Abstract
In many temperate animals, reproductive cycles coincide with seasonal weather changes resulting in behaviour changes such as movement and habitat selection. In social species, these physiological and environmental changes can alter the costs and benefits of social interactions, impacting the structure of animal groups. In little brown myotis (Myotis lucifugus), a gregarious bat occupying much of North America, the pregnancy and lactation phases present different challenges to energy balance and maternal movement, and reduced forage distance has been observed during the lactation period. As such, we hypothesized that differences between reproductive phases alter the roost switching decisions of individual bats and therefore the overall group structure of little brown myotis maternity colonies. We observed that adult females were less likely to switch roosts during the lactation period even when accounting for changing weather conditions. This shift in roost switching behaviour may be the source of observed differences in group structure between reproductive periods. We reported a decline in network cohesiveness, but no meaningful variation in individual roost fidelity and association strengths of dyads between reproductive phases. These results support the contention that reproductive processes in female little brown myotis influence sociality and overall roosting patterns within maternity groups.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Animals modify their behaviour in response to seasonal changes in both the environment (e.g. weather) and their physiology (e.g., reproductive condition). For many mammalian taxa, behavioural patterns change at parturition. This may be due to different amounts of time invested in parental care and promoting offspring survival. Strategies to maximize offspring survival vary interspecifically and may include selecting areas of lower predation risk when offspring are most vulnerable at the expense of forage quality1, seeking out higher forage quality in areas of higher predation risk to meet the energetic demands of lactation2. Social behaviours may also vary with an individual’s changing reproductive status3,4, as the benefits of group living vary throughout the reproductive cycle. For example, ungulates often socially isolate prior to parturition2,5 while similarly in Scottish blackface sheep (Ovis aries), individuals with offspring take a more peripheral position in a flock6. The nature of interactions between individuals may also vary with breeding season, breeding status, and sex. For instance, females may increase agonistic interactions to protect offspring, while males increase affiliative interactions to improve access to breeding opportunities7. Additionally, within one sex, breeding status has been shown to be a predictor of behaviour, where mothers may increase their distance from conspecifics while nonmothers in the same group do not6.
Temperate bats have distinct seasonal cycles of movement and reproduction which includes a spring-summer season where females aggregate in large maternity groups8,9. At least some females switch roosts frequently throughout this period, resulting in changes in the composition of roosting subgroups each day10,11. While meeting the energetic demands of pregnancy and lactation, bats balance energy conservation via torpor with the potential costs of torpor to the timing of parturition and offspring survival12,13,14, and regularly employ social thermoregulation to minimize torpor use15,16. Accordingly, female bats may select warmer roosts or roosts that are used by a greater number of conspecifics to facilitate social thermoregulation15,17. Lactation represents a period of higher energetic demand for adult females than pregnancy18, but as ambient temperature is typically higher during lactation compared to pregnancy, it is not clear how energetic demands of different reproductive periods may influence bat roost selection. Not only is roost selection influenced by thermoregulatory needs, bats also need to balance foraging and parental care after parturition. Female little brown myotis (Myotis lucifugus) have been shown to travel shorter distances during foraging following parturition as females frequently return to roosts to nurse their pups19. Changes in foraging behaviour, along with the limited mobility of pre-volant young may impact the ability of females to change roosts.
Roosts used by bats may vary in temperature regime and proximity to forage sites, among other factors, and as such bats may switch roosts regularly20. Selecting appropriate roosts seems particularly important for reproductive females due to its influence on torpor use and therefore offspring development21. For example, Lausen and Barclay22 have shown that pregnant temperate bats select roosts with warmer and more stable temperatures late in the pregnancy period and during lactation22. Therefore, reproductive females should select roosts with appropriate microclimates that maximize energy balance given their physiological condition and the environmental conditions. However, as external environmental conditions change, conditions within the roost may also change requiring females to change roosts. This was demonstrated in big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus), where high ambient temperatures increased the frequency at which females switched roost buildings or moved within buildings23.
Bat roosting decisions also influence, and are influenced by, social relationships24,25,26,27,28,29. Individuals may select roosts based on the presence of certain conspecifics29 and thereby influence their social environment by the information they bring, or physically by their effect on space and roost temperature. Social grouping may confer additional benefits, such as energy conservation, cooperation during foraging, or by reducing inter-individual conflict30. The benefits and effects of group behaviour, and thus the structure of bat groups may also change between seasons. Variation in network structure and subsequently social relationships between seasons and reproductive periods has been shown in other social mammal species including Tasmanian devil (Sarcophilus harrisii)31, rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)32, and black-tailed prairie dogs (Cynomys ludovicianus)7. However, these studies focused on differences between mating and nonmating seasons in both male and female individuals rather than relationships between only females. Previous studies of female bats have shown greater aggregation of individuals at roosts during pregnancy compared to lactation in some species11,33, but smaller roosting groups during pregnancy in others34, and colony fragmentation during post-lactation in little brown myotis35. Further, it is expected that movement patterns of females are affected during lactation due to nursing requirements19, and thus reproductive phases may have distinct impacts on the social structure of bat groups.
