Abstract
Why education is linked to higher cognitive function in aging is fiercely debated. Leading theories propose that education reduces brain decline in aging and enhances tolerance to brain pathology or that it does not affect cognitive decline but, rather, reflects higher early-life cognitive function. To test these theories, we analyzed 407,356 episodic memory scores from 170,795 participants older than 50 years, alongside 15,157 brain magnetic resonance imaging scans from 6,472 participants across 33 Western countries. More education was associated with better memory, larger intracranial volume and slightly larger volume of memory-sensitive brain regions. However, education did not protect against age-related decline or weakened effects of brain decline on cognition. The most parsimonious explanation for the results is that the associations reflect factors present early in life, including propensity of individuals with certain traits to pursue more education. Although education has numerous benefits, the notion that it provides protection against cognitive or brain decline is not supported.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$32.99 / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$259.00 per year
only $21.58 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout






Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability
Each dataset has different owners. Contact information to be used for data access is specified in Supplementary Table 3. Parts of the data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (https://adni.loni.usc.edu/). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in the analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at https://adni.loni.usc.edu/wp-content/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf. More information about the Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging (VETSA), including a list of VETSA investigators, is available at https://psychiatry.ucsd.edu/research/programscenters/vetsa/index.html.
References
GBD 2019 Dementia Forecasting Collaborators. Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Public Health 7, e105–e125 (2022).
Wolters, F. J. et al. Twenty-seven-year time trends in dementia incidence in Europe and the United States: the Alzheimer Cohorts Consortium. Neurology 95, e519–e531 (2020).
Chen, Y. et al. Dementia incidence trend in England and Wales, 2002–19, and projection for dementia burden to 2040: analysis of data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Lancet Public Health 8, e859–e867 (2023).
Gerstorf, D. et al. Today’s older adults are cognitively fitter than older adults were 20 years ago, but when and how they decline is no different than in the past. Psychol. Sci. 34, 22–34 (2023).
Livingston, G. et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2024 report of the Lancet standing Commission. Lancet 404, 572–628 (2024).
Suemoto, C. K. et al. Risk factors for dementia in Brazil: differences by region and race. Alzheimers Dement. 19, 1849–1857 (2023).
Lock, S. L., Chura, L. R., Dilworth-Anderson, P. & Peterson, J. Equity across the life course matters for brain health. Nat. Aging 3, 466–468 (2023).
Ritchie, H., Samborska, V., Ahuja, N., Ortiz-Ospina, E. & Roser, M. Global Education. Our World in Data https://ourworldindata.org/global-education (2023).
Opdebeeck, C., Martyr, A. & Clare, L. Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 23, 40–60 (2016).
Sepulcre, J. College education as a modulator of the aging brain. Nat. Aging 1, 980–981 (2021).
Cabeza, R. et al. Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 701–710 (2018).
Nyberg, L., Lovden, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U. & Backman, L. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 292–305 (2012).
Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M. et al. Association between educational attainment and amyloid deposition across the spectrum from normal cognition to dementia: neuroimaging evidence for protection and compensation. Neurobiol. Aging 59, 72–79 (2017).
Gazzina, S. et al. Education modulates brain maintenance in presymptomatic frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 90, 1124–1130 (2019).
Del Ser, T., Hachinski, V., Merskey, H. & Munoz, D. G. An autopsy-verified study of the effect of education on degenerative dementia. Brain 122, 2309–2319 (1999).
Nyberg, L. et al. Educational attainment does not influence brain aging. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2101644118 (2021).
Stern, Y., Barnes, C. A., Grady, C., Jones, R. N. & Raz, N. Brain reserve, cognitive reserve, compensation, and maintenance: operationalization, validity, and mechanisms of cognitive resilience. Neurobiol. Aging 83, 124–129 (2019).
Stern, Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460 (2002).
Stern, Y. et al. A framework for concepts of reserve and resilience in aging. Neurobiol. Aging 124, 100–103 (2023).
Lovden, M. et al. No moderating influence of education on the association between changes in hippocampus volume and memory performance in aging. Aging Brain 4, 100082 (2023).
Lovden, M., Fratiglioni, L., Glymour, M. M., Lindenberger, U. & Tucker-Drob, E. M. Education and cognitive functioning across the life span. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 21, 6–41 (2020).
