Abstract
Cognitive deficits and overweight are prominent challenges in the treatment of psychosis, which have a direct impact on patients’ quality of life. We aim to determine whether there is an association of overweight with cognitive performance and whether there are sex differences in this association. We included 170 individuals with first-episode psychosis (FEP) (mean age 23.08 years, 32.9% females) attending an early intervention service who underwent clinical, biometric, and cognitive assessments by the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery. A set of two-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were conducted for each cognitive test. Sex, overweight, and their interaction were included as factors. Nearly 34% of the participants were overweight without differences between males and females. The excess of weight did not exert any main effect on cognition; however, overweight females performed significantly worse than non-overweight females in processing speed, verbal learning and memory, reasoning and problem-solving, and global cognitive function, whereas in males, there were no differences. Our findings highlight that sex matters in the study of metabolic and cognitive factors in FEP to develop targeted interventions based on sex perspectives.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
It is well established that individuals with schizophrenia (SCZ) have increased rates of overweight and other metabolic syndrome components compared to healthy individuals1,2. Despite treatment with antipsychotics (AP) having a direct impact on weight gain3,4, increased cardiovascular risk and metabolic abnormalities have also been observed in individuals at the early stages of psychosis and in AP-naive individuals5,6,7,8, which suggests an intrinsic vulnerability even before treatment. A greater prevalence of both overweight and obesity has been observed in females with established SCZ compared with males9,10. In contrast, there is a lack of conclusive research conducted on first-episode psychosis (FEP) patients. Two studies with AP-naive subjects7,11, reported that females were more likely to be overweight than males, whereas other12 did not find differences.
Cognitive impairment is a core feature of psychosis, and the most commonly affected domains are working memory, processing speed, motor and verbal learning, and perception13,14. Studies aiming to investigate sex differences in cognitive performance in both established SCZ and FEP samples have yielded conflicting results15,16. Some studies have indicated that females perform better than males in terms of verbal and visual memory, processing speed, and social cognition17,18,19. Conversely, other studies have reported that males perform better than females in terms of attention20, processing speed21, and visual memory22. Additionally, others have not identified sex differences23,24,25 in cognitive functioning.
Overweight and obesity have been linked to an increased risk of cognitive impairment and dementia26,27,28,29. In addition, an inverse association between BMI and cognitive function has been documented in both obese and community-based samples26,27,28,29. Nevertheless, despite the high prevalence of overweight and cognitive dysfunction in patients with SCZ, the association between these two factors has been understudied. The limited number of studies on patients with established SCZ suggest that overweight is associated with reduced psychomotor abilities, decreased processing speed, and poorer performance in executive function and visual learning and memory30,31. However, less evidence exists for FEP patients. A previous study conducted by our group revealed a significant association between overweight and executive function in FEP patients, especially in inhibitory control32. Furthermore, another study suggested a negative correlation between BMI and intelligence quotient scores33, without significant associations observed with any other specific cognitive domain. Nevertheless, only two studies have considered sex differences in established SCZ patients, reporting a positive association between BMI and language scores in males but no relationship in females10,34. In addition, a recent study identified sex-specific associations between metabolic risk factors and cognitive impairment in a sample of individuals with SCZ. Specifically, dyslipidemia and hyperlipidemia were inversely correlated with performance on several cognitive domains in females and males, respectively35.
To the best of our knowledge, no other study has investigated the association of overweight and sex with cognition in FEP patients. Given this knowledge gap, we aimed to determine (1) whether there is an association between overweight and cognitive performance, and (2) whether this association is different in females than males.