Little brown myotis colonies are typically comprised of interconnected and sympatric social communities, where the frequencies with which individuals share roosts with one another are not entirely explained by roost fidelity36. There has been some suggestion in other Myotis species that these interconnected communities may facilitate information transfer37,38,39, and it is known that social thermoregulatory benefits of co-roosting improve offspring survival and development15,22. We hypothesized that changes in the physiological requirements of female little brown myotis due to reproductive condition will affect the roosting decisions and social structure of a maternity colony. Based on this hypothesis, we tested the prediction that (1) the likelihood of adult female bats using the same roost on consecutive days would be highest during lactation, when females have non-volant offspring even when environmental pressure (i.e., weather) to switch roosts is accounted for. We also predicted (2) that, given the time and energetic requirements to revisit roosts at night to nurse pups19 as well as to carry non-volant pups between roosts, not only would females generally switch roosts less frequently during lactation, but that they would also use a smaller subset of monitored roosts more frequently and others less frequently than during pregnancy. Finally (3), we predicted that the network-level patterns of co-roosting associations in the maternity colony would change between the pregnancy and lactation periods as less frequent switching, as in predictions (1) and (2), would reduce overall connectivity of the maternity colony. As bats return to the same roosts within and between years, there is the potential for social familiarity and preferences to form among individuals, preferences which may become more apparent when the number of associates is reduced, as we predict may occur during the lactation period.
Results
Three hundred and seventy seven adult female little brown myotis met the criteria of having at least ten observations before 25th percentile of parturition dates and at least 10 observations after the 75th percentile of parturition dates in a single year (Table 1). Of the 377 individuals, 218 were included in multiple years and two individuals were included in eight of the nine years of the study. Numbers of tagged individuals who we suspect entered or left our monitored system following parturition, individuals for which there were at least ten day roost observations in one reproductive period but zero observations in the other reproductive period in a single year, did not correlate with the timing of the parturition cut-off date.
Variation in daily roost switching behaviour
Results of the model selection analysis suggested the candidate model that included maternal care and thermoregulation best explained the probability of bats reusing the same roost on consecutive days (AICc = 21530.8, model weight = 0.64), and the global model which also included flight conditions was the second best (AICc= 21532.0, model weight = 0.36) and only other model included in the 95% confidence set (Table 1). Our results showed that standard errors of parameter estimates did not overlap zero and were thus likely significant for changes in absolute humidity between consecutive days and reproductive period as predictors of the probability of bats using the same roost (Table 2). Our results specifically showed that a greater change in absolute humidity between consecutive days reduced the likelihood that a bat would reuse a roost (Fig. 1A). Also, as predicted, bats were more likely to reuse a roost on consecutive days following parturition (Fig. 1). Model-averaged parameter estimates revealed temperature, maximum wind gust speed, and the interaction between temperature and humidity were not significant parameters for predicting the probability of using the same roost on consecutive days as the model estimates plus or minus the standard errors overlapped zero (Table 2). However, it must be noted that there are likely many unaccounted for factors that also explain roost switching behaviour as model fit was low, with a corrected R2 of 0.24 for both the global model and the model including thermoregulation and maternal care.
A) Predicted probability of bats reusing the same roost on consecutive days as a function of change in absolute humidity from one day to the next, change in temperature from one day to the next, and reproductive period. Changes in temperature had little effect on the probability of roost reuse while change in absolute humidity was highly influential. B) The proportion of observed bats that reused a roost on consecutive days across all dates and weather conditions in the pregnancy and lactation periods from 2012–2017, the period when hourly weather data were available. Each point represents the proportion of bats that reused the same roost on any given consecutive day period.
Variation in roost fidelity and dyadic roosting relationships with reproductive period
On average, roost fidelity of individuals was significantly correlated (average = 0.66, SE = 0.02, range: 0.56—0.72) before and after parturition based on Spearman rank correlation (p < 0.001). This means that there was moderate correlation in the frequency that individual bats used each monitored roost before and after parturition, although roost use patterns by some individuals changed between reproductive periods. The nature of the change in behaviour varied greatly among bats, where for some individuals, this change was primarily a reduction in the number of different roosts used, while in others there was a change in the frequency that different roosts were used. For example, in 2017, one bat was detected in seven different roost boxes during the pregnancy period and only two boxes during the lactation period while many other bats showed no change in the total number of boxes used during a season. Meanwhile, box use frequencies also occasionally changed where, for example, one bat in 2019 used box 106 most frequently (15 of 25 detections) during the pregnancy period but box 108 (15 of 27 detections) most frequently during the lactation period.
Association strengths (SRI) between dyads during the pregnancy period were significantly correlated with association strengths during the lactation period (p < 0.001), indicating that, on average, many relationships between females were also consistent throughout the maternity season. However, there was considerable variability among dyads as some individuals that associated frequently during the pregnancy period never associated during lactation and vice versa. The average Mantel R score based on a Spearman Rank Correlation of SRI between reproductive periods was 0.51 (Range = 0.42–0.67; SE = 0.03) indicating that while some dyads showed consistent association patterns throughout the entire maternity period, others experienced changes in association patterns between pregnancy and lactation.
Changes in association patterns among bats did not appear to be related to changes in individual roost use patterns (Fig. 2; β = 0.84, SE = 0.51, p = 0.15). Analyses were also run using the half weight index (HWI) and yielded comparable results.
Relationship between correlation in roost use patterns and correlation in association strengths (Simple Ratio Index; SRI) between the pregnancy and lactation periods of each year. Linear regression revealed a significant but weak effect of correlation in roost use patterns between reproductive periods on the similarity in association strengths (SRI; β = 0.84, SE = 0.51, p = 0.15, R2 = 0.17).
Variation in network structure among reproductive periods
Each of the Coefficient of Variation of the Simple Ratio Index (CV SRI), graph density, and clustering coefficient changed significantly between reproductive periods (all p < 0.05). The CV SRI increased during lactation while graph density and clustering coefficient each decreased (Fig. 3). Analyses using HWI and the CV HWI yielded comparable results.