Seblova, D., Berggren, R. & Lovden, M. Education and age-related decline in cognitive performance: systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res. Rev. 58, 101005 (2020).
Schneeweis, N., Skirbekk, V. & Winter-Ebmer, R. Does education improve cognitive performance four decades after school completion? Demography 51, 619–643 (2014).
Glymour, M. M., Kawachi, I., Jencks, C. S. & Berkman, L. F. Does childhood schooling affect old age memory or mental status? Using state schooling laws as natural experiments. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 62, 532–537 (2008).
Gorman, E. Does schooling have lasting effects on cognitive function? Evidence from compulsory schooling laws. Demography 60, 1139–1161 (2023).
Brinch, C. N. & Galloway, T. A. Schooling in adolescence raises IQ scores. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 425–430 (2012).
Lager, A., Seblova, D., Falkstedt, D. & Lovden, M. Cognitive and emotional outcomes after prolonged education: a quasi-experiment on 320 182 Swedish boys. Int. J. Epidemiol. 46, 303–311 (2017).
Courtin, E. et al. Long-term effects of compulsory schooling on physical, mental and cognitive ageing: a natural experiment. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 73, 370–376 (2019).
Ritchie, S. J. & Tucker-Drob, E. M. How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychol. Sci. 29, 1358–1369 (2018).
Walhovd, K. B., Lovden, M. & Fjell, A. M. Timing of lifespan influences on brain and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 27, 901–915 (2023).
Van Hootegem, A., Rogeberg, O., Bratsberg, B. & Lyngstad, T. H. Correlation between cognitive ability and educational attainment weakens over birth cohorts. Sci. Rep. 13, 17747 (2023).
Calandri, I. L. et al. Sex and socioeconomic disparities in dementia risk: a population attributable fractions analysis in Argentina. Neuroepidemiology 58, 264–275 (2024).
Walhovd, K. B. et al. Education and income show heterogeneous relationships to lifespan brain and cognitive differences across European and US cohorts. Cereb. Cortex 32, 839–854 (2022).
Paradela, R. S. et al. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in seven Latin American countries: an analysis using cross-sectional survey data. Lancet Glob. Health 12, e1600–e1610 (2024).
Gross, A. L. et al. Harmonisation of later-life cognitive function across national contexts: results from the Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocols. Lancet Healthy Longev. 4, e573–e583 (2023).
Tulving, E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 1–25 (2002).
Nyberg, L. in The Sage Handbook of Cognitive and Systems Neuroscience Vol. 1 (eds Boyle, G. J. et al.) 47–60 (Sage Publications, 2023).
Nyberg, L. et al. Biological and environmental predictors of heterogeneity in neurocognitive ageing: evidence from Betula and other longitudinal studies. Ageing Res. Rev. 64, 101184 (2020).
Borsch-Supan, A. et al. Data resource profile: the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Int. J. Epidemiol. 42, 992–1001 (2013).
Walhovd, K. B. et al. Healthy minds 0–100 years: optimising the use of European brain imaging cohorts (‘Lifebrain’). Eur. Psychiatry 50, 47–56 (2018).
Mehrbrodt, T., Gruber, S. & Wagner, M. Scales and Multi-item Indicators in the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE-ERIC, 2019); https://share-eric.eu/fileadmin/user_upload/SHARE_Working_Paper/WP_Series_45_2019.pdf
Zhang, Y. S. et al. Educational attainment and later-life cognitive function in high- and middle-income countries: evidence from the Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 79, gbae005 (2024).
Wood, S. N. & Scheipl, F. gamm4: generalized additive mixed models using ‘mgcv’ and ‘lme4’. R package version 0.2-6 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gamm4 (2020).
Cadar, D. et al. An international evaluation of cognitive reserve and memory changes in early old age in 10 European countries. Neuroepidemiology 48, 9–20 (2017).
Walhovd, K. B. et al. Brain aging differs with cognitive ability regardless of education. Sci. Rep. 12, 13886 (2022).
Heilbronner, R. L. et al. Official position of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology on serial neuropsychological assessments: the utility and challenges of repeat test administrations in clinical and forensic contexts. Clin. Neuropsychol. 24, 1267–1278 (2010).
Tucker-Drob, E. M., Johnson, K. E. & Jones, R. N. The cognitive reserve hypothesis: a longitudinal examination of age-associated declines in reasoning and processing speed. Dev. Psychol. 45, 431–446 (2009).