Results
Sample characteristics
Table 1 shows the sample characteristics. The sample consisted of 170 FEP patients (56 females and 114 males), and the mean age was 23.05 years. The mean BMI of the whole sample was 23.77 kg/m2; 33.53% of the participants were overweight (n = 57), 24.6% of whom were obese (n = 14). Almost the whole sample was receiving AP treatment (94.1%), and the median treatment duration was 88 days. No differences were found in the prevalence of overweight between males and females (Table 1). When groups were compared by weight status, the only significant difference was that overweight participants had a median AP treatment duration of 125 days, whereas non-overweight participants had a median treatment duration of 67 days (U = 4272.50, p < 0.0005). When groups were compared by sex, females had greater CDS scores than males (U = 3898.5, p = 0.0149), whereas males were treated with a significantly greater AP dose than females (U = 2600.50, p = 0.0489).
Associations between sex and overweight status, and cognitive performance
There were no significant differences between groups regarding weight status in any of the cognitive test scores (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 2). However, when comparing cognitive scores between females and males, females exhibited significantly higher scores in BACS-SC [t = −2.656 (95% CI −0.736 to −0.108), p = 0.0087], HVLT-R [t = −3.551 (95% CI −0.863 to −0.246), p = 0.0005] and MSCEIT-ME [t = −2.807 (95% CI −0.758 to −0.132), p = 0.0056] subtests than males (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 2).
TMT-A Trail-Making Test Part A, BACS-SC Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia-Symbol Coding, HVLT-R Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, WMSIII-SS Weschler Memory Scale III-Spatial Span, LNS Letter-Number Sequencing, NAB-Mazes Neuropsychological Assessment Battery-Mazes, BVMT-R Brief Visual Memory Test-Revised, Fluency Verbal Fluency Test, MSCEIT-ME Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test-Managing Emotions, CPT-IP Continuous Performance Test-Identical Pairs.
*p-value < 0.05, **p-value < 0.001. TMT-A Trail-Making Test Part A, BACS-SC Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia-symbol Coding, HVLT-R Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-revised, WMSIII-SS Weschler Memory Scale III-Spatial Span, LNS Letter-number Sequencing, NAB-Mazes Neuropsychological Assessment Battery-mazes, BVMT-R Brief Visual Memory Test-revised, fluency Verbal Fluency Test, MSCEIT-ME Mayer-salovey-caruso Emotional Intelligence Test-managing Emotions, CPT-IP Continuous Performance Test-identical Pairs.
Two-way ANCOVAs were performed for each cognitive test, adjusting for the following variables showing a p < 0.1 in the univariate analysis: educational level (years), CDS score, AP CPZE, treatment duration, and frequency of cannabis and alcohol use. The analyses (Table 2) revealed a significant main effect of overweight status on HVLT-R, and a significant main effect of sex on BACS-SC, HVLT-R, and MSCEIT-ME scores. There was a significant interaction effect between overweight status and sex on the BACS-SC (F = 5.672, p = 0.018), HVLT-R (F = 12.471, p < 0.001), and NABMazes (F = 8.751, p = 0.004). Additionally, a significant interaction effect was found for the global cognition z-score (F = 8.555, p = 0.004). Figure 3 shows the marginal mean analyses from the ANCOVAs revealing the direction of the interaction effects across the statistically significant tests. In all cases, being overweight was associated with lower scores than being non-overweight in females. However, no differences in cognitive scores were found between overweight and non-overweight males.
Partial correlations were conducted using BMI as a continuous variable controlled by education level, CDS score, AP CPZE, treatment duration, and frequency of cannabis and alcohol use in the whole sample and stratifying by sex groups. Results show a negative correlation between BMI and HVLT-R z-score (ρ = −0.390, p = 0.006) only in females, with no other significant correlations. Further correlation results are shown in the Supplementary Table 3.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study to report sex differences in the association of overweight and cognitive performance in a sample of FEP patients. Our main finding is that non-overweight females significantly outperformed overweight females in terms of processing speed, verbal learning and memory, reasoning and problem-solving, and global cognitive function, whereas these differences were not found in males. Given the lack of previous studies, our findings should be considered preliminary and need replication.