The change in (A) coefficient of variation of the simple ratio index (CV SRI), (B) graph density, and (C) clustering coefficient between pregnancy and lactation periods. Each line connects points relating to a single year. P-values are reported based on a paired t-test comparing values between reproductive periods. CV SRI showed a significant increase following parturition while graph density and clustering coefficient both showed a significant decrease.
Discussion
In animals that invest significantly in parental care, it is expected that behavioural patterns will change once offspring are present. We demonstrated that female little brown myotis generally maintained similar roost fidelity during lactation as during the pregnancy period (prediction 2) but were less likely to switch roosts during the lactation period in response to changing weather conditions than during the pregnancy period (prediction 1). Roosting association strengths between individuals during pregnancy were also predictive of these relationships during lactation, and our results suggested that although patterns in roost use may change once pups are present in the system, individual variation in selection for both associates and roosts were generally maintained throughout the maternity season (prediction 3). However, just as bats may be limited in their ability to switch roosts during the lactation period, likely due to the need to frequently revisit the same roost at night to nurse non-volant pups19, bats in our study did not maintain all co-roosting social connections that existed prior to parturition into the lactation period, as evidenced by declines in network density and clustering coefficient. Overall, our findings suggest that reproduction may be an important factor in the patterns of co-roosting associations of female little brown myotis and influence the wider structure of the maternity network.
In support of our first prediction, regardless of weather conditions, bats were more likely to reuse a roost on consecutive days following parturition. These results could reflect that females are either more limited in their ability to change roosts when caring for a pup and/or that there is a benefit to keeping a pup in the same roost. Importantly, changes in weather conditions did have an impact on the probability of switching roosts across both reproductive periods, and we found changes in absolute humidity to be the most influential environmental factor in determining whether female bats switched roosts between consecutive days, with no significant effect of changes in temperature. The influence of absolute humidity is also supported by findings demonstrating absolute ambient moisture as the most important factor in determining evaporative water loss and the surface temperature of animals40, and thus switching roosts during periods of high absolute humidity may allow females to avoid torpor use and the associated delays in gestation14. It remains to be determined how reduced roost switching during lactation at periods of high absolute humidity may impact survival of both the mother and offspring.
Meanwhile, given that female Bechstein’s bats (M. bechsteinii) preferred warmer roosts during the lactation period41, it is surprising that temperature did not have a significant effect on roost switching patterns in little brown myotis. This is unlike the findings of Ellison et al. (2007), and that the effect of temperature appeared similar in both reproductive periods. These results may be due to the similar construction of the monitored roost boxes along with the presence of other bats, therefore making social thermoregulation more influential on the temperature of the roost. Wind gusts and precipitation also did not appear to be influential in affecting daily roost switching of bats in this study. Therefore, conditions outside the roost may not be important determinants of roost selection and strong winds or heavy rains not limiting to mobility, particularly given the close proximity of the roosts we monitored. However, our results also suggest that there are many unaccounted-for factors influencing the roost switching decisions of female little brown myotis. Factors such as changes in foraging sites and other social mechanisms may represent other, unmeasured factors in roosting decisions. Roost type has also been shown to affect roost switching in response to precipitation in other temperate bat species in Atlantic Canada42, and therefore the use of artificial roost boxes may also have impacted our results. The distance between our study site and the nearest weather station at Pippy Park (≈ 54 km) and the variable nature of precipitation and wind across a landscape compared to temperature and humidity may also have limited our ability to make inferences about precipitation and wind as factors influencing roost switching decisions, particularly given that Pippy Park is much closer to the Atlantic Ocean (≈ 4 km) than Salmonier Nature Park (≈ 20 km).
We found that although individuals maintained fidelity to the same roosts across the pregnancy and lactation periods, there was evidence of less roost switching during the lactation period. Although previous research has found that maternity group formation is relatively stable in little brown myotis and Yuma bat (M. yumanesis) groups35, it is unsurprising that roost use patterns differed slightly between reproductive periods given previous evidence for differential roost preference41, reduced mobility and roost switching during lactation19, and that seasonal changes in body composition and hormone status, such as those experienced throughout a reproductive cycle, can impact an individual’s thermoregulatory needs43. It is additionally possible that during the pregnancy period, bats regularly use more roosts, and then reduce to this “core” set during lactation to which they have high fidelity as our results on roost switching and changes in roost fidelity suggest. As we do not know how many unmonitored roosts are used by the bats in Salmonier Nature Park, further study would be needed to quantify the number of different roosts used by reproductive females in both pregnancy and lactation periods, and how females may be selecting the roosts that they use the most frequently. However, given the similarity in construction of monitored roost boxes and lack of information on prey availability, we can only speculate as to why bats differed from each other in their potential roost preferences.
It is important to note that in social animals, not only are behavioural decisions influenced by the environment an individual uses, but that these decisions can also alter the environment itself, a concept now known as social niche construction44,45. Based on the relationship between individuals and their social environment, decisions made by parents about site selection and social association with conspecifics throughout their reproductive cycle can also influence the wider population and therefore the behaviour of conspecifics. The observed changes in roosting decisions here, in turn resulted in changes in the social structure of the animal group, specifically a decline in network connectivity, and it is unknown whether or how this reduction in connectivity during lactation improves outcomes for mothers and pups. We reported a weak relationship in which years with higher correlation in roost use patterns between reproductive phases also displayed a higher correlation in association patterns. Given this, we cannot rule out our third prediction, that in addition to limitations in roosting decisions imposed by parental care, the social needs and preferences of mothers may change once pups are present. We found an overall decline in network connectivity, an expected result given the less frequent roost switching of lactating females. The higher network connectivity during pregnancy may also facilitate information transfer as in rhesus macaques, females played an important role in information transfer during the mating season32. Our results were also consistent with other network studies on bats that report little brown myotis and big brown bat roosting groups are larger prior to parturition11,33. However, as we were only focused on individuals which met minimum criteria over the study period and did not have information on the number of pups in the system or the number of untagged bats, it remains uncertain whether changes in roosting group size, as reported by Olson and Barclay46, also occur in this system.