Strand, S., Deary, I. J. & Smith, P. Sex differences in cognitive abilities test scores: a UK national picture. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 76, 463–480 (2006).
Eurostat Regional Yearbook, 2024 Edition (Publications Office of the European Union, 2024); https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/web/products-flagship-publications/w/ks-ha-24-001
Evans, M. D. R., Kelley, J., Sikora, J. & Treiman, D. J. Family scholarly culture and educational success: books and schooling in 27 nations. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 28, 171–197 (2010).
Deary, I. J., Pattie, A. & Starr, J. M. The stability of intelligence from age 11 to age 90 years: the Lothian birth cohort of 1921. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2361–2368 (2013).
Bratsberg, B., Fjell, A. M., Rogeberg, O. J., Skirbekk, V. F. & Walhovd, K. B. Differences in cognitive function at 18 y of age explain the association between low education and early dementia risk. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 121, e2412017121 (2024).
Sharp, E. S. & Gatz, M. Relationship between education and dementia: an updated systematic review. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 25, 289–304 (2011).
Fürtjes, A. E. et al. Measurement characteristics and genome-wide correlates of lifetime brain atrophy estimated from a single MRI. Nat. Commun. (in the press).
van Loenhoud, A. C., Groot, C., Vogel, J. W., van der Flier, W. M. & Ossenkoppele, R. Is intracranial volume a suitable proxy for brain reserve? Alzheimers Res. Ther. 10, 91 (2018).
Engvig, A. et al. Effects of cognitive training on gray matter volumes in memory clinic patients with subjective memory impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 41, 779–791 (2014).
Engvig, A. et al. Effects of memory training on cortical thickness in the elderly. Neuroimage 52, 1667–1676 (2010).
Lovden, M. et al. Spatial navigation training protects the hippocampus against age-related changes during early and late adulthood. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 620.e9–620.e22 (2012).
Grasby, K. L. et al. The genetic architecture of the human cerebral cortex. Science 367, eaay6690 (2020).
Brathen, A. C. S. et al. Cognitive and hippocampal changes weeks and years after memory training. Sci. Rep. 12, 7877 (2022).
de Lange, A. G., Brathen, A. C. S., Rohani, D. A., Fjell, A. M. & Walhovd, K. B. The temporal dynamics of brain plasticity in aging. Cereb. Cortex 28, 1857–1865 (2018).
Judd, N. & Kievit, R. No effect of additional education on long-term brain structure—a preregistered natural experiment in thousands of individuals. eLife 13, RP101526 (2025).
Anderson, E. L. et al. Education, intelligence and Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from a multivariable two-sample Mendelian randomization study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 49, 1163–1172 (2020).
Okbay, A. et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature 533, 539–542 (2016).
Lee, J. J. et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 50, 1112–1121 (2018).
Ackerman, P. L. Adult intelligence: the construct and the criterion problem. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 987–998 (2017).
Li, W. et al. Timing in Early Childhood Education: How Cognitive and Achievement Program Impacts Vary by Starting Age, Program Duration, and Time since the End of the Program (Annenberg Institute for School Reform at Brown University, 2020); https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED610271
Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., Baldwin, A. & Baldwin, C. Stability of intelligence from preschool to adolescence: the influence of social and family risk factors. Child Dev. 64, 80–97 (1993).
Tucker-Drob, E. M. & Briley, D. A. Continuity of genetic and environmental influences on cognition across the life span: a meta-analysis of longitudinal twin and adoption studies. Psychol. Bull. 140, 949–979 (2014).
Edwin, T. H. et al. Trajectories of occupational cognitive demands and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in later life: the HUNT4 70+ study. Neurology 102, e209353 (2024).
Kivimaki, M. et al. Cognitive stimulation in the workplace, plasma proteins, and risk of dementia: three analyses of population cohort studies. BMJ 374, n1804 (2021).
Lipnicki, D. M. et al. Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study. PLoS Med. 14, e1002261 (2017).
Fry, A. et al. Comparison of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of UK Biobank participants with those of the general population. Am. J. Epidemiol. 186, 1026–1034 (2017).
Ritchie, S. J., Bates, T. C. & Deary, I. J. Is education associated with improvements in general cognitive ability, or in specific skills? Dev. Psychol. 51, 573–582 (2015).