The prevalence of overweight within our sample (33.53%) aligns with findings from previous studies with FEP patient samples36,37,38. The median AP treatment duration of the sample was less than 90 days; however, overweight participants had received AP treatment for approximately 4 months, and non-overweight participants had received AP treatment for approximately 2 months, reflecting the rapid weight gain associated with AP use at the beginning of treatment3,4. In line with the findings of Correll et al. 12, we did not find differences in the rate of overweight between males and females, and similar to previous studies39,40, we found more severe symptoms of depression in females and a higher AP dosage in males15,41.
We did not find an independent effect of overweight on cognitive performance, thus contrasting with prior studies of individuals with established SCZ30,31, but our results are in agreement with the scarce previous research on FEP patients32. Notably, research to date has also reported inconsistent results regarding the direction of the association between overweight/obesity and cognitive performance in the general population42,43,44, as well as in established SCZ populations45,46,47,48. In fact, the diversity in methodological approaches, sample features, and cognitive tests limits the comparability of these studies30. As mentioned earlier, the most consistent finding within obese individuals and the general population is that deficits in executive functioning may affect the regulation of eating behavior28,49. Thus, the extent to which excess weight influences cognitive performance and whether specific cognitive domains, such as executive dysfunction, play a role in weight gain in individuals with psychosis spectrum disorders must still be determined.
Concerning sex differences in cognitive performance, we observed that females outperformed males in terms of processing speed, verbal learning and memory, and social cognition. These findings are in line with an earlier study on an FEP sample by Ferrer-Quintero et al. 17,19, who highlighted enhanced verbal abilities and social cognition in females, especially in emotional regulation50 (assessed by the MSCEIT-ME). As discussed in the aforementioned study, the objective measurements of social cognition are not always accurate, as it is difficult to assess the behavioral or emotional response in a fictional social situation. Meaning that many factors could mediate this association (e.g., verbal abilities to comprehend the social task). However, the observed differences in processing speed require further investigation, as the results remain inconclusive, with previous research showing opposite or incongruent results in FEP and established SCZ patients16,22. Notably, in the general population, there seems to be a consensus that females score better than males in verbal learning tasks, and males outperform females in spatial memory tasks51. Researchers have attempted to explain sex differences in cognitive performance, and one of the most accepted theories is that males and females use different brain networks in cognitive processing pathways. Moreover, these pathways are influenced by the effects of sex hormones, which can lead to differences in cognitive performance52. Similarly, the role of the hippocampus in cognitive processes, together with the prefrontal cortex53, and its sensitivity to sex hormones could contribute to these changes and differences51,52. Researchers have attempted to explain sex differences in cognitive performance, and one of the most accepted theories is that males and females use different brain networks in cognitive processing pathways. Moreover, these pathways are influenced by the effects of sex hormones, which can lead to differences in cognitive performance52. Similarly, the role of the hippocampus in cognitive processes, together with the prefrontal cortex53, and its sensitivity to sex hormones could contribute to these changes and differences51,52.
In addition, males and females also show different mechanisms for metabolic and endocrine regulation54,55. Leptin, for instance, is a hormone known to be involved in eating behavior and metabolism regulation and provides feedback between the central nervous system and fat cells. Leptin’s role is linked to metabolic syndrome, as increased blood leptin levels have been associated with the development of certain conditions associated with metabolic syndrome56. In fact, a previous study by our group57 revealed an association between increased blood leptin levels and a higher BMI. Interestingly, a correlation between blood leptin levels and food-craving behavior was found only in females. Notably, food-craving behavior is linked to cognitive performance through inhibitory control and prefrontal cognitive processes such as executive functioning49,58. Furthermore, a meta-analysis suggested that blood leptin levels are inversely correlated with the activation of the prefrontal cortex59, and a reduction in the activity of the prefrontal cortex could lead to a lack of cognitive control and executive function impairment60. In the case of prolactin levels, which are also upregulated in AP-naive early-stage psychosis patients61, a previous study on the part of our sample showed a direct association between prolactin levels and cognitive performance only in male participants62. However, research has revealed no associations between prolactin levels and BMI or overweight in early-stage psychosis patients63,64; therefore, even though associations among prolactin levels, overweight status, sex, and cognition have not yet been identified, there is more than sufficient evidence of a link between hormone levels and cognitive performance.