While it remains unclear what the benefits or consequences of reduced network connectivity during lactation may be, for the endangered little brown myotis, it may be important to consider these patterns when supplementing roosting habitat. As such, while roosts close together as recommended by Holroyd et al.47 may allow greater connectivity and reduce the costs of switching roosts when pups are present, a greater availability of more distant roosts in variable locations may be important to allow pregnant females to accommodate their thermoregulatory needs. We also suggest that even in small populations, multiple roost options is important for ensuring that females can reproduce successfully for years to come. Given that parturition in this species is coincident with seasonal changes in weather conditions, we cannot rule out that some of the observed changes in roost use patterns may be attributable to seasonal differences in the environment independent of reproductive needs. That said, given the consistency of these changes with our expectation based on reproductive pressures, and the importance of reproductive period when assessing roost switching behaviour even when environmental factors are controlled for, it is reasonable to infer that the reproductive cycle is an important influence on network structure. As the reproductive status of many individuals in our study is uncertain, we could not determine how a shift in reproductive condition may affect roosting decisions at an individual level. Future study investigating parturition timing in conjunction with behavioural changes at an individual level will be crucial for gaining a deeper understanding of the relationship between reproduction and social behaviour as parturition in little brown myotis and other temperate bat species is largely asynchronous10,48. Thus our data represents an approximation of when behavioural shifts are expected. However, individual bats may change their habitat or social needs at different times. As our analysis represents a broad overview of what may be occurring in the population at an average parturition date, it is expected that changes would be even more pronounced when analyses are refined to the level of the individual. As changes in roosting patterns are expected to occur at the time of parturition, it would be of interest to investigate whether the association patterns between individuals may be based on similarity in reproductive timing.
Other limitations of our research include that our study focused on individuals present throughout the study period and transient individuals may be influencing network structure and interacting at unmonitored locations. We do not know how the inclusion of nearby, unmonitored roosts would affect these results but expect that patterns of association and roost-use of our included bats are reflective of their general behaviour patterns given that we required a minimum number of observations for inclusion in network analysis. Finally, we assumed that years were sufficiently independent from each other due to the large number of individuals that entered or left the study system between years, but the consistency in individual roosting decisions and social relationships between years remains to be determined. Similar studies in systems where a known, high percentage of individuals are monitored over multiple years or in a closed system would be highly beneficial for more confidently characterizing the causes and consequences of the patterns we identify here.
Overall, our data support the contention that female little brown myotis roosting patterns and thus the social landscape of maternity groups are influenced by reproductive processes. Females expectedly switched roosts less frequently and co-roosted with a smaller subset of the maternity group during the lactation period, but what the fitness consequences or benefits may be for these behaviour changes remains to be determined. Consistent with studies in other mammal species1,5,49,50, these findings lend additional support to the contention that in animals demonstrating maternal care, the presence of offspring has a strong influence on decisions parents make, and that individuals require flexibility in responses to their environment throughout a reproductive cycle.
Methods
Data collection
Using mist nets (Avinet, Dryden, New York, USA) and harp traps (Austbat Research Equipment, Lower Plenty, Victoria, Australia) little brown myotis were captured in and around Salmonier Nature Park (n = 1604; Lat: 47.3º, Long: -53.3º) Newfoundland, Canada from 2012 to 2021 between May 15th and August 19th of each year. Reproductive status51 and age52 were determined for each individual and then a passive integrative transponder (PIT) tag (0.09 g; EID-ID100 implantable transponders, EIDAPInc, Sherwood Park, Alberta, Canada and Trovan Electronic Identification Systems, North Ferriby, UK) was subcutaneously implanted between the scapula. Transponder antennas (LID650, Dorset Identification, The Netherlands) were deployed at eleven roost boxes within a 1.1 km2 area from April to September each year to collect data on roost use. One roost box was placed on a building while the other ten were paired on five poles throughout the study area. Distances between boxes ranged from centimeters, for those placed on the same pole, 10–20 m between poles within a section of the study area, and 100 m to 1 km between sections. Further details on the study system can be found in Sunga et al.36. The last detection before sunset, but after sunrise of the same day, was used to infer the roost box in which an individual spent the day and represents an ‘observation’. Due to potential errors in PIT-tag recording, an unknown number of unmonitored roosts, and an unknown proportion of the population being untagged, it is expected that many instances of roost use were unrecorded.
