elements of teaching18,57,58. In turn, leaders benefit from influencing followers by enhancing their social status, access to material resources, strengthening alliances, opportunities for marriage and mating, and promoting group cohesion18—all of which promote their own well-being, survival, and success. This suggests that leader-directed teaching may be an important mechanism for transmitting complex social norms. Thus, here, we investigate whether leaders play a disproportionate role in teaching opaque culture and how this teaching influences the perpetuation of cooperation-enhancing norms across generations.
Following cognitive psychologists and evolutionary anthropologists7,10,44,59, we define teaching as a process of social learning occurring when an individual modifies their behavior at some cost to themselves, specifically to impart knowledge, skills, or behaviors to a learner. It is a taxonomy of behaviors that solve adaptive problems created by the pressures for effective social learning7. Although teaching is not a uniquely human behavior7,45,60, the cognitive basis, process, function, and qualitative nature of human teaching represents a significant evolutionary development in the hominin lineage61,62. Unlike other forms of social learning, such as observation, imitation, or collaborative learning49,63, teaching requires deliberate effort to overcome informational asymmetries and facilitate a directional transfer of knowledge. Teaching is thus inherently cooperative, involving not only the investment of time and resources by the teacher (parent, elder, ritual leader, or technical specialist) but also the active engagement of the learner. Both the teacher and learner may thus benefit from this interaction, but each also incurs at least the opportunity costs of their participation32,45,64,65,66.
As the scope and complexity of essential cultural information broaden, teaching becomes more critical for effective cultural transmission67. Teaching begins as early as infancy and is a common practice with young children. Hewlett & Roulette12, for instance, documented teaching behaviors in infancy among Aka hunter–gatherers in the Congo Basin, where caregivers used ostensive cues like pointing and vocalizations to guide learning. Boyette & Hewlett11 further found that while foundational social norms are reinforced through teasing and feedback starting in early childhood, direct instruction—a more intensive form of teaching7—becomes more common as children grow older. During middle childhood, teaching not only increases in frequency but is also targeted toward transmitting skills that are challenging to learn, such as spear hunting, basketry, sharing, and cooperation11,68,69,70. By ten years, the development of several cognitive processes, including analogical reasoning, relational processing, and episodic memory, contribute to increased capacities for abstract reasoning17. At this stage, children are thus able to grasp complex and opaque cultural information, including supernatural beliefs13,14 and sharing norms15,71. Considering that, for transmission to be successful, teachers must also be attuned to the pre-existing knowledge and cognitive maturity of the learner69, here we investigate whether the teaching of opaque culture disproportionately occurs from middle childhood onwards.
Opaque culture refers to socially transmitted information that encompasses complex beliefs, values, norms, and practices. Unlike instrumental culture, which deals with tangible skills and material practices, opaque culture includes elements such as cultural history, social norms, kinship systems, appropriate language use (i.e., linguistic pragmatics), and the nuance of social etiquette1,72,73, which are all embedded in broader, often non-intuitive cultural frameworks. It also includes symbolic culture, such as religious beliefs and representations, ethnic ideologies, and moral norms and values74,75. Despite the fundamental role of opaque culture in human sociality, the evolutionary literature on social learning and teaching has primarily focused on the transmission of instrumental culture12,68,76,77,78,79,80, with less emphasis on the processes of transmission associated with learning opaque culture.
Yet, opaque culture often requires explicit instruction or deep contextual understanding for effective transmission and is not easily inferred through direct observation or imitation10. The successful transmission of opaque culture may often involve a process of deuterolearning, where individuals not only learn specific cultural content but also develop the cognitive frameworks necessary to interpret and internalize this information within their cultural context81. Deuterolearning enables individuals to ‘learn how to learn’ complex norms, making it possible for them to apply and adapt abstract principles across various social situations. Ethnographic work on the Mbendjele BaYaka in the Congo Basin provides a compelling example82, where the concept of ekila serves as a moral framework guiding behavior and cooperation. From an early age, Mbendjele children learn about ekila through implicit pedagogical actions that provoke curiosity, such as observing parents’ food taboos, or overhearing remarks about ekila. By middle childhood, however, more explicit teaching, such as explanation, advice, and initiation, is provided to help increasingly curious children understand the complex, non-observable rules of ekila. In turn, ekila acts as a vital structure for social harmony and well-being within the group82. Because opaque culture forms the foundation of much of human cooperation, here we investigate the mechanisms and socio-ecology associated with the transmission of such norms.
We aim to quantitatively evaluate relationships between evidence for teaching, potential differences for instrumental versus opaque culture, and associations with community leadership. We thus address three related questions:
-
1.
Is teaching more prevalent in the transmission of opaque versus instrumental culture?
-
2.
What age groups are primarily targeted for teaching opaque culture?
-
3.
Are community leaders or socially influential individuals likely to engage in teaching?
By exploring these questions, here, we show that opaque culture is transmitted primarily through teaching, particularly from older individuals to children in middle childhood, and that community leadership is tied to such instructional processes. In contrast, instrumental culture is often learned through non-teaching social learning, such as observation and imitation, indicating distinct pathways for acquiring different forms of cultural knowledge.
Results
To address our aims, we conducted exploratory analyses using ethnographic data from the hunter–gatherer social learning (HGSL) database83, developed by Garfield et al.63 using the electronic Human Relations Area Files World Cultures Database (eHRAF), a digitized repository of thousands of primary ethnographic texts describing more than 300 societies. These texts have been subject coded by the HRAF at the paragraph level following the outline of cultural materials (OCM) subject classification system, which consists of more than 900 subject codes covering all aspects of social life and human culture. The OCM classification system is hierarchical and nested, consisting of major groups that are composed of conceptually related individual codes. The HGSL data includes a sample of ethnographic paragraphs that discuss social learning, their associated OCM codes, and researcher-coded variables on specific descriptions of social learning, where each paragraph may include multiple unique instances of social learning description (henceforth termed instances). The database structure and coding process is further described in the “Methods” section and in the Supplementary Methods.
Our analyses relied on three sources of evidence at both the instance and paragraph levels. First, we used the OCM major subject and individual codes associated with the sample paragraphs to visually explore relationships between teaching and instrumental versus opaque culture and to statistically model counts of evidence for teaching at the paragraph level using the complete set of major subject codes as predictors. We then used a term-document matrix developed from the corpus of ethnographic paragraphs and text analytic techniques to identify the lexical content of ethnographic paragraphs associated with the OCM codes, indicating transmission of both instrumental and opaque culture. Lastly, we regressed instances of teaching, compared to other non-teaching social learning processes, on researcher-coded variables from the ethnographic paragraphs using a Bayesian multi-level logistic regression model. The model included cultural domain, mode of social learning, and the age and gender of the learner as predictors and accounted for the hierarchical structure of our data. These analytic approaches leverage three distinct (albeit related) sources of evidence: researcher-coded data, eHRAF-provided data, and original text produced by ethnographers, from which results could potentially converge.