While discussing our results, we must acknowledge several limitations of our study. On the one hand, stratification by sex and BMI limited subgroup size. Second, our sample was not AP-naive, and we could not assess the effect of each AP due to the sample size; however, we minimized its impact by controlling for AP dosage and treatment duration. We used BMI as an indicator of overweight. However, there are other biometric variables that we did not include to define metabolic conditions65,66. The absence of a healthy control group did not allow us to determine whether the observed cognitive differences were not due to sex-related biological variations. Finally, the cross-sectional nature of the study did not allow us to infer causality.
Despite these limitations, our results suggest the presence of an interaction effect between sex and overweight on cognitive performance in FEP patients, in which the presence of overweight is associated with a worse performance in several domains in females; conversely, these effects were not observed in males. Given the lack of previous studies, our findings should be considered preliminary and need replication. We can speculate that metabolic alterations, together with the action of hormones, may exert differential effects on cognitive processes in males and females. However, as previously mentioned, the inverse association cannot be excluded. Biology of males and females differ due to genetic, neurochemical, hormonal, metabolic, and anatomic factors that are present since conception and evolve throughout neurodevelopment: childhood, adolescence, and adulthood67. Considering this complex picture, it is still unknown the mechanism underlying the present findings. However, our findings highlight that sex matters and that differences cannot be ignored in the study of metabolic and cognitive factors in the psychosis field. These findings, therefore, require replication in other samples, and further research on metabolic issues is needed to contribute to the development of specific clinical strategies to improve patients’ quality of life.
Methods
Participants
For the present study, we selected 170 FEP attending the Early Psychosis Intervention Program (EIP) at the Hospital Universitari Institut Pere Mata (Reus, Catalonia, Spain) with complete neuropsychological, clinical, and biometric data. First episode of psychoses (FEP) was defined as the onset of a full psychotic disorder according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV)68 confirmed by the Spanish version of the Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I). The exclusion criteria were as follows: more than 12 months of illness duration, more than 12 months of AP treatment, psychosis induced by substances or other medical conditions, intellectual disability, severe head injury, and the inability to understand or speak Spanish or Catalan fluently.
Ethical approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee of the Pere Virgili Research Institute (IISPV). After a complete description of the study was given to the participants, written informed consent was obtained.
Procedure
All participants were assessed when they were clinically stable enough to understand and sign the informed consent and to undergo the cognitive assessment. At study entry, participants underwent biometric, clinical, and cognitive assessments.
Biometric assessments
The biometric weight and height examinations were performed by trained nurses. According to general and World Health Organization criteria, a BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2 was considered overweight, and a BMI < 25 kg/m2 was considered non-overweight65.
Clinical assessments
Sociodemographic, clinical, and treatment-related information was obtained through direct interviews by trained psychiatrists. Sociodemographic data included sex, age, educational level, and ethnicity. The clinical assessments included the Spanish versions of the Positive and Negative Symptoms Scale (PANSS)69 to assess the severity of psychotic symptoms (higher scores indicating greater severity), the Calgary Depression Scale (CDS)70 to assess the severity of depressive symptoms (higher scores reflecting more severity) and the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF)68 to evaluate general functioning (higher scores meaning better functioning). Information on the duration of untreated psychosis (DUP), variables regarding AP treatment (main AP, treatment length, and AP dosage), and substance use was collected through direct interviews. Each AP dosage was transformed into chlorpromazine equivalents (CPZE) in mg/day71, and substance use was categorized as frequent use ( ≥1 time/week) or nonfrequent use ( <1 time/week).