Across the entire study period, there were 1083 tagged adult females, including some females that were initially tagged as juveniles. Males were not included in these analyses as they were rarely detected at maternity roosts and do not have the same variation in energy requirements as females throughout the maternity season18. Juveniles were excluded as it is uncertain whether juvenile movements are independent from their mothers37,53, and juveniles would only be present for the second portion of the maternity season. Among all adult females, the number of day roost records varied between 1 and 88 (mean = 22) day roost records across a maternity season, and the number of tagged adult females within a monitored roost varied anywhere from 0 to 110 individuals on any given day. Adult females that may have been non-reproductive in a given year were included in our analyses. Data were constrained to include detections between 15 May and 15 August of each year and only individuals that were observed > 10 times in each of the pregnancy and lactation reproductive periods in a single year were included54. For each year the pregnancy period was determined as the period from 15 May until the date when up to 25% of femles in which parturition could be detected were expected to have had their pups in each year (Table 3)55. The lactation period was determined as the period from the date when at least 75% of females were expected to have had their pups until 15 August. Observations between these dates were not included to reduce contamination of data regarding the pregnancy and lactation periods. As parturition is relatively asynchronous, these cutoffs were chosen to reduce the number of individuals already lactating included in the parturition period while also ensuring that there were not many still pregnant individuals for a significant portion of the lactation period. Further, a later cut-off would risk more individuals beginning to depart for swarming and hibernation sites during the observation window and thus fewer observations on their lactation-state roost use and association patterns. Individuals were included in each year in which they met these criteria, and so some individuals were included in multiple years of analysis.
Variation in daily roost switching behaviour
To assess the extent to which reproductive period may influence daily roosting decisions in bats, we applied an information theoretic approach using Conditional Akaike’s Information Criterion43 of a candidate set of mixed effects models to explain variation in the likelihood of reusing the same roost on consecutive days based on logistic models. Despite some roosts providing some internal microclimate variation 21, we expected that large changes in external weather conditions are more likely to impact internal roost conditions or nearby foraging opportunities, and therefore lead to roost switching. However, we also predicted that reproductive period would also influence the likelihood of roost switching, even when external weather conditions are accounted for. We quantified the probability of individual bats reusing the same roost on consecutive days in each reproductive period and in response to daily weather conditions between 15 May and 15 August. This analysis included information for all individuals for which we observed day roosting in our monitored boxes on consecutive days on at least one instance. As hourly precipitation data were not available for the entire study period, this analysis included data only to 2017, with dates included in the analysis only if precipitation data was available on consecutive days.
Each bat on each day was assigned a 0 or 1 based on its location on the consecutive day where a 1 indicated a bat that was in the same roost on day one and day two while a 0 indicated that a bat was in a different, monitored roost on day one than on day two. Any bats that were recorded on day one, but not recorded on day two were not included in the analysis due to the possibility that the disappearance may actually be a missed read at the same roost, creating contamination of our dataset. Although it is expected that the number of bats that reuse a roost between consecutive days may be underestimated due to missed observations, it is expected that these observations were missed randomly and do not covary with our independent variables.
Our candidate model set consisted of multiple, nonmutually exclusive explanations for the roosting decisions of female bats. A candidate model of thermoregulatory effects driving roost use included change in mean nightly temperature between consecutive days (⁰C), change in mean nightly absolute humidity between consecutive days (g/m3), and an interaction between these terms as it is expected that a night that is both cold and humid would pose a different thermoregulatory challenge than a night that is cold and dry13. Both temperature and absolute humidity were calculated based on the average of hourly recordings taken between 02:00 and 07:00 on each day, when we expected bats would make their day roosting decisions. The change in temperature (∆Cº) and change in absolute humidity (∆AH) was then calculated as the absolute difference between the mean on each day, with the expectation that a greater change in either of these variables would decrease the likelihood of a bat reusing the same roost. This is because roosts may have different microclimates and therefore if weather drives21,] at least in part, roost selection decisions, individuals are less likely to reuse the same roost as weather conditions show greater variability one day to the next. A candidate model of flight conditions included maximum nightly windspeed and total precipitation, as it was expected that very high winds or heavy precipitation would result in more energetically expensive flight and increase the likelihood of bats reusing a roost. The maximum nightly windspeed was calculated based on the maximum reported wind gust between 02:00 and 07:00 on each day, and total precipitation as the sum of all precipitation also between 02:00 and 07:00. Finally, a candidate model of maternal care was tested broadly with a binary variable of whether the date was before (0) or after (1) the population parturition date estimate, separating the pregnancy and lactation periods for this study (Table 3), where bats were predicted to more frequently reuse a roost on consecutive days during the lactation period, when more females are expected to be nursing young. The random effect of year was included to account for annual changes based on factors such as spring climate conditions and timing of emergence from hibernation13,56,57, and it is unknown whether roost preferences vary annually.
A random effect of individual was also included to account for the fact that some individuals may be included in this analysis more than others due to greater fidelity to our monitored roosts. All weather data with the exception of hourly precipitation information were obtained from the Environment Canada weather station at St. John’s International Airport, approximately 57 km northeast of Salmonier Nature Park, through “weathercan”58. Precipitation data was provided from the Pippy Park weather station (Government of Newfoundland and Labrador, Department of Environment and Climate Change, Water Resources Management Division (WRMD)), approximately 54 km northeast of Salmonier Nature Park. Although available weather stations are not immediately near our study site, given we are looking at changes in weather conditions rather than absolute conditions, we expect that the stations are close enough such that large changes at these nearby weather stations equate to large weather changes at our study sites.
Our candidate model set al.so included models that represented combinations of these potential explanations for roost switching behaviour, such as torpor (temperature and humidity) with flight conditions (wind and precipitation) in one model (Table 1). Additional two-way interaction terms were not included to avoid overfitting of models without clear biological precedent. Further, due to a correlation between variables, ∆AH and precipitation were never included in the same model and precipitation was omitted from the global model and the combined model of maternal care and flight condition model. Using a Corrected Akaike’s Information Criterion (AICc) we calculated AICc values and determined the best performing model. We then selected the 95% confidence set of models, and applied multi-model averaging of variable estimates59.