The HGSL data includes \(149\) ethnographic paragraphs describing social learning among \(23\) egalitarian hunter–gatherers (see Supplementary Note 1). These paragraphs collectively include \(277\) descriptions of instances of social learning. See Fig. S1 for the geographic distribution of the sample and Garfield et al.63 for additional background information and descriptive results.
Opaque culture is primarily taught
The mosaic plot in Fig. 1 depicts the relationship between evidence for teaching versus non-teaching social learning (based on researcher-coded data) and opaque versus instrumental cultural transmission (based on eHRAF OCM individual subject codes), at the instance level (see also Table S2). Although instrumental cultural transmission frequently occurs through both teaching and non-teaching social learning, opaque cultural information is predominantly transmitted through teaching, with relatively fewer instances of non-teaching social learning. This descriptive relationship suggests the transmission of opaque culture is more commonly associated with teaching than other non-teaching social learning processes in the ethnographic record of hunter–gatherers.
Mosaic plot illustrating the relationship between evidence for teaching and non-teaching social learning, based on a binary researcher-coded measure of teaching at the instance level (1 = evidence for teaching, 0 = evidence for non-teaching social learning) with opaque versus instrumental culture (evidence for Opaque culture if that instance stems from a paragraph associated with OCM codes Transmission of cultural norms (867) or Transmission of beliefs (869); evidence for Instrumental culture if that instance stems from a paragraph associated with the OCM code Transmission of skills (868)). Areas of the bars are proportional to the counts of instances in that category. For illustration purposes, we removed 19 instances where both opaque and instrumental culture were associated with the paragraph providing the instance, as these were nearly evenly distributed between the evidence for teaching and non-teaching social learning and, thus, were not informative (n = 258 instances). Raw data are given in Supplementary Note 2.
Opaque culture positively predicts evidence for teaching
At the paragraph level, we used eHRAF’s major subject codes as predictors of evidence for teaching using an elastic net Poisson regression model (with lasso penalty, i.e., many predictors are shrunk to zero). Figure 2 displays non-zero coefficients from this model, indicating positive and negative predictors of teaching. In this model, coefficients correspond to the subject code groups predictive of greater counts of evidence for teaching at the paragraph level. Under the optimized \({\lambda }_{{MIN}}\) model the strongest positive predictor was Community Leadership with History And Culture Change, Kinship, Religious Practices, Religious Beliefs, and Traditional Ecological Knowledge being weak positive predictors—all features of opaque culture. The strongest negative predictors were Settlement Patterns; Art; Woodworking; Food Processing; Leather, Textiles, And Fabrics; and Sickness–all features of instrumental culture (excepting Art, which we return to in the “Discussion”). Under the more conservative \({\lambda }_{1{-{\rm{SE}}}}\) estimates, Community Leadership is the only meaningful positive predictor.
Coefficients are standard Poisson regression coefficients (i.e., log count ratio per standard deviation of predictor). Model weights were set at the number of instances per paragraph. All variables were standardized before fitting. Coefficients under \({\lambda }_{{\rm{MIN}}}\) (right) represent the optimized model determined via 10-fold cross-validation. Coefficients under \({\lambda }_{1{{\rm{SE}}}}\) (left) are those under \(\lambda\) within one standard error of \({\lambda }_{{\rm{MIN}}}\) (i.e., more conservative estimates). Triangles indicate positive coefficients, and circles indicate negative coefficients. Blue points indicate subject codes identified as opaque culture. Red points indicate subject codes identified as instrumental culture. Point color is for interpretation only and was not incorporated into the model fitting.
Lexical structure in the ethnography of instrumental versus opaque cultural transmission
Figure 3 displays non-zero coefficients from text analytic elastic net regression models (with lasso penalty). In these models, coefficient values correspond to the words within the complete 9945 word corpus (2909 unique words) of ethnographic paragraphs predictive of paragraphs being associated with the transmission of instrumental culture (i.e., paragraphs associated with the subject Code 868 Transmission of skills) or with the transmission of opaque culture (i.e., paragraphs with either subject codes 867 Transmission of cultural norms or 869 Transmission of beliefs).
Coefficients indicate the words whose frequencies in each paragraph best predict association with A transmission of instrumental culture or B transmission of opaque culture (based on eHRAF subject codes). Positive values are represented by blue triangles and negative values are represented by red circles.
Positive predictors tentatively associate the transmission of instrumental culture with subsistence skills and knowledge (bow, hunt, food, plant), manufacturing (canoe), and learning via imitation (imitate). Negative predictors dissociate the transmission of instrumental culture with teaching (speculatively, tell), behavior (act), leadership or social status (respect, power, role), kinship relations (wife, relative, family, generation, sibling), and religion and cosmology (religious, dream).
For the transmission of opaque culture, predictors tentatively imply positive associations with social behavior (behavior, act, people), leadership and status (respect), the supernatural and religious domains (sacred, dream), and teaching (tell, instruct, teach). Negative associations with the transmission of opaque culture include learning by imitation (imitate), material technology (canoe), and subsistence (shoot, fish, food, plant, hunt(er)). Note terms associated with interpersonal and family relationships included positive predictors (people, wife, child) and negative predictors (girl, daughter, accompany).
Opaque culture is taught by older individuals to children in middle childhood
At the instance level, we used researcher-coded variables to predict evidence for teaching (compared to non-teaching social learning) using a Bayesian multi-level logistic regression model, which accounted for the hierarchical structure of our data (i.e., instances within paragraphs within documents). See Fig. 4, Supplementary Data 1, and Supplementary Note 3 for model results. Contrast analysis of posterior distributions identified meaningfully different posteriors between levels within Domain, Age, and Mode, but not Gender (see Table S6). Evidence for teaching was more likely to be associated with social learning of cultural values and kinship compared to social learning of subsistence skills (\(\varDelta \hat{\beta }=\) 2, LCI 1.05, UCI 3) and manufacturing skills (\(\varDelta \hat{\beta }=\) 1.8, LCI 0.41, UCI 3.3); middle childhood compared to early childhood (\(\varDelta \hat{\beta }=\) 2.5, LCI 0.889, UCI 4.12); and vertical or oblique modes of transmission compared to horizontal transmission (contrast of vertical and horizontal reported here: \(\varDelta \hat{\beta }=\) 3.2, LCI 1.71, UCI 4.81).
Discussion
Based on comparative analyses of a sample of the ethnographic record of 23 egalitarian hunter–gatherers, we found evidence that the transmission of opaque culture was more frequently described as occurring via teaching, whereas instrumental culture was described as being transmitted via both teaching and other non-teaching forms of social learning (e.g., observation, copying). Older adults were more likely to be teachers and to teach individuals from middle childhood onwards. We also found that the OCM code Community Leadership was the strongest positive predictor of evidence for teaching in a penalized logistic regression model. In what follows, we illustrate these findings using the ethnographic record and interpret them considering cultural evolutionary theory.