Cognitive assessments
Cognitive tests were administered by experienced psychologists, and all participants completed the tests in a fixed order. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB)72,73 was administered to the participants. The MCCB is a neuropsychological battery comprising ten tests that measure seven cognitive domains, including the Trail-Making Test Part A (TMT-A), the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia-Symbol Coding (BACS-SC) subtest, and the Category Fluency Test-Animal Naming (Fluency) to measure processing speed; the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R) to measure verbal learning and memory; the Weschler Memory Scale III-Spatial Span (WMSIII-SS) and the Letter-Number Span (LNS) subtests to measure working memory; the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery-Mazes (NAB-Mazes) subtest to measure reasoning and problem-solving; the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R) to measure visual learning and memory; the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test-Managing Emotions (MSCEIT-ME) subtest to measure social cognition (specifically emotional regulation); and the Continuous Performance Test-Identical Pairs (CPT-IP) to measure attention and vigilance.
The z-scores of the MCCB subtests were determined by obtaining the direct score mean and standard deviation from a database with results from a sample of 58 healthy controls who had participated in previous studies74,75. Higher z-scores represented better performance; thus, the z-score for the TMT-A subtest needed to be inverted. A global cognition z-score was derived from the means of all the cognitive tests.
The control sample comprised both friends and relatives who were not genetically related to the patients. To determine their status as healthy controls, we conducted screenings for previous and current psychiatric disorders through the General Health Questionnaire76. Sociodemographic characteristics of the HC compared with the FEP sample are presented in Supplementary Table 1.
Statistical analysis
Differences in demographic, biometric, and cognitive data were first compared separately regarding sex (female vs. male) or weight status (overweight vs. non-overweight). The normality of the distribution of variables was checked by the Kolmogorov‒Smirnov test. The dependent variables were normalized through the rank-based inverse normal transformation (RankNorm function from the RNOmni R package, version 1.0.1.2) in RStudio (version 2023.5.0) to ensure the ANCOVA assumptions were met by all the variables. Two-sided Student’s t-tests and Mann‒Whitney U-tests were used to compare parametric and nonparametric continuous variables, respectively, and the Chi-squared test was used to compare categorical variables.
A two-way univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted for each cognitive test. Sex (female and male), overweight status (overweight and non-overweight), and their interaction were included as factors. Each analysis was adjusted for variables displaying intergroup differences at a level of significance p < 0.1. Multiple testing correction was applied through the Bonferroni method. Given that 11 ANCOVAs were performed, only models with p < 0.0045 were considered to be statistically significant (0.05/11 = 0.0045).
Additionally, partial correlations using BMI as a continuous variable were carried out for exploratory purposes.
All the analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 25.
Data availability
The dataset for this manuscript is not publicly available due to issues of data ownership and participant consent.
References
Abdul Rashid, N. A. et al. Unraveling the relationship between obesity, schizophrenia and cognition. Schizophr. Res. 151, 107–112 (2013).
Mitchell, A. J., Vancampfort, D., De Herdt, A., Yu, W. & De Hert, M. Is the prevalence of metabolic syndrome and metabolic abnormalities increased in early schizophrenia? A comparative meta-analysis of first episode, untreated and treated patients. Schizophr. Bull. 39, 295–305 (2013).
Misiak, B., Stańczykiewicz, B., Łaczmański, Ł. & Frydecka, D. Lipid profile disturbances in antipsychotic-naive patients with first-episode non-affective psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 190, 18–27 (2017).
Wu, H. et al. Antipsychotic-induced weight gain: dose-response meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Schizophr. Bull. 48, 643–654 (2022).
Alonso, Y. et al. Risk factors for metabolic syndrome in individuals with recent-onset psychosis at disease onset and after 1-year follow-up. Sci. Rep. 12, 1–11 (2022).
Cordes, J. et al. Prevalence of metabolic syndrome in female and male patients at risk of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 181, 38–42 (2017).
García-Rizo, C. et al. Prolactin, metabolic and immune parameters in naive subjects with a first episode of psychosis. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 110, 110332 (2021).
Gallart-Palau, X. et al. Gradual increase in inflammation-linked glycoproteins and a proatherogenic lipoprotein profile in the early stages of psychosis as characterized by 1 H NMR blood analysis. J. Proteome Res. 22, 2271–2280 (2023).