Variation in roost fidelity and dyadic roosting relationships with reproductive period
To assess possible changes in roost preferences between reproductive periods in each year, we generated matrices of roost fidelity, the proportion of days in which individuals roosted in each monitored roost box. Only individuals that were observed > 10 times in both of the pregnancy and lactation reproductive periods in a single year were included to ensure sufficient sampling of each period within individuals. For each reproductive period-year combination, we generated an individual-by-roost matrix populated with the proportion of days an individual bat was observed in each monitored box calculated against the total number of days that bat was observed. We then compared these roost fidelity matrices between reproductive periods for each year using a Spearman Rank Correlation to quantify the extent to which roost preferences during pregnancy may be indicative of roost preferences during lactation.
Next we calculated the association strength between pairs of individuals (dyads) based on the simple ratio index (SRI) which is a proportion of the number of instances a dyad was found to be associating compared to the proportion of instances dyads were not associating60,61. We applied the gambit-of-the-group assumption61 such that dyads were assumed to be associating if they were recorded in the same roost on the same day. To assess associate preferences, we generated an individual-by-individual matrix with values corresponding to the SRI between each dyad using the function get_network in the package “asnipe”62. In each year, we conducted a Mantel test using the function mantel in the package “ecodist”63 with 10,000 permutations to test the similarity of associate preferences before and after parturition. Mantel R was calculated using the Spearman Rank Correlation to account for skew in the distribution of SRI values.
We then performed a simple linear regression to determine if the correlation in roost use by individuals before and after parturition was predictive of the correlation in SRI between reproductive periods. This allowed us to make inference on how a change in roost preferences may impact changes in social relationships and vice versa.
Variation in network structure among reproductive periods
Using individuals that were detected at least 10 times in each of the pregnancy and lactation periods, we assessed the changes in network metrics between pregnancy and lactation periods. As we only used individuals which met the above-criteria in both periods, the same number of individuals were used to construct each reproductive period network within a year. For each reproductive period-year network based on co-roosting associations, we calculated several metrics. The coefficient of variation of the SRI (CV SRI) was calculated by computing the standard deviation of all SRI values over the mean of all SRI values then multiplying by 100. Graph density, a proportional measure of the number of connections present in the network versus the number of potential connections, was calculated using the function edge_density. Clustering coefficient, the probability that individuals with a common associate are also themselves connected64 was calculated with the function transitivity in the package “igraph”65. We also calculated community assortativity (Rcom) a measure of the reliability of the assortment of individuals between potential social communities66. For each year, we conducted a paired t-test for each of the following network metrics; graph density, clustering coefficient, and CV SRI, to determine if there was a significant difference before and after parturition.
All analyses were conducted and figures created in R version 4.0.0 (R Core Team 2020).
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available as data are part of an ongoing, long-term study, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Example code used for analysis described in this manuscript and to produce the included figures will be made available on GitHub following acceptance.
References
Severud, W. J., DelGiudice, G. D. & Obermoller, T. R. Association of moose parturition and post-parturition habitat with calf survival. J. Wildl. Manage.83, 175–183 (2019).
Berg, J. E. et al. Mothers’ movements: shifts in Calving Area Selection by partially migratory elk. J. Wildl. Manage.85, 1476–1489 (2021).
Shizuka, D. et al. Across-year social stability shapes network structure in wintering migrant sparrows. Ecol. Lett.17, 998–1007 (2014).
Rose, P. E. & Croft, D. P. Evaluating the social networks of four flocks of captive flamingos over a five-year period: temporal, environmental, group and health influences on assortment. Behav. Process.175, (2020). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104118
Karsch, R. C., Cain, J. W., Rominger, E. M. & Goldstein, E. J. Desert bighorn sheep lambing habitat: parturition, nursery, and predation sites. J. Wildl. Manage.80, 1069–1080 (2016).
Pérez-Barbería, F. J. & Walker, D. M. Dynamics of social behaviour at parturition in a gregarious ungulate. Behav. Process.150, 75–84 (2018).
Kusch, J. M. & Lane, J. E. Variation of social strategies within and between individual black-tailed prairie dogs (Cynomys ludovicianus) during the reproductive season. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.75, 10 (2021).
McCracken, G. F. & Wilkinson, G. S. Bat Mating Systems. in Reproductive Biology of Bats (eds. Crichton, E. G. & Krutzsch, P. H.) 321–362Academic Press, doi: (2000). https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-012195670-7/50009-6
Fenton, M. B. Science and the conservation of bats. J. Mammal. 78, 1–14 (1997).
Fenton, M. B. & Barclay, R. M. R. Myotis lucifugus. Mammalian Species. 142, 1–8 (1980).
Willis, C. K. R. & Brigham, R. M. Roost switching, roost sharing and social cohesion: forest-dwelling big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, conform to the fission-fusion model. Anim. Behav.68, 495–505 (2004).
Racey, P. & Swift, S. M. Variations in gestation length in a colony of pipistrelle bats (Pipistrellus pipistrellus) from year to year. J. Reprod. Fertil.61, 123–129 (1981).
Frick, W. F., Reynolds, D. S. & Kunz, T. H. Influence of climate and reproductive timing on demography of little brown myotis Myotis Lucifugus. J. Anim. Ecol.79, 128–136 (2010).
Dzal, Y. A. & Brigham, R. M. The tradeoff between torpor use and reproduction in little brown bats (Myotis lucifugus). J. Comp. Physiol. B. 183, 279–288 (2013).