Opaque culture was more commonly associated with teaching than instrumental culture. Features of opaque culture often include proscriptions for proper prosocial behavior and are more likely to require teaching specifically because they are opaque and difficult to observe or intuit. Aranda boys in the central Australian desert, for example, undergo an intense initiation process before marriage, designed to teach the intricacies of sharing norms among affinal kin. Penniman84 (pp. 31–32) explains:
“The phratry into which a boy is to marry first tries him out in the Alkirakiwuma, throwing him into the air, beating, and generally hazing him…While he remains in a dazed condition, he is instructed in the primary duties of generosity about food, and of respect for those who are older and wiser. The food which he gets by his own endeavors must be divided among his future father-in-law, his own wife-to-be, and children. What remains is to be divided between himself and his mura and ipmunna women, the women by whose marriage his wife is determined.”
Figure 2 revealed the subject codes positively associated with teaching exclusively captured opaque cultural domains (i.e., Community Leadership, History and Culture Change, Kinship, Traditional Ecological Knowledge, Religious Practices). In contrast, negative predictors primarily captured instrumental culture (e.g., Settlement Patterns, Woodworking, Sickness, Food Quest). Art, which we defined as opaque culture, but includes instrumental components, was also a negative predictor. Relatedly, text analysis of the transmission of opaque versus instrumental culture indirectly suggests that teaching (e.g., tell, teach) is associated with opaque culture. In contrast, other forms of social learning (e.g., imitate) are associated with instrumental culture (Fig. 3). Results from the Bayesian model of researcher-coded data (Fig. 4) further show that teaching is more likely for the transmission of cultural values and kinship than for manufacturing and subsistence, relative to non-teaching social learning.
These findings suggest that elements of instrumental culture may be more easily learned through less costly, non-teaching forms of social learning, such as observation and imitation, and horizontally in collaborative play groups. Shostak85 (p. 107) describes opportunities for social learning of subsistence, manufacture, domestic work, and childcare among San children in southern Africa:
“Many of their [children’s] games are imitations of adult activities: hunting, gathering, singing and trancing, playing house, and playing at parenthood and marriage. (Little children, carried about by older children, often become the “sons and daughters” of these “mothers and fathers.”) Occasionally their imitations become reality—foraging roots and berries in the area just beyond the village, or even catching or trapping small animals and birds. Because little formal teaching is done—observation and practice are the basis of all learning—it is in these groups that children acquire many of the skills that will make them productive adults.”
We also found that teaching was likely to be developmentally biased towards middle childhood and adolescence. These developmental stages are critical as individuals begin to understand the complexities of sociality. Wiessner86 (p. 32321) illustrates, “as individuals mature, they become increasingly aware of the complexities of cooperation with people in the outer layers of the onion of society.” In addition to learning important subsistence skills, Innokentii87 (p. 105) explains the emphasis on teaching of opaque cultural knowledge among Aleut adolescents in the North American Arctic:
“During the long training period, children learned not only the skills necessary to livelihood; they were also taught the proverbs and legends, the mores and values, the ideal and actual behaviors which constitute Aleut culture. Traditional controls were thus instilled in the children and the largely informal social structure was handed down.”
Finally, we found that teaching was most likely to have occurred via oblique and vertical modes—that is, from adults. The OCM code Community Leadership was the strongest positive predictor of evidence for teaching in a penalized logistic regression model, further suggesting that, although any adult may teach, leaders are often adept teachers. Among the Ona in Tierra del Fuego, Gusinde88 (p. 1455) explains that while anyone can teach during Klóketen, a boys’ initiation ceremony, elder leaders take on the primary instructional role:
“To be sure, everyone present is entitled to admonish and teach the candidates, but he does this rather casually and fragmentarily. The first and coherent teachings are the duty of the leader or of the man whom he appoints for this. This teacher, as I should like to call him, should basically be a dignified and influential elder, with blameless character and moral superiority, for whose uprightness others show the greatest respect.”
Leaders and prestigious individuals are ideal teachers because they will often be repositories of accumulated, domain-specific cultural information and because they tend to have teaching-related skills. For example, among Bolivian Tsimane, community leaders mediate disputes and possess traits such as extroversion, trustworthiness, confidence, and the ability to attract the attention of other group members20,89. Among Ethiopian Chabu, elected community leaders scored higher on peer-rated measures of intelligence and prestige (i.e., respect) and were more likely to be nominated as ideal mentors for learning culturally important skills compared to non-leaders27,90.
We now put forward the leader-directed teaching hypothesis to spark new research towards advancing our understanding of the evolution of cooperation, our capacities for teaching, our leader-follower psychology, and human uniqueness91,92,93,94,95. Cultural evolutionary theories of human cooperation have often emphasized the role of prestige and prestige-biased learning in influencing group behavior and shaping cultural change22,26,54,96,97. A notable example is the Big Man Mechanism proposed by Henrich and colleagues98, which suggests that prestige-based leadership plays a fundamental role in fostering cooperation within societies and that prestigious individuals, who derive status from skills and knowledge in culturally valued domains, can influence others to cooperate by modeling culturally appropriate behavior26. Theories of prestige-bias have been informative and influential in helping build contemporary evolutionary thinking on leadership and cooperation. Prestige-biased imitation, however, is only one side of the coin. Furthermore, systematic cross-cultural studies54,63 have only documented limited evidence that high-status, prestigious individuals are functional sources of learning through observation and imitation. Our findings suggest there is ethnographic evidence supporting teacher-directed social learning.
Several authors have implicated teaching in the transmission of more complex culture or in cumulative cultural evolution11,49,66,80. Norms concerning proper social behavior, kinship obligation, reciprocal economic exchanges, cultural histories, and religious values are difficult to both teach and learn, in part due to their opacity. Yet individuals and their groups can benefit if other group members comply with cooperative, complex norms. Within any group, leaders are well-positioned to transmit opaque cultural norms because their role provides them with a platform for broadcasting information to a wide audience. Leaders would be incentivized to actively teach naive individuals opaque cooperation-enhancing norms because promoting prosocial behavior will benefit themselves, but also their kin, and their social partners99,100, as well as better equip their groups to compete against and cooperate with other groups101. Thus, both within and between group cooperation will be enhanced if informed and influential group members actively teach prosocial opaque culture to naive and influenceable individuals. Leaders may also be further compensated for teaching via additional prestige, reciprocal altruism, mating opportunities, direct payments, or other “knock-on” benefits102,103,104. In sum, leader-directed teaching of opaque cooperative norms would be a mutually beneficial strategy, where the costs incurred by influential teachers are, at least partly, outweighed by the long-term benefits of cultivating cooperation within the group.
Importantly, we do not believe community leaders are especially likely to achieve their widespread social influence because of demonstrated success as teachers. The leader-directed teaching hypothesis predicts, instead, that individuals who have already attained social influence through various means may use their position to teach opaque cultural norms, thereby reinforcing their influence and ensuring the transmission of prosocial norms and behavior. Teaching can enhance a leader’s influence, but leadership is typically established through broader contributions to the community.