Li, W., Lin, S., Yue, L., Fang, Y. & Xiao, S. Sex differences in obesity and cognitive function in Chinese elderly patients with chronic schizophrenia. Front. Endocrinol. 13, 742474 (2022).
Wei, C. W., Chen, Y. Q., Ma, M., Xiu, M. H. & Zhang, X. Y. Sex differences in the association of body mass index with symptoms and cognitive deficits in Chinese patients with chronic schizophrenia. Transl. Psychiatry 10, 18 (2020).
Zhou, Y. et al. Sex differences in metabolic disorder patterns of first-episode drug-naive patients with schizophrenia. Psychoneuroendocrinology 124, 105061 (2021).
Correll, C. U. et al. Cardiometabolic risk in patients with first-episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 1350 (2014).
Nuechterlein, K. H., Ventura, J., Subotnik, K. L. & Bartzokis, G. The early longitudinal course of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. J. Clin. Psychiatry 75, 25–29 (2014).
Schaefer, J., Giangrande, E., Weinberger, D. R. & Dickinson, D. The global cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: Consistent over decades and around the world. Schizophr. Res. 150, 42–50 (2013).
Ochoa, S., Usall, J., Cobo, J., Labad, X. & Kulkarni, J. Gender differences in schizophrenia and first-episode psychosis: a comprehensive literature review. Schizophr. Res. Treat. 2012, 1–9 (2012).
Freeman, H. B. & Lee, J. in Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. Vol. 63, 463–474 (NLM (Medline), 2023).
Ferrer-Quintero, M. et al. The effect of sex on social cognition and functioning in schizophrenia. NPJ Schizophr. 7, 57 (2021).
Zhang, B. et al. Gender differences measured by the MATRICS consensus cognitive battery in chronic schizophrenia patients. Sci. Rep. 7, 11821 (2017).
Ferrer-Quintero, M. et al. Males and females with first episode psychosis present distinct profiles of social cognition and metacognition. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 272, 1169–1181 (2022).
Mu, L. et al. Sex differences in association between clinical correlates and cognitive impairment in patients with chronic schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 131, 194–202 (2020).
Ittig, S. et al. Sex differences in cognitive functioning in at-risk mental state for psychosis, first episode psychosis and healthy control subjects. Eur. Psychiatry 30, 242–250 (2015).
Ayesa-Arriola, R. et al. No sex differences in neuropsychological performance in first episode psychosis patients. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 149–154 (2014).
Bozikas, V. P. et al. Sex differences in neuropsychological functioning among schizophrenia patients. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 44, 333–341 (2010).
Lee, J. et al. The effects of age and sex on cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: Findings from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) study. PLoS ONE 15, e0232855 (2020).
Vila‐Badia, R. et al. Cognitive functioning in first episode psychosis. Gender differences and relation with clinical variables. Early Inter. Psychiatry 15, 1667–1676 (2021).
Gunstad, J., Lhotsky, A., Wendell, C. R., Ferrucci, L. & Zonderman, A. B. Longitudinal examination of obesity and cognitive function: results from the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Neuroepidemiology 34, 222–229 (2010).
Lokken, K. L., Boeka, A. G., Austin, H. M., Gunstad, J. & Harmon, C. M. Evidence of executive dysfunction in extremely obese adolescents: a pilot study. Surg. Obes. Relat. Dis. 5, 547–552 (2009).
Gunstad, J. et al. Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr. Psychiatry 48, 57–61 (2007).
Mallorquí-Bagué, N. et al. Type 2 diabetes and cognitive impairment in an older population with overweight or obesity and metabolic syndrome: baseline cross-sectional analysis of the PREDIMED-plus study. Sci. Rep. 8, 16128 (2018).
Bora, E., Akdede, B. B. & Alptekin, K. The relationship between cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and metabolic syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 47, 1030–1040 (2017).
Hagi, K. et al. Association between cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 78, 510–518 (2021).