Willis, C. K. R. & Brigham, R. M. Social Thermoregulation exerts more influence than microclimate on forest roost preferences by a cavity-dwelling bat. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.62, 97–108 (2007).
Webber, Q. M. R. & Willis, C. K. R. An experimental test of effects of ambient temperature and roost quality on aggregation by little brown bats (Myotis lucifugus). J. Therm. Biol.74, 174–180 (2018).
Sedgeley, J. A. Quality of cavity microclimate as a factor influencing selection of maternity roosts by a tree-dwelling bat, Chalinolobus tuberculatus, in New Zealand. J. Appl. Ecol.38, 425–438 (2001).
Kurta, A., Bell, G. P., Nagy, K. A. & Kunz, T. H. Energetics of pregnancy and lactation in Freeranging Little Brown bats (Myotis lucifugus). Physiol. Zool.62, 804–818 (1989).
Henry, M., Thomas, D. W., Vaudry, R. & Carrier, M. Foraging distances and home range of pregnant and lactating little brown bats (Myotis lucifugus). J. Mammal. 83, 767–774 (2002).
Hoeh, J. P. S., Bakken, G. S., Mitchell, W. A. & Id, J. M. O. K. In artificial roost comparison, bats show preference for rocket box style. PLoS ONE 13(10):1–16 (2018).
Bergeson, S. M., Brigham, R. M. & O’keefe, J. M. Free-ranging bats alter thermoregulatory behavior in response to reproductive stage, roost type, and weather. J. Mammal. 102, 705–717 (2021).
Lausen, C. L. & Barclay, R. M. R. Thermoregulation and roost selection by reproductive female big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) roosting in rock crevices. 235–244 doi: (2003). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952836903003686
Ellison, L. E., O’Shea, T. J., Neubaum, D. J. & Bowen, R. A. Factors influencing movement probabilities of big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) in buildings. Ecol. Appl.17, 620–627 (2007).
Wilkinson, G. S. The social organization of the common vampire bat. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.17, 111–121 (1985).
Wilkinson, G. S. et al. Kinship, association, and social complexity in bats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.73, 7 (2019).
Chaverri, G., Gamba-Rios, M. & Kunz, T. H. Range overlap and association patterns in the tent-making bat Artibeus watsoni. Anim. Behav.73, 157–164 (2007).
Mavrodiev, P., Fleischmann, D., Kerth, G. & Schweitzer, F. Quantifying individual influence in leading-following behavior of Bechstein’s bats. Sci. Rep.11, 1–13 (2021).
Kerth, G. Causes and consequences of sociality in bats. Bioscience. 58, 737–746 (2008).
Bartonička, T. & Řehák, Z. Influence of the microclimate of bat boxes on their occupation by the soprano pipistrelle Pipistrellus pygmaeus: possible cause of roost switching. Acta Chiropt.9, 517–526 (2007).
Carter, G. G. & Wilkinson, G. S. Cooperation and Conflict in the Social Lives of Bats. in Bat Evolution, Ecology, and Conservation doi: (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-7397-8
Hamede, R. K., Bashford, J., McCallum, H. & Jones, M. Contact networks in a wild tasmanian devil (Sarcophilus harrisii) population: using social network analysis to reveal seasonal variability in social behaviour and its implications for transmission of devil facial tumour disease. Ecol. Lett.12, 1147–1157 (2009).
Brent, L. J. N., MacLarnon, A., Platt, M. L. & Semple, S. Seasonal changes in the structure of rhesus macaque social networks. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.67, 349–359 (2013).
Webber, Q. M. R. et al. Social network characteristics and predicted pathogen transmission in summer colonies of female big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.70, 701–712 (2016).
Patriquin, K. J., Leonard, M. L., Broders, H. G. & Garroway, C. J. Do social networks of female northern long-eared bats vary with reproductive period and age? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.64, 899–913 (2010).
Rensel, L. J., Hodges, K. E. & Lausen, C. L. Maternity colony social structure of myotis in British Columbia, Canada. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.76(159), (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-022-03265-8
Sunga, J., Webber, Q. M. R., Humber, J., Rodrigues, B. & Broders, H. G. Roost fidelity partially explains maternity roosting association patterns in Myotis lucifugus. Anim. Behav.194, 67–78 (2022).
Wilkinson, G. S. Information transfer at evening bat colonies. Anim. Behav.44, 501–518 (1992).
Ripperger, S. et al. Proximity sensors reveal social information transfer in maternity colonies of Greater noctule bats. bioRxiv. 421974https://doi.org/10.1101/421974 (2018).
Kerth, G. & Reckardt, K. Information transfer about roosts in female Bechstein’s bats: An experimental field study. in Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences vol. 270 511–515Royal Society, (2003).
Kurta, A. The misuse of relative humidity in ecological studies of hibernating bats. Acta Chiropt.16, 249–254 (2014).
Kerth, G., Weissmann, K. & König, B. Day roost selection in female Bechstein’s bats (Myotis bechsteinii): a field experiment to determine the influence of roost temperature. Oecologia. 126, 1–9 (2001).
Patriquin, K. J. et al. Weather as a proximate explanation for fission–fusion dynamics in female northern long-eared bats. Anim. Behav.122, 47–57 (2016).
Terrien, J., Perret, M. & Aujard, F. Behavioural thermoregulation in mammals: a review. Fronteirs Bioscience. 16, 1428–1444 (2011).
Stamps, J. & Groothuis, T. G. G. The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectives. Biol. Rev.85, 301–325 (2010).