Among egalitarian societies in particular, respected individuals who have a disproportionate level of influence and who are informed on opaque culture, will be a key mode by which the cognitively challenging and costly instruction of opaque culture can occur; what Garfield et al.54 and Hagen et al.105 refer to as providing a “computational service” (see also ref. 100). However, leaders in egalitarian societies must balance this role with the risk of ridicule or loss of status if they appear to boast about their knowledge, as prestige-avoiding norms discourage overt displays of expertise18,51,106. To maintain influence and provide prosocial investments to the community, leaders need others to adopt the same opaque norms yet must find ways to share their knowledge without appearing boastful. Teaching provides an ideal solution, allowing leaders to demonstrate their knowledge prosocially, much like how hunters share meat to subtly display their skill without directly seeking praise. As our results demonstrate, the ethnographic record from egalitarian hunter–gatherer societies supports the prevalence of teaching practices closely associated with the transmission of intricate opaque cultural norms and social influence.
Our study faces limitations inherent in ethnography-based analyses, particularly in interpreting and coding ethnographic texts. Among the primary challenges is the potential for conceptual ambiguity in the language ethnographers used and their subjective biases. Our text analysis focused on the lexical dimensions of ethnographic passages, identifying patterns in cultural transmission by examining the usage and frequency of specific terms within the passages. This approach, however, is necessarily constrained by the inherent subjectivity of ethnographers and the specific cultural contexts they documented. To address these limitations, we examined potential sources of meta-ethnographic bias by fitting a Bayesian bias assessment model that considered factors such as the total number of pages of ethnography available for each society, document page count, publication date, and the presence of a woman co-author. Results suggested that none of these factors were associated with evidence for teaching in our data (see Fig. S3 and Supplementary Data 3). However, other unknown factors related to ethnographer variation might still influence our results.
Additionally, the ethnographic record is subject to the absence-of-evidence problem, where observable and public behaviors, such as teaching, might be reported more frequently than more subtle forms of social learning. Finally, since our sample is restricted to relatively egalitarian hunter–gatherer societies, we cannot generalize these patterns to all societies or societies characterized by other forms of social organization or subsistence economics. Our results provide insight into patterns within the ethnographic record, but generalizations should be made with caution. They are particularly useful, however, for evaluating theoretical models and generating hypotheses for further testing.
Here, we operationalized instrumental and opaque culture as a dichotomy. However, we acknowledge that in practice, instrumental and opaque cultural knowledge are rarely transmitted in isolation from each other107. Specifically, instrumental knowledge may represent a foundation upon which higher-level competencies and strategic adaptations, including opaque knowledge, is layered. Visual arts, such as painting, sculpture, and craftwork, are clear examples. Future theoretical work should examine the cumulative and interactive nature of instrumental and opaque knowledge108.
In principle, this work could face concerns related to the ecological fallacy, which arises when one infers individual-level processes from aggregate data. However, our analyses are conducted at the paragraph level, and our conclusions are similarly framed at that level rather than generalized to societies. We also include random intercepts for paragraphs and documents, thus capturing the nested structure of the data. Importantly, our findings are descriptive: they spotlight recurrent patterns rather than offer causal claims about individuals or societies. Subsequent investigation collecting finer-grained data on individual behaviors could shed light on the causal processes underlying these patterns. Future work should also examine how society-level norms and environmental factors shape leader-directed teaching. This might involve testing whether cultural traditions, phylogenetic relationships, or other socio-ecological dimensions map onto the paragraph-level patterns reported here. More granular field or observational data would help clarify how such macro-level influences interact with the more micro-level individual teaching processes we hypothesize.
Additional studies should also explore the mechanisms and decision-making processes through which opaque norms are transmitted and maintained within human groups. We suspect kinship intensity plays a role in shaping moral judgments. In a cross-cultural study based on ethnographic texts from 146 societies Curtin and colleagues6 found societies with high kinship intensity–defined as the degree to which social relationships, obligations, and moral judgments within a society are strongly influenced by the closeness and strength of kinship ties109—tend to rely less on mental states and intentions when making moral judgments, instead focusing more on outcomes. Therefore, focusing on macro-level, cross-societal variation in social structures could be insightful in understanding how norms and values related to morality are reproduced and maintained in different cultural contexts, including beyond egalitarian social contexts110.
The leader-directed teaching hypothesis suggests an integration and exploration of interactions between teaching, leadership, and the evolution of cooperation. The motivations and behaviors of informed, influential individuals and the responses of naive individuals to their teachings provide opportunities for further investigation and stands to enrich our understanding of human social dynamics, cultural evolution, and the emergence and maintenance of cooperation among human societies. It is our hope that future work will apply results from comparative analyses presented here in the field, the lab, and in formal modeling, towards testing and further developing the leader-directed teaching hypothesis.
Methods
This work complies with all relevant ethical regulations and was determined exempt from ethical review by the Institutional Review Board at Washington State University, given that no human subjects’ data were collected, and analyses exclusively rely on published archival texts.
The hunter–gatherer social learning database
We draw on the hunter–gatherer social learning (HGSL) database, initially developed by Garfield et al.63 and now publicly available83. The HGSL data set includes all the egalitarian hunter–gatherers available in the eHRAF at the time of its development, which included ethnographic texts on social learning. And here, egalitarianism is used to exclude hunting and gathering societies Kelly111 would describe as complex hunter–gatherers, e.g., hunting and gathering societies with substantial and institutionalized social stratification. It also excludes the equestrian foragers, which tended to demonstrate substantial inherited inequality. It does not include, for example, the Manus from Oceania. Although they are classified as hunter–gatherers, they are also characterized by hereditary slavery and do not meet the criteria of egalitarianism. This data set, like much of the evolutionary anthropological social learning literature, focuses on more egalitarian communities because it is assumed children have more agency and capacity for self-directed learning. There was no self-selection of societies or ethnographic documents by us or by Garfield et al.63 in the sample construction process.
The HGSL data includes \(149\) paragraphs from \(80\) ethnographic documents describing social learning among hunter–gatherers taken from the electronic Human Relations Area Files World Cultures (eHRAF) database (https://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu), in compliance with eHRAF terms of service. These \(149\) paragraphs included \(277\) unique descriptions of social learning, referred to as instances of social learning. Each instance was coded for the social domain in which learning occurred (e.g., subsistence skills, cultural values and kinship), the mode of learning (e.g., horizontal, vertical), the process of social learning (e.g., teaching, observation, stimulus enhancement, trial and error), and the gender and age of the learner (see Table 1). For our purposes here, we made the following revisions to the original coding scheme: we collapsed the Language and Miscellaneous skills domains, we collapsed the oblique mode with the two oblique subtypes; we also collapsed the three teaching processes into teaching, all other processes into non-teaching social learning; and lastly, and we removed the Individual learning data. This provides our binary measure of teaching, which is coded as \(1\) when an instance provides evidence of teaching and \(0\) when it provides evidence for another non-teaching social learning process.