Sanchez-Gistau, V. et al. Relationship between ANKK1 rs1800497 polymorphism, overweight and executive dysfunction in early psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 209, 278–280 (2019).
Whitson, S. et al. Cognitive ability and metabolic physical health in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr. Res Cogn. 24, 100194 (2021).
Yang, M. et al. Sexual dimorphism in the relationship between Forkhead-Box P2 and BMI with cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14, 920352 (2022).
Huang, H. et al. Association between metabolic risk factors and cognitive impairment in schizophrenia based on sex. Psychiatry Investig. 20, 930–939 (2023).
Kolenic, M. et al. Obesity, dyslipidemia and brain age in first-episode psychosis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 99, 151–158 (2018).
McWhinney, S. et al. Obesity as a risk factor for accelerated brain ageing in first-episode psychosis—a longitudinal study. Schizophr. Bull. 47, 1772–1781 (2021).
Muntané, G. et al. Polygenic risk scores enhance prediction of body mass index increase in individuals with a first episode of psychosis. Eur. Psychiatry 66, e28 (2023).
Hong, S. I., Bennett, D. & Rosenheck, R. A. Gender differences in outcomes of early intervention services for first episode psychosis. Early Inter. Psychiatry 17, 715–723 (2023).
Irving, J. et al. Gender differences in clinical presentation and illicit substance use during first episode psychosis: a natural language processing, electronic case register study. BMJ Open 11, e042949 (2021).
Arranz, S. et al. The impact of sex and cannabis on clinical features in first–admitted patients with psychosis. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 36, 235–243 (2020).
Dye, L., Boyle, N. B., Champ, C. & Lawton, C. The relationship between obesity and cognitive health and decline. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 76, 443–454 (2017).
Elias, M. F., Elias, P. K., Sullivan, L. M., Wolf, P. A. & D’Agostino, R. B. Lower cognitive function in the presence of obesity and hypertension: The Framingham heart study. Int. J. Obes. 27, 260–268 (2003).
Kendig, M. D., Leigh, S. J. & Morris, M. J. Unravelling the impacts of western-style diets on brain, gut microbiota and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 233–243 (2021).
Depp, C. A. et al. Association of obesity and treated hypertension and diabetes with cognitive ability in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord. 16, 422–431 (2014).
Friedman, J. I. et al. The effects of hypertension and body mass index on cognition in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 1232–1239 (2010).
Guo, X. et al. The relationship between obesity and neurocognitive function in Chinese patients with schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry 13, 109 (2013).
Wysokiński, A., Dzienniak, M. & Kłoszewska, I. Effect of metabolic abnormalities on cognitive performance and clinical symptoms in schizophrenia. Arch. Psychiatry Psychother. 15, 13–25 (2013).
Lavagnino, L., Arnone, D., Cao, B., Soares, J. C. & Selvaraj, S. Inhibitory control in obesity and binge eating disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 714–726 (2016).
Cabello, R., Sorrel, M. A., Fernández-Pinto, I., Extremera, N. & Fernández-Berrocal, P. Age and gender differences in ability emotional intelligence in adults: a cross-sectional study. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1486–1492 (2016).
Lee, B. H., Richard, J. E., de Leon, R. G., Yagi, S. & Galea, L. A. M. Sex differences in cognition across aging. in sex differences in brain function and dysfunction. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 62, 235–284 (2022).
Bowman, R., Frankfurt, M. & Luine, V. Sex differences in cognition following variations in endocrine status. Learn. Mem. 29, 234–245 (2022).
Lee, I. & Byeon, J. S. Learning-dependent changes in the neuronal correlates of response inhibition in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Exp. Neurobiol. 23, 178–189 (2014).
Gerdts, E. & Regitz-Zagrosek, V. Sex differences in cardiometabolic disorders. Nat. Med. 25, 1657–1666 (2019).
Ventura-Clapier, R. et al. Sex in basic research: concepts in the cardiovascular field. Cardiovasc. Res. 113, 711–724 (2017).