Fjell, A. M. et al. What is normal in normal aging? Effects of aging, amyloid and Alzheimer’s disease on the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus. Prog. Neurobiol. 117, 20–40 (2014).
Gruber, S., Hunkler, C. & Stuck, S. Generating easySHARE: Guidelines, Structure, Content and Programming (SHARE-ERIC, 2014); https://share-eric.eu/fileadmin/user_upload/SHARE_Working_Paper/SHARE_WP_Series_17_2014.pdf
Bergmann, M., Kneip, T., De Luca, G. & Scherpenzeel, A. Survey Participation in the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE), Wave 1-7: Based on Release 7.0.0 (SHARE-ERIC, 2019); https://share-eric.eu/fileadmin/user_upload/SHARE_Working_Paper/WP_Series_41_2019_Bergmann_et_al.pdf
Börsch-Supan, A. & Gruber, S. easySHARE. Release version: 8.0.0. SHARE-ERIC https://doi.org/10.6103/SHARE.easy.800 (2022).
Walhovd, K. B. et al. Neurodevelopmental origins of lifespan changes in brain and cognition. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 9357–9362 (2016).
Nilsson, L. G. et al. The Betula prospective cohort study: memory, health, and aging. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 4, 1–32 (1997).
Nilsson, L.-G. et al. Betula: a prospective cohort study on memory, health and aging. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 11, 134–148 (2004).
Rajaram, S. et al. The Walnuts and Healthy Aging Study (WAHA): protocol for a nutritional intervention trial with walnuts on brain aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8, 333 (2016).
Vidal-Piñeiro, D. et al. Task-dependent activity and connectivity predict episodic memory network-based responses to brain stimulation in healthy aging. Brain Stimul. 7, 287–296 (2014).
Bertram, L. et al. Cohort profile: the Berlin Aging Study II (BASE-II). Int. J. Epidemiol. 43, 703–712 (2014).
Gerstorf, D. et al. Editorial. Gerontology 62, 311–315 (2016).
Shafto, M. A. et al. The Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) study protocol: a cross-sectional, lifespan, multidisciplinary examination of healthy cognitive ageing. BMC Neurol. 14, 204 (2014).
Idland, A. V. et al. Biomarker profiling beyond amyloid and tau: cerebrospinal fluid markers, hippocampal atrophy, and memory change in cognitively unimpaired older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 93, 1–15 (2020).
Mueller, S. G. et al. The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Neuroimaging Clin. N. Am. 15, 869–877 (2005).
Cattaneo, G. et al. The Barcelona Brain Health Initiative: a cohort study to define and promote determinants of brain health. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10, 321 (2018).
Dagley, A. et al. Harvard Aging Brain Study: dataset and accessibility. Neuroimage 144, 255–258 (2017).
Miller, K. L. et al. Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1523–1536 (2016).
Breitner, J. C. S., Poirier, J., Etienne, P. E. & Leoutsakos, J. M. Rationale and structure for a new Center for Studies on Prevention of Alzheimer’s Disease (StoP-AD). J. Prev. Alzheimers Dis. 3, 236–242 (2016).
Tremblay-Mercier, J. et al. Open science datasets from PREVENT-AD, a longitudinal cohort of pre-symptomatic Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage Clin. 31, 102733 (2021).
LaMontagne, P. J. et al. OASIS-3: longitudinal neuroimaging, clinical, and cognitive dataset for normal aging and Alzheimer disease. Preprint at medRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2019.12.13.19014902 (2019).
Kremen, W. S., Franz, C. E. & Lyons, M. J. Current status of the Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging (VETSA). Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 783–787 (2019).
Reuter, M., Schmansky, N. J., Rosas, H. D. & Fischl, B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage 61, 1402–1418 (2012).
Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I. & Dale, A. M. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195–207 (1999).
Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B. & Dale, A. M. High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 272–284 (1999).
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B. & Sereno, M. I. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194 (1999).
Destrieux, C., Fischl, B., Dale, A. & Halgren, E. Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. Neuroimage 53, 1–15 (2010).
Fischl, B. et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355 (2002).
Rutherford, S. et al. Charting brain growth and aging at high spatial precision. eLife 11, e72904 (2022).
Rutherford, S. et al. The normative modeling framework for computational psychiatry. Nat. Protoc. 17, 1711–1734 (2022).
Van Rossum, G. & Drake, F. L. Python 3 Reference Manual (CreateSpace, 2009).