Flack, J. C., Girvan, M., De Waal, F. B. M. & Krakauer, D. C. Policing stabilizes construction of social niches in primates. Nature. 439, 426–429 (2006).
Olson, C. R. & Barclay, R. M. R. Concurrent changes in group size and roost use by reproductive female little brown bats (Myotis lucifugus). Can. J. Zool.91, 149–155 (2013).
Holroyd, S. et al. Best Management Practices for the Use of Bat Houses in U.S. and Canada With Focus on Summer Habitat Mitigation for Little Brown Myotis, Yuma Myotis, and Big Brown Bat. https://doi.org/10.7944/P99K4BF5 doi:10.7944/P99K4BF5.
Krochmal, A. R. & Sparks, D. W. Timing of birth and estimation of age of juvenile Myotis septentrionalis and Myotis lucifugus in West-central Indiana. J. Mammal. 88, 649–656 (2007).
Hinton, J. W. & Chamberlain, M. J. Space and Habitat Use by a Red Wolf Pack and their pups during pup-rearing. J. Wildl. Manage.74, 55–58 (2010).
Pitman, J. W., Cain, J. W., Liley, S. G., Gould, W. R. & Quintana, N. T. Post-parturition habitat selection by elk calves and adult female elk in New Mexico. J. Wildl. Manage.78, 1216–1227 (2014).
Racey, P. A. Reproductive Assessment in Bats. In: Kunz TH (Ed) Ecological and Behavioural Methods for the Study of Bats (Smithsonian Institution, 1988).
Anthony, E. L. P. Age Determination in Bats. In: Kunz TH (Ed) Ecological and Behavioural Methods for the Study of Bats (Smithsonian Institution, 1988).
Ripperger, S. et al. Proximity sensors on common noctule bats reveal evidence that mothers guide juveniles to roosts but not food. Biol. Lett.15(2), (2019). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2018.0884
Sunga, J., Webber, Q. M. & Broders, H. Influence of number of individuals and observations per individual on an estimate of subgroup structure. PLoS One. 16, e0252471 (2021).
Sunga, J., Humber, J. & Broders, H. Individual variation in Parturition Timing within and among years for a Bat maternity colony. Front. Bioscience-Scholar. 15, 8 (2023).
Lučan, R. K., Weiser, M. & Hanák, V. Contrasting effects of climate change on the timing of reproduction and reproductive success of a temperate insectivorous bat. J. Zool.290, 151–159 (2013).
Willis, C. K. R., Brigham, R. M. & Geiser, F. Deep, prolonged torpor by pregnant, free-ranging bats. Naturwissenschaften. 93, 80–83 (2006).
LaZerte, S. E., Albers, S. & weathercan Download and format weather data from Environment and Climate Change Canada. J. Open. Source Softw.3, 571 (2018).
Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. R. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference (Springer, 2002). https://doi.org/10.1007/b97636
Cairns, S. J. & Schwager, S. J. A comparison of association indices. Anim. Behav.35, 1454–1469 (1987).
Whitehead, H. Analyzing Animal Societies: Quantitative Methods for Vertebrate Social Analysis (University of Chicago Press, 2008).
Farine, D. R. Animal social network inference and permutations for ecologists in R using asnipe. Methods Ecol. Evol.4, 1187–1194 (2013).
Goslee, S. C. & Urban, D. L. The ecodist package for dissimilarity-based analysis of ecological data. J. Stat. Softw.22, 1–19 (2007).
Wey, T., Blumstein, D. T., Shen, W. & Jordán, F. Social network analysis of animal behaviour: a promising tool for the study of sociality. Anim. Behav.75, 333–344 (2008).
Csardi, G. T. & Nepusz, T. The igraph software package for complex network research. InterJournal Complex. Sy, 1695(5), 1–9 (2006).
Shizuka, D. & Farine, D. R. Measuring the robustness of network community structure using assortativity. Anim. Behav.112, 237–246 (2016).
Acknowledgements
We thank Bruce Rodrigues for his involvement in data collection for this project. We thank the Hubley Family for granting access to their land and acknowledge the contributions of the many hands that were involved in data collection over the many years of monitoring, including Karissa Appleby, Nicole Besler, Lynne Burns, Mackenzie Donahue, Cody Fouts, Amanda Gallant, Alicia Irwin, Laura Johnson, Shelley Moores, Jordi Segers and Kayley Smith. We also acknowledge the important contributions of the entire Salmonier Nature Park Staff, especially Michael Blackwood, to the program and the many other Department of Fisheries, Forestry and Agriculture Staff who have participated.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
J.S. and H.B. conceptualized the study and J.S. drafted the initial manuscript and lead revisions of the manuscript while edits and ideas were contributed by J.H. and H.B. H.B. and J.H. also oversaw initial data collection.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethics declarations
Methods followed animal use protocol and standard operating procedures approved by both the animal care committee of Saint Mary’s University, Halifax, Nova Scotia (AUP number 16 − 12) and the University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario (AUP number 30066). Wildlife scientific research permits from the Government of Newfoundland and Labrador, Department of Fisheries and Land Resources, Forestry and Wildlife Branch were obtained for each year of the study. All researchers were trained to carry out methods in accordance with relevant permit guidelines and approved operating procedures and animal use protocols. All methods are reported in accordance with applicable ARRIVE guidelines.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Sunga, J., Humber, J. & Broders, H. Changes in roosting decisions and group structure following parturition in little brown myotis (Myotis lucifugus). Sci Rep 14, 22810 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-73063-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-73063-x