The HGSL database also provides paragraph and society-level meta-data. For each paragraph the associated eHRAF document ID is provided, the raw text, as well as every outline of cultural materials (OCM) code associated with the paragraph. The OCM coding scheme is an ethnographic classification system of human behavior, social life and customs, material culture, biology, and human-ecological environments, designed to capture a wide range of topics relevant to the human sciences. The core and unique feature of the HRAF World Cultures database has been to associate each of more than 2.5 million paragraphs, stemming from more than 6000 primary ethnographic documents, describing more than 350 societies, with the relevant OCM codes discussed in the paragraph. The OCM coding scheme is hierarchically organized and includes more than 90 OCM major categories (such as 550 Social Stratification and 740 Health and Welfare) which contain more than 700 individual codes (such as 554 Status, role, & prestige; 744 Public Health & Sanitation). The HGSL database used three OCM codes in an advanced search to extract ethnographic paragraphs on cultural transmission: 867 Transmission of Cultural Norms, 868 Transmission of Skills, and 869 Transmission of Beliefs for a subset of relatively egalitarian hunter–gatherer societies–all the egalitarian hunter–gatherer societies, following definitions from Kelly111, available at the time of data collection (see Garfield et al.63 for further details). The HGSL database also includes society-level meta-data, such as geo-coordinates for each society and the region and sub-region (provided by HRAF). This database provides quantitative measures developed from a systematic sample of ethnographic paragraphs on hunter–gatherers, suitable for quantitative analyses, with limited selection bias (e.g., due to “cherry-picked” cases, documents, or societies).
Analytic strategy
We use text analysis, exploratory graphical methods, Bayesian regression, and penalized regression towards addressing our aims. All empirical analyses were conducted with R version 4.3.2 (2023-10-31).
Descriptive visualization
We use a mosaic plot to visually explore the relationship between teaching and the transmission of opaque versus instrumental culture. Figure 1 illustrates the relationship between evidence for teaching versus non-teaching social learning and the type of cultural transmission (opaque versus instrumental) across the instances of social learning. Specifically, the plot depicts the counts of instances which provide evidence for teaching or non-teaching social learning and how these instances are associated with either opaque or instrumental culture OCM codes within the respective paragraphs.
For clarity, we excluded 19 instances where both opaque and instrumental culture were associated with the same paragraph, as these cases were nearly evenly distributed between teaching and non-teaching social learning and did not contribute additional information.
Text analysis of hunter–gatherer cultural transmission
We conduct text analysis to explore the lexical content of the ethnography of social learning in reference to transmission of opaque versus instrumental culture. We created a term document matrix of all informative words in our corpus of texts, which captures the frequency of each unique term within each paragraph. We first identified if each paragraph provided evidence for transmission of instrumental culture (i.e., paragraphs associated with the OCM code 868 Transmission of skills) and/or opaque culture (i.e., paragraphs associated with either 867 Transmission of cultural norms or 869 Transmission of beliefs). We then fitted an elastic net logistic regression model (with the lasso penalty, \(\alpha=1\)) of both of these outcomes as a function of the frequencies of all 2909 unique words using the glmnet package112. Words that were strong positive predictors epitomized the lexical content of paragraphs which provided evidence for that type of social learning and negative predictors epitomized the lexical content of paragraphs negatively associated with that type of social learning.
This approach allows the ethnographic record to speak for itself by uncovering patterns in lexical content that may not be apparent through manual coding methods, providing an additional level of validation. It also offers readers a window into the content of the paragraphs without directly reading them. Moreover, by analyzing the raw text independent of researcher-coded variables, we include a source of evidence that has not been directly shaped by our research design choices. This not only strengthens the case for the alignment between lexical content and OCM codes but offers some assurance that patterns in the data emerge from the text in its own right, rather than being overly shaped by prior frameworks.
Penalized regression model with OCM predictors
We leveraged the OCM codes provided by eHRAF as predictor variables to identify the topics and features commonly associated with ethnographic descriptions of teaching among egalitarian hunter–gatherers. We use the OCM categories (specifically the sum of the number of OCM individual codes per category, for each paragraph), as predictors of evidence for social learning via teaching.
Given the relatively large number of OCM categories available as predictor variables (\(76\)) for evidence of teaching at the paragraph level and the relatively small sample size of ethnographic paragraphs in our sample (\(164\)), we employed the glmnet package112 to fit a penalized lasso regression model with Poisson errors. Penalized regression, such as lasso, is particularly useful when the number of predictors (\(p\)) is large relative to the number of cases (\(n\)) and is often recommended in such scenarios because it helps address multicollinearity and reduce the potential for overfitting through the introduction of a penalty term112,113. This modeling approach identifies the OCM code categories predictive of greater counts of evidence for teaching at the paragraph level. We used 10-fold cross-validation to determine the optimum value of the penalty term \(\lambda\), which minimized cross-validation error. Additionally, we employed a second \(\lambda\), representing the largest value of \(\lambda\) within one standard error of the minimum, to provide more conservative parameter estimates by increasing shrinkage. We report coefficients from both the optimal \({\lambda }_{{MIN}}\) model and the more conservative \({\lambda }_{1{-{\rm{SE}}}}\) model. All variables were centered and standardized by one standard deviation prior to fitting. For interpretation purposes, we categorized each OCM code group as either Instrumental or Opaque culture, although this categorization did not influence the model fitting process. OCM codes that are positive predictors reveal the subjects most associated with ethnographic descriptions of teaching, while negative predictors indicate subjects associated with other, non-teaching, processes of social learning.
Bayesian model with researcher-coded predictors
To better understand socio-ecological variation in teaching on a more fine-grained level we used Bayesian multi-level logistic regression to identify predictors of evidence for teaching (coded as \(1\)) compared to other forms of social learning (coded as \(0\)), at the instance level (there are eight instances of non-social learning in the HGSL data which we exclude from all analyses here). Bayesian models employed an index variable approach for categorical predictors114, including random effects for paragraph and document to account for multiple instances per paragraph and multiple paragraphs per document. The model was specified to capture the nonlinear associations between the predictors and the outcome. We compared model fit to an intercept-only model, as well as identical models that additionally included a random effect for either continental region or society, using the loo package for model selection (Table S3).