Kia, S. M. et al. Closing the life-cycle of normative modeling using federated hierarchical Bayesian regression. PLoS ONE 17, e0278776 (2022).
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B. & Christensen, R. H. B. lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26 (2017).
Vidal-Piñeiro, D. et al. Reliability of structural brain change in cognitively healthy adult samples. Imaging Neurosci. 3, imag_a_00547 (2025).
R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing https://www.r-project.org/ (2024).
Bürkner, P.-C. brms: an R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. J. Stat. Softw. 80, 1–28 (2017).
Carpenter, B. et al. Stan: a probabilistic programming language. J. Stat. Softw. 76, 1–32 (2017).
Hofman, M. D. & Gelman, A. The No-U-Turn Sampler: adaptively setting path lengths in Hamiltonian Monte Carlo. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 15, 1593–1623 (2014).
Wickham, H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis 2nd edn (Springer, 2016).
Mowinckel, A. M. & Vidal-Piñeiro, D. Visualization of brain statistics with R packages ggseg and ggseg3d. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 3, 466–483 (2020).
Robinson, D. fuzzyjoin: join tables together on inexact matching. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/fuzzyjoin/index.html (2020).
Acknowledgements
The Lifebrain consortium is funded by EU Horizon 2020 grant agreement number 732592 (Lifebrain). The different substudies are supported by different sources. LCBC is supported by the European Research Council under grant agreement numbers 283634 and 725025 (to A.M.F.) and number 313440 (to K.B.W.) as well as the Norwegian Research Council (325878 and 262453 to A.M.F.; 325001, 301395 and 239889 to K.B.W.; 249931 to A.M.F. and K.B.W.; 324882 to D.V.-P.; 325415 to H.G.); the National Association for Public Health’s Dementia Research Program, Norway (to A.M.F.); and the University of Oslo through the UiO:Life Science convergence environment (to A.M.F.). Betula is supported by a scholar grant from the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation to L.N. Barcelona is partially supported by a Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness grant to D.B.-F. (grant no. PID2022–137234OB-100 (AEI/FEDER, UE)) and to G.C. and J.S. (grant no. PID-2022–139298OA-C22 (MCIN /AEI /10.13039/501100011033 / FEDER, UE)); by the Walnuts and Healthy Aging Study (grant no. NCT01634841), funded by the California Walnut Commission, Sacramento, California; and by ICREA Academia 2019 and 2024 awards. BASE-II has been supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research under grant numbers 16SV5537, 16SV5837, 16SV5538, 16SV5536K, 01UW0808, 01UW0706, 01GL1716A and 01GL1716B and by the European Research Council under grant agreement number 677804 (to S.K.). A.P.-L. is partly supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (R01AG076708), the Jack Satter Foundation and the BrightFocus Foundation. Part of the research was conducted using the UK Biobank resource under application number 32048. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, preparation of the manuscript or decision to publish. L.O.W. is funded by the South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authorities (no. 2017095), by the Norwegian Health Association (no. 19536 and no. 1513) and by Wellcome Leap’s Dynamic Resilience Program (jointly funded by Temasek Trust) (no. 104617). Parts of the data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Pre-Symptomatic Evaluation of Novel or Experimental Treatments for Alzheimer’s Disease (PREVENT-AD) program. Data were provided, in part, by OASIS-3 (OASIS-3 principal investigators: T. Benzinger, D. Marcus and J. Morris; NIH P50AG00561, P30NS09857781, P01AG026276, P01AG003991, R01AG043434, UL1TR000448 and R01EB009352). Parts of the data collection and sharing for this project were provided by the Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN). Cam-CAN funding was provided by the UK Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (grant no. BB/H008217/1), together with support from the UK Medical Research Council and the University of Cambridge. Parts of the data are from VETSA, which is funded by National Institute of Aging (NIA) R01 grants AG018384, AG018386, AG050595, AG022381 and AG076838. The content is the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIA, the NIH, the US Department of Veterans Affairs, the US Department of Defense, the National Personnel Records Center, the National Archives and Records Administration, the Internal Revenue Service, the National Opinion Research Center, the National Research Council, the National Academy of Sciences or the Institute for Survey Research. Temple University provided invaluable assistance in the conduct of the Vietnam Era Twin Registry. The Cooperative Studies Program of the US Department of Veterans Affairs provided financial support for development and