Dong, M. & Ren, J. What fans the fire: insights into mechanisms of leptin in metabolic syndrome-associated heart diseases. Curr. Pharm. Des. 20, 652–658 (2014).
Martorell, L. et al. Increased levels of serum leptin in the early stages of psychosis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 111, 24–29 (2019).
Macaluso, F. et al. Body mass index and leptin are related to cognitive performance over 10 years in women with and without HIV infection. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 107, e1126–e1135 (2022).
Zanchi, D. et al. The impact of gut hormones on the neural circuit of appetite and satiety: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 457–475 (2017).
Friedman, N. P. & Robbins, T. W. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 72–89 (2022).
Petruzzelli, M. G. et al. Hyperprolactinemia and insulin resistance in drug naive patients with early onset first episode psychosis. BMC Psychiatry 18, 246 (2018).
Montalvo, I. et al. Sex differences in the relationship between prolactin levels and impaired processing speed in early psychosis. Aust. N.Z. J. Psychiatry 52, 585–595 (2018).
Vochoskova, K. et al. Weight and metabolic changes in early psychosis―association with daily quantification of medication exposure during the first hospitalization. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 148, 265–276 (2023).
González-Blanco, L. et al. Prolactin concentrations in antipsychotic-naïve patients with schizophrenia and related disorders: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 174, 156–160 (2016).
World Health Organization. Obesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic: report of a WHO consultation. World Health Organization https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/42330 (2000).
Müller, M. J., Braun, W., Enderle, J. & Bosy-Westphal, A. Beyond BMI: conceptual issues related to overweight and obese patients. Obes. Facts 9, 193–205 (2016).
Szadvári, I., Ostatníková, D. & Babková Durdiaková, J. Sex differences matter: males and females are equal but not the same. Physiol. Behav. 259, 114038 (2023).
American Psychiatric Association. DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. (APA, Washington, 1994).
Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A. & Opler, L. A. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13, 261–276 (1987).
Sarró, S. Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of the Spanish version of the Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 68, 349–356 (2004).
Gardner, D. M., Murphy, A. L., O’Donnell, H., Centorrino, F. & Baldessarini, R. J. International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 686–693 (2010).
Nuechterlein, K. H. et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 203–213 (2008).
Kern, R. S. et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 2: co-norming and standardization. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 214–220 (2008).
Sanchez-Gistau, V. et al. Clinical and cognitive correlates of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in first-episode psychosis: a controlled study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 36, 90–99 (2020).
Arranz, S. et al. The relationship between the level of exposure to stress factors and cannabis in recent onset psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 201, 352–359 (2018).
Goldberg, D. P. & Hillier, V. F. A scaled version of the general health questionnaire. Psychol. Med. 9, 139–145 (1979).
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to acknowledge the participants and families who took the time to participate in the research. This work was supported by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (grant numbers PI18/00514, PI21/00848, and PI21/00612) and co-funded by the European Union; the Generalitat de Catalunya (grant number SLT006/17/357); the IISPV-Cerca Program of the Generalitat de Catalunya; and co-funded by FEDER; the Catalan Agency of Research and Universities (AGAUR) (grant numbers 2017SGR-00444 and 2021SGR-01065); and the Universitat Rovira I Virgili-Martí Franquès program (grant number PMF2021-IN000681 to Martí Llaurador-Coll).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Martí Llaurador-Coll: conceptualization, methodology, data curation, software, formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review & editing, visualization. Ángel Cabezas: investigation, data curation. M. José Algora: investigation, data curation. Montse Solé: investigation, data curation. Elisabet Vilella: writing—review & editing, supervision, project administration, funding acquisition. Vanessa Sánchez-Gistau: conceptualization, methodology, investigation, data curation, writing—review & editing, visualization, supervision, project administration, funding acquisition.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Llaurador-Coll, M., Cabezas, Á., Algora, M. et al. Sex differences in the association of overweight with cognitive performance in individuals with first-episode psychosis. Schizophr 10, 95 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41537-024-00521-w
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41537-024-00521-w