Models were fit using brms, which interfaces with Rstan to fit Bayesian models using Hamiltonian Markov Chain Monte Carlo. Markov chain convergence was assessed using standard diagnostics (number of effective samples, the Gelman-Rubin diagnostic, and visual inspection of trace plots). Following McElreath114, we compute the expected difference (contrasts) between levels within categorical predictors by subtracting the two posterior distributions (e.g., posterior \({\alpha }_{{\rm{domain}},1}\) \(-\) posterior \({\alpha }_{{\rm{domain}},2}\)) and interpret the 95% intervals of contrast distributions which do not include \(0\) as meaningful differences between levels within categorical predictors (reporting mean differences of two distributions, \(\varDelta \hat{\beta }\), and their lower and upper 95% credible intervals, LCI and UCI). The probability of an instance of social learning involving teaching was modeled as a function of four key predictors: learner age [Infancy, Early childhood, Childhood, Middle childhood, Adolescence, General], learner gender [Female, Male, Neutral], mode of instruction [Oblique, Vertical, Horizontal, Unknown], and cultural domain [Cultural values and kinship, Religiosity, Ecology, Misc. skills, Manufacturing, Subsistence]. The model was structured to allow for a direct estimation of the nonlinear effects of these categorical predictors on the log odds of teaching presence without including an intercept. This approach enables us to represent the contributions of each level of each predictor rather than defaulting to a baseline level of teaching presence. Specifically, the response variable is modeled as a Bernoulli distribution, where the probability parameter \({\mu }_{i}\) is defined as the sum of several indexed intercepts corresponding to the levels of each predictor and random effects associated with the document and text record identifiers, \({u}_{i}\) and \({v}_{i}\).
The multi-level Bayesian logistic regression modeling approach is described as:
The intercepts \({\alpha }_{{{domain}},j}\), \({\alpha }_{{{mode}},k}\), \({\alpha }_{{{age}},l}\), and \({\alpha }_{{{\rm{gender}}},m}\) correspond to the levels of the categorical predictors \({\rm{domain}}\), \({\rm{mode}}\), \({\rm{age}}\), and \({\rm{gender}}\), respectively, where each intercept follows a normal distribution with a mean of \(0\) and a standard deviation of \(1\), reflecting a neutral prior belief about the direction and magnitude of these effects and to impose a regularizing effect constraining parameter estimates114,115. For the standard deviations of the random intercepts for document and paragraph, we used exponential priors with a rate of \(1\), indicating an expectation for moderate variation across documents and paragraphs.
The model was estimated using Hamiltonian Monte Carlo (HMC) sampling, with four chains, each running for 4000 iterations, including a warm-up phase of 2000 iterations. To improve the convergence properties of the sampler and the accuracy of posterior estimates, we set the adapt_delta parameter to 0.99, aiming for a high acceptance rate of proposed samples. We conducted a prior sensitivity analysis, which showed that weaker priors provided better predictive performance; however, the alternative priors did not lead to different interpretations. Results are reported in Supplementary Note 4 and Supplementary Data 2.
We also fit a bias assessment model to investigate potential influences of meta-ethnographic bias, such as document publication date, the presence of a woman author, document page length, and continental region. All variables, excluding the presence of a woman author, were provided by eHRAF. We manually coded if each document included a woman author or co-author, drawing on previously published information when available18,116,117,118. Results revealed these potential sources of bias were conclusively not associated with evidence of teaching. We, therefore, do not include them in our predictor model. See Supplementary Note 5 for additional details.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Code availability
Code available at the Open Science Foundation.
References
Axelrod, R. An evolutionary approach to norms. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 80, 1095–1111 (1986).
Chudek, M. & Henrich, J. Culture-gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 218–226 (2011).
Gintis, H. Geneculture coevolution and the nature of human sociality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 878–888 (2011).
Raihani, N. The Social Instinct: How Cooperation Shaped the World. (Random House, 2021).
Nichols, R. et al. Cultural evolution: a review of theoretical challenges. Evolut. Human Sci. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2024.2 (2024).
Curtin, C. M. et al. Kinship intensity and the use of mental states in moral judgment across societies. Evolut. Human Behav. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.002 (2020).
Kline, M. A. How to learn about teaching: an evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behav. Brain Sci. 38, 1–71 (2015).
Kruger, A. C. & Tomasello, M. Cultural learning and learning culture. in The Handbook of Education and Human Development: New Models of Learning, Teaching and Schooling (eds Olson, D. R. & Torrance, N.) (Blackwell Publishing, 1996).
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J. & Hurtado, A. M. A theory of human life history evolution: diet intelligence, and longevity. Evolut. Anthropol. 9, 156–185 (2000).
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 1149–1157 (2011).
Boyette, A. H. & Hewlett, B. S. Teaching in Hunter-Gatherers. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 9, 771–797 (2018).
Hewlett, B. S. & Roulette, C. J. Teaching in hunter–gatherer infancy. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3, 150403 (2016).
Astuti, R. & Harris, P. L. Understanding mortality and the life of the ancestors in rural Madagascar. Cogn. Sci. 32, 713–740 (2008).
Harris, P. L. Children’s understanding of death: from biology to religion. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 373, 20170266 (2018).
House, B. R. et al. Universal norm psychology leads to societal diversity in prosocial behaviour and development. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 36–44 (2020).
Paus, T. Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 60–68 (2005).
Dumontheil, I. Development of abstract thinking during childhood and adolescence: The role of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 57–76 (2014).
Garfield, Z. H., Syme, K. L. & Hagen, E. H. Universal and variable leadership dimensions across human societies. Evolut. Hum. Behav. 41, 397–414 (2020).
Van Vugt, M. Evolutionary origins of leadership and followership. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 354–371 (2006).
von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H. & Stieglitz, J. Leadership in an Egalitarian Society. Hum. Nat. 25, 538–566 (2014).
Kantner, J. Identifying the pathways to permanent leadership. in The Evolution of Leadership: Transitions in Decision Making from Small-Scale to Middle-Range Societies (eds Vaughn, K. J., Eerkens, J. W. & Kanter, J.) 249–281 (SAR, 2010).
Garfield, Z. H., von Rueden, C. & Hagen, E. H. The evolutionary anthropology of political leadership. Leadersh. Q. 30, 59–80 (2019).
Mumford, E. The Origins of Leadership. (University of Chicago Press, 1909).
Lewis, H. S. Leaders and Followers: Some Anthropological Perspectives (Addison-Wesley, 1974).
von Rueden, C. & van Vugt, M. Leadership in small-scale societies: Some implications for theory, research, and practice. Leadersh. Q. 26, 978–990 (2015).
Henrich, J. & Gil-White, F. J. The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolut. Hum. Behav. 22, 165–196 (2001).
Garfield, Z. H. & Hagen, E. H. Investigating evolutionary models of leadership among recently settled Ethiopian hunter-gatherers. Spec. Issue Evolut. Biol. Leadersh. 31, 101290 (2020).
Koh, D., Lee, K. & Joshi, K. Transformational leadership and creativity: a meta-analytic review and identification of an integrated model. J. Organ. Behav. 40, 625–650 (2019).
Mesoudi, A. Cultural evolution and cultural psychology. in Handbook of cultural psychology 2nd edition (eds Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D.) 144–162 (Guilford Press, 2019).
Barry, H., Josephson, L., Lauer, E. & Marshall, C. Agents and techniques for child training: cross-cultural codes 6. Ethnology 16, 191–230 (1977).
Lightner, A. D., Heckelsmiller, C. & Hagen, E. H. Ethnoscientific expertise and knowledge specialisation in 55 traditional cultures. Evolut. Human Sci. 3, 1–28 (2021).