maintenance of the Vietnam Era Twin Registry. We would also like to acknowledge the continued cooperation and participation of the members of the Vietnam Era Twin Registry and their families. Part of the data collection and sharing was funded by the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (NIH grant U01 AG024904) and DOD ADNI (Department of Defense award no. W81XWH-12–2–0012). The ADNI is funded by the NIA and the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering and through generous contributions from the following: AbbVie, the Alzheimer’s Association; the Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation; Araclon Biotech; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen; Bristol Myers Squibb; CereSpir, Inc.; Cogstate; Eisai, Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; EuroImmun; F. Hoffmann-La Roche, Ltd. and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; Fujirebio; GE Healthcare; IXICO, Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical Research & Development, LLC; Lumosity; Lundbeck; Merck & Co., Inc.; MesoScale Diagnostics, LLC; NeuroRx Research; Neurotrack Technologies; Novartis Pharmaceuticals; Pfizer, Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Takeda Pharmaceutical Company; and Transition Therapeutics. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (https://fnih.org/). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer’s Therapeutic Research Institute at the University of Southern California. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern California. Parts of the data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Harvard Aging Brain Study (HABS - P01AG036694; https://habs.mgh.harvard.edu). The HABS was launched in 2010, funded by the NIA, and is led by principal investigators R. A. Sperling and K. A. Johnson at Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School. The SHARE data collection has been funded by the European Commission, DG RTD through FP5 (QLK6-CT-2001-00360), FP6 (SHARE-I3: RII-CT-2006-062193, COMPARE: CIT5-CT-2005-028857, SHARELIFE: CIT4-CT-2006-028812), FP7 (SHARE-PREP: GA no. 211909, SHARE-LEAP: GA no. 227822, SHARE M4: GA no. 261982, DASISH: GA no. 283646) and Horizon 2020 (SHARE-DEV3: GA no.; 676536, SHARE-COHESION: GA no. 870628, SERISS: GA no. 654221, SSHOC: GA no. 823782, SHARE-COVID19: GA no. 101015924) and by DG Employment, Social Affairs & Inclusion through VS 2015/0195, VS 2016/0135, VS 2018/0285, VS 2019/0332, VS 2020/0313, SHARE-EUCOV: GA no. 101052589 and EUCOVII: GA no. 101102412. Additional funding from the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (01UW1301, 01UW1801, 01UW2202), the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science, the NIA (U01_AG09740-13S2, P01_AG005842, P01_AG08291, P30_AG12815, R21_AG025169, Y1-AG-4553-01, IAG_BSR06-11, OGHA_04-064, BSR12-04, R01_AG052527-02, R01_AG056329-02, R01_AG063944, HHSN271201300071C, RAG052527A) and various national funding sources is gratefully acknowledged (see https://www.share-eric.eu/).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
A.M.F., K.B.W., O.R. and D.V.-P. conceptualized the study. A.M.F., O.R., D.V.-P. and Ø.S. analyzed the data. A.M.D. wrote the paper. All authors critically revised the paper and approved the final version.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
A.P.-L. serves as a paid member of the scientific advisory boards for Neuroelectrics, Magstim Inc., TetraNeuron, Skin2Neuron, MedRhythms and AscenZion. He is co-founder of TI Solutions and co-founder and chief medical officer of Linus Health. A.P.-L. is also listed as an inventor on several issued and pending patents on the real-time integration of transcranial magnetic stimulation with electroencephalography and magnetic resonance imaging and applications of non-invasive brain stimulation in various neurological disorders as well as digital biomarkers of cognition and digital assessments for early diagnosis of dementia. The other authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Medicine thanks Sarah Ackley, Jasmine Mah and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editor: Jerome Staal, in collaboration with the Nature Medicine team.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Extended data
Supplementary information
41591_2025_3828_MOESM1_ESM.pdf
Supplementary Tables 1–9 and Figs. 1–11. MRI samples, Data availability, Education in the MRI cohorts, Memory testing in the MRI cohorts, MRI acquisition and preprocessing, SHARE sample distributions, Memory testing in SHARE and Sensitivity analyses of cognition.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Fjell, A.M., Rogeberg, O., Sørensen, Ø. et al. Reevaluating the role of education on cognitive decline and brain aging in longitudinal cohorts across 33 Western countries. Nat Med 31, 2967–2976 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-025-03828-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-025-03828-y