Charbonneau, M., Curioni, A., McEllin, L. & Strachan, J. W. A. Flexible cultural learning through action coordination. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916231182923 (2023).
Davis, H. E., Stieglitz, J., Kaplan, H., Tayo, A. M. & Gurven, M. The formal schooling niche: longitudinal evidence from Bolivian Amazon demonstrates that higher school quality augments differences in children’s abstract reasoning. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/d3sgq (2020).
Hewlett, B. S. & Hewlett, B. L. Ebola, Culture and Politics: The Anthropology of an Emerging Disease. (Cengage Learning, 2007).
Marini, M. Cultural evolution and economic growth: a theoretical hypothesis with some empirical evidence. J. Socio-Econ. 33, 765–784 (2004).
Henrich, J. The Weirdest People in the World: How the West Became Psychologically Peculiar and Particularly Prosperous. (Penguin UK, 2020).
Social inequality before farming? Multidisciplinary approaches to the study of social organization in prehistoric and ethnographic hunter-gatherer-fisher societies (2020).
Singh, M. & Glowacki, L. Human social organization during the Late Pleistocene: Beyond the Nomadic-egalitarian Model. https://doi.org/10.32942/osf.io/vusye (2021).
Legare, C. H., Wen, N. J., Herrmann, P. A. & Whitehouse, H. Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning. Cognition 142, 351–361 (2015).
Berl, R. E. & Hewlett, B. S. Overimitation in the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin. in vol. 153 77–77 (2014).
Bandura, A. Social learning through imitation. in Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1962 211–274 (Univer. Nebraska Press, 1962).
Galef Jr, B. G. & Heyes, C. Social learning and imitation. Learn. Behav. 32, 261–269 (2004).
Gurven, M. D., Davison, R. J. & Kraft, T. S. The optimal timing of teaching and learning across the life course. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 375, 20190500 (2020).
Lew-Levy, S. et al. Who teaches children to forage? Exploring the primacy of child-to-child teaching among Hadza and BaYaka Hunter-Gatherers of Tanzania and Congo. Evolut. Human Behav. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.07.003 (2019).
Thornton, A. & Raihani, N. J. The evolution of teaching. Anim. Behav. 75, 1823–1836 (2008).
Boyette, A. H. & Hewlett, B. S. Autonomy, equality, and teaching among Aka foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Congo Basin. Hum. Nat. 28, 289–322 (2017).
Gardner, P. M. Foragers’ pursuit of individual autonomy. Curr. Anthropol. 32, 543–572 (1991).
Kramer, K. L. Childhood Teaching and Learning among Savanna Pumé Hunter-Gatherers. Human Nat. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09392-x (2021).
Lew-Levy, S., Lavi, N., Reckin, R., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J. & Ellis-Davies, K. How do hunter-gatherer children learn social and gender norms? A meta-ethnographic review. Cross-Cult. Res. 52, 213–255 (2018).
Lee, R. B. & Daly, R. H. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Hunters and Gatherers. (Cambridge University Press, 1999).
Boehm, C. Egalitarian behavior and reverse dominance hierarchy. Curr. Anthropol. 34, 227 (1993).
Boehm, C. Hierarchy in the Forest: the Evolution of Egalitarian Behavior. (Harvard University Press Cambridge, MA, 1999).
Kaplan, H. S., Mueller, T., Gangestad, S. & Lancaster, J. B. Neural capital and life span evolution among primates and humans. in Brain and Longevity (eds Finch, C., Robine, J.-M. & Christen, Y.) 69–97 (Springer, 2003).
Garfield, Z. H., Hubbard, H., Robert & Hagen, E. H. Evolutionary models of leadership: Tests and synthesis. Hum. Nat. 30, 23–58 (2019).
Neel, J. V. On being headman. Perspect. Biol. Med. 23, 277–294 (1980).
Kaplan, H., Lancaster, J. & Robson, A. Embodied capital and the evolutionary economics of the human life span. Popul. Dev. Rev. 29, 152–182 (2003).
Garfield, Z. H. Correlates of conflict resolution across cultures. Evolut. Human Sci. 3, 1–16 (2021).
Glowacki, L. & von Rueden, C. Leadership solves collective action problems in small-scale societies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 370, 1–13 (2015).
Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H. & Hewlett, B. L. Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 366, 1168–1178 (2011).
Caro, T. M. & Hauser, M. D. Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Q. Rev. Biol. 67, 151–174 (1992).
Caldwell, C. A., Renner, E. & Atkinson, M. Human teaching and cumulative cultural evolution. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 9, 751–770 (2018).
Hitchcock, R. K. Hunters and gatherers past and present: Perspectives on diversity, teaching, and information transmission. Rev. Anthropol. 48, 5–37 (2019).
Garfield, Z. H., Garfield, M. J. & Hewlett, B. S. A cross-cultural analysis of hunter-gatherer social learning. in Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers (eds Hewlett, B. S. & Hideaki, T.) 19–34 (Springer, 2016).
Tehrani, J. J. & Riede, F. Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: Learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeol. https://doi.org/10.1080/00438240802261267 (2008).
Gweon, H. Inferential social learning: Cognitive foundations of human social learning and teaching. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 896–910 (2021).
Fogarty, L., Strimling, P. & Laland, K. N. The evolution of teaching. Int. J. Org. Evolut. 65, 2760–2770 (2011).
Castro, L. & Toro, M. A. Cumulative cultural evolution: The role of teaching. J. Theor. Biol. 347, 74–83 (2014).
Dira, S. J. & Hewlett, B. S. Learning to spear hunt among Ethiopian Chabu adolescent hunter-gatherers. in Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers (eds Hewlett, B. S. & Hideaki, T.) 71–81 (Springer, 2016).
Lew-Levy, S. et al. Costly teaching contributes to the acquisition of spear hunting skill among BaYaka forager adolescents. Proc. R. Soc. B 289, 20220164 (2022).
Hewlett, B. Social learning and innovation in adolescence. Hum. Nat. 32, 239–278 (2021).
House, B. R. et al. Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 14586–14591 (2013).
Rindos, D. et al. Darwinian selection, symbolic variation, and the evolution of culture [and Comments and Reply]. Curr. Anthropol. 26, 65–88 (1985).
Sperber, D. Rethinking Symbolism. (CUP Archive, 1975).
André, J.-B., Baumard, N. & Boyer, P. Cultural evolution from the producers’ standpoint. Evolut. Hum. Sci. 5, e25 (2023).
Purzycki, B. G. et al. The cognitive and cultural foundations of moral behavior. Evolut. Hum. Behav. 39, 490–501 (2018).
Berkes, F., Colding, J. & Folke, C. Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. Ecol. Appl. 10, 1251–1262 (2000).
Boyette, A. H. Children’s play and culture learning in an egalitarian foraging society. Child Dev. 87, 759–769 (2016).
Lew-Levy, S., Milks, A., Kiabiya Ntamboudila, F., Broesch, T. & Kline, M. A. BaYaka adolescent boys nominate accessible adult men as preferred spear hunting models. Curr. Anthropol. https://doi.org/10.1086/716853 (2021).
Reyes-García, V., Gallois, S. & Demps, K. A multistage learning model for cultural transmission: evidence from three indigenous societies. in Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers: Evolutionary and Ethnographic Perspectives (eds Terashima, H. & Hewlett, B. S.) 47–60 (Springer Japan, 2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_4.
Salali, G. D. et al. Knowledge-sharing networks in hunter-gatherers and the evolution of cumulative culture. Curr. Biol. 26, 2516–2521 (2016).
Bateson, G. Steps to an Ecology of Mind Collected Essays in Anthropology, Psychiatry, Evolution, and Epistemology. (Chandler, 1972).
Lewis, J. Ekila: Blood, bodies, and egalitarian societies. J. R. Anthropol. Inst. 14, 297–315 (2008).
Garfield, Z. H., Garfield, M. & Hewlett, B. Zhgarfield/sociallearningdata: Update 1. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13692162 (2024).
Penniman, T. K. Arunta religion. Sociol. Rev. 21, 10–37 (1929).
Shostak, M. Nisa: The Life and Words of a !Kung Woman. (Harvard University Press, 1981).
Wiessner, P. The role of third parties in norm enforcement in customary courts among the Enga of Papua New Guinea. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 117, 32320–32328 (2020).
Innokentii, M. et al. Notes on the Islands of the Unalaska District. in (Russian-American Company, 1840).
Gusinde, M. Fireland Indians: Vol. 1. The Selk’nam, on the life and thought of a hunting people of the Great Island of Tierra del Fuego. in Expeditions 32, 1176, 51 plates (Verlag der Internationalen Zeitschrift, 1931).
von Rueden, C. R. Unmaking egalitarianism: Comparing sources of political change in an Amazonian society. Evolut. Human Behav. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.09.001 (2022).
Garfield, Z. H. & Hagen, E. Decision-making expertise, intelligence, and social alliances are associated with conflict resolution skills among recently settled Ethiopian hunter-gatherers (2024).
Barrett, H. C. The Search for Human Cognitive Specializations. in Evolutionary Neuroscience (ed Kaas, J. H.) 917–929 (Academic Press, 2020). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-820584-6.00039-8.
Hill, K., Barton, M. & Hurtado, A. M. The emergence of human uniqueness: Characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evolut. Anthropol. 18, 187–200 (2009).
Tomasello, M. Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. (Harvard University Press, 2019).
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. On the universality of human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: the role of genetics and adaptation. J. Personal. 58, 17–67 (1990).
Van Vugt, M., Hogan, R. & Kaiser, R. B. Leadership, followership, and evolution: Some lessons from the past. Am. Psychol. 63, 182–196 (2008).
Cheng, J. T. Dominance, prestige, and the role of leveling in human social hierarchy and equality. Curr. Opin. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.10.004 (2019).
Jiménez, Á. V. & Mesoudi, A. Prestige-biased social learning: Current evidence and outstanding questions. Palgrave Commun. 5, 20 (2019).
Henrich, J., Chudek, M. & Boyd, R. The Big Man Mechanism: how prestige fosters cooperation and creates prosocial leaders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 370, 20150013 (2015).
Cofnas, N. Power in cultural evolution and the spread of prosocial norms. Q. Rev. Biol. 93, 297–318 (2018).
Hagen, E. H. & Garfield, Z. Leadership and Prestige, Mothering, Sexual Selection, and Encephalization: The Computational Services Model. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/9bcdk (2019).
Richerson, P. et al. Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behav. Brain Sci. 39, 1–68 (2016).
Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K. & Hurtado, M. ‘It’s a wonderful life’: Signaling generosity among the Ache of Paraguay. Evolut. Hum. Behav. 21, 263–282 (2000).
Price, M. E. & Van Vugt, M. The evolution of leader-follower reciprocity: The theory of service-for-prestige. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–17 (2014).
Wiessner, P. Hunting, healing, and hxaro exchange: A long-term perspective on !Kung (Ju/’hoansi) large-game hunting. Evolut. Hum. Behav. 23, 407–436 (2002).
Hagen, E. H., Garfield, Z. H. & Lightner, A. D. Headmen, shamans, and mothers: Natural and sexual selection for computational services. Evolut. Hum. Behav. 46, 106651 (2025).
Boehm, C. Purposive social selection and the evolution of human altruism. Cross-Cult. Res. 42, 319–354 (2008).
Minkov, M. Cross-Cultural Analysis: the Science and Art of Comparing the World’s Modern Societies and Their Cultures. (SAGE, 2013).
Hewlett, B. S., Boyette, A. H., Lew-Levy, S., Gallois, S. & Dira, S. J. Cultural transmission among hunter-gatherers. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 121, e2322883121 (2024).
Schulz, J. F., Bahrami-Rad, D., Beauchamp, J. P. & Henrich, J. The Church, intensive kinship, and global psychological variation. Science 366, eaau5141 (2019).
Castro, L., Castro-Nogueira, M. Á., Villarroel, M. & Toro, M. Á. Assessor teaching and the evolution of human morality. Biol. Theory 16, 5–15 (2021).
Kelly, R. L. The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter-Gatherer Lifeways. (Smithsonian Inst Pr, 2013).
Friedman, J. H., Hastie, T. & Tibshirani, R. Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. J. Stat. Softw. 33, 1–22 (2010).
Zou, H. & Hastie, T. Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. J. R. Stat. Soc. 67, 301–320 (2005).
McElreath, R. Statistical Rethinking: A Bayesian Course with Examples in R and Stan. (Chapman and Hall/CRC, 2020). https://doi.org/10.1201/9780429029608.
Gelman, A. et al. Bayesian Data Analysis. (Chapman and Hall/CRC, 2015). https://doi.org/10.1201/b16018.
Post, E. R. & Macfarlan, S. J. Tracking cross-cultural gender bias in reputations. Cross-Cult. Res. 54, 346–363 (2020).
Garfield, Z. H. et al. The content and structure of reputation domains across human societies: A view from the evolutionary social sciences. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200296 (2021).
Mehr, S. A. et al. Universality and diversity in human song. Science 366, 1–17 (2019).
Acknowledgements
Jorge Peña provided extensive comments on earlier drafts of this paper and made valuable contributions to developing the ideas presented. Barry Hewlett also provided very useful comments, which improved the paper.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Z.H.G. initiated the project, designed the analytical framework, conducted the analyses, was primarily responsible for interpreting the results, and produced the first draft of the paper. S.L.L. provided extensive review and feedback on the paper and contributed to developing the core ideas and hypotheses.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Alexander Bolyanatz, Joshua Conrad Jackson and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Garfield, Z.H., Lew-Levy, S. Teaching is associated with the transmission of opaque culture and leadership across 23 egalitarian hunter-gatherer societies. Nat Commun 16, 3387 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-025-58764-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-025-58764-9