Abstract
Psychedelic drugs, particularly taking small amounts of psychedelics in a cyclical pattern over days (so-called microdosing), have garnered growing scientific and public interest, but representative data on different dosage levels is scarce. To better understand this trend, we surveyed a nationwide sample of 11,299 adults in Germany. The survey assessed lifetime and past six-month psychedelic use by dosage as well as socio-demographic variables (sex, age, education, employment status, household equivalence income, partner arrangements, and place of residence). Results show that 5.0% of respondents self-reported lifetime psychedelic use, while 0.7% reported past six-month use. Medium to high dosing was more prevalent than microdosing. Moreover, high probabilities of using multiple forms of psychedelics were uncovered. We also observed variation in use across socio-demographic groups. For example, psychedelics use was less likely in females than males, and older than younger respondents. Past six-month microdosing was less likely in rural areas, and past six-month medium to high dosing was less prevalent in individuals with higher income or who live with a partner. This study shows limited support for widespread use in Germany and highlights diverse usage patterns across socio-demographics. These findings can inform policies, especially considering the overlap in usage of various substances.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
A resurgence of interest in psychedelic and related substances, often referred to as a “Psychedelic Renaissance”,1 has gained momentum in recent years. After a peak in popularity in the 1950s and 1960s, empirical research ground to a halt due to prohibition, lack of production of a standard supply, among other factors,2 leaving many fundamental aspects of psychedelics unclear. Today, the field still lacks consensus on questions ranging from basic mechanisms of action to population-based usage patterns. Therefore, it is imperative that researchers across the medical and social sciences work together to characterize the current resurgence at all levels of analysis, from both clinical and non-clinical perspectives.
Researchers in the medical sciences generally agree on the relatively untapped potential that psychedelic research has for future mental health treatment (e.g., to treat depression, addiction, or post-traumatic stress disorder), including scholars in psychedelic public health, voicing concerns about equitable access to these potentially useful health-promoting tools3. This scientific hope and excitement, however, may also generate increasing interest and usage among the general public before psychedelics are approved for clinical use4,5,6,7,8,9,10. Regardless, this optimism has led governments to ease research restrictions, decriminalize and legalize psychedelics, and has led venture capital funds to invest in psychedelics research11,12,13,14. Forecasts of the growing psychedelic global drug market value predict a range from $6.82 billion USD15 to $7.2 billion USD16 in 2029.
Still, in most countries, use of psychedelics outside of clinical settings remains under strict prohibition, leading to additional challenges in conducting basic research, identifying users, and accurately estimating the prevalence of use of different substances and dosages. Some countries, such as the United States and Australia, are softening bans on select psychedelics, including psilocybin, while LSD restrictions have remained mostly untouched11. In Germany, the country in which the current study was conducted, psychedelic and related substances are mainly illegal and discussed for medical use only17. After the (regulated) legalization of cannabis in 2024, further legalization of other illicit substances (including psychedelics) is not planned. While media in Germany seem to more frequently discuss therapeutic uses of psychedelic substances,18,19 also the non-medical use for recreational reasons or enhancement purposes receives attention20,21. Therefore, it is worth investigating whether such attention finds traction in prevalence rates, supporting the expectation of psychedelics becoming a kind of “folk drug”22.
What is considered a psychedelic substance varies by research fields and includes both naturally occurring and chemically produced substances. Among them are more “classic” serotonin 2 A receptor (5-HT2AR) agonists (e.g., psilocybin, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), N,N-Dimethyltryptamine (DMT), and mescaline); mixed serotonin and dopamine reuptake inhibitors and releasers (i.e., empathogens or entactogens such as the stimulant, 3,4-Methylenedioxy methamphetamine (MDMA)); glutamate receptors antagonists (i.e., dissociative anaesthetic agents such as ketamine); and atypical psychedelics with various mechanisms of action (e.g., ibogaine or tetrahydrocannabinol (THC))23,24,25. Research examining different doses of psychedelic substances reveals a clear difference in subjective effects. While there is discussion and also disagreement about how doses could be defined,[e.g., 26,27 in this study, we divided them into two broad categories, although we acknowledge that more fine-grained categories exist[e.g., 28 (see “Limitations” section): microdoses and medium to high doses. Microdosing psychedelics involves consuming a small amount of the substance in a cyclical pattern over periods of days29. Some scholars define this amount to be about one-tenth to one-twentieth of recreational doses, depending on the substance28. The acute subjective effects experienced by users range from being subtle to imperceptible; the desired benefits of this form of psychedelic consumption tend to revolve around overall boosts in mood, concentration, creativity, among others30,31,32,33. Medium to high doses of psychedelics lead to noticeable and, at times, profound subjective effects. Users of these higher doses tend to seek the experience of alterations in mood and perception, including hallucinations, along with mystical or spiritual experiences34.
While generally considered safe substances, psychological (for example anxiety, paranoia, suicidality) as well as physical side effects (such as hypertension and cardiovascular effects or, even more rarely, death) have been reported30,35,36. Medical risks of classic psychedelics are often minimal; one study compared the need for medical help in relation to substances used and deemed psilocybin among the safest37. A meta-analysis found that side effects such as increased heart rate and blood pressure as well as feelings of nausea and anxiety were common during the acute phase of psychedelic administration, but subsided within hours38.. This analysis focused on clinical studies that employed ayahuasca, LSD and psilocybin. Research on use of these classic psychedelics outside clinical settings has found similar results. Substances such as MDMA and ketamine have very different risk profiles, and therefore the risks and adverse effects of these may not be considered as safe, even if they are sometimes used in similar ways as classic psychedelics. For example, the combination of esketamine nasal spray and antidepressants has been shown to lead to clinical benefits for those with treatment-resistant depression, compared to antidepressants, alone39. Side effects, such as feelings of dissociation and dizziness, were transient and typically subsided within approximately two hours of esketamine administration. Use of ketamine outside clinical settings is associated with multiple risks, such as paranoia, panic, psychosis, cardiorespiratory problems especially when used in combination with other drugs, and fatal overdoses40. Similar risks are described with MDMA, particularly in high doses, such as anxiety and panic attacks, seizures, arrhythmia and hyperthermia, sexual risk-taking, damaged brain functions or death (although some deaths may be due to drug interactions, hypothermia, or other, related factors, rather than an overdose)41.
Identifying current patterns of use is a good first step to better characterize this possible health concern. Much of the older research generally recruits self-identified users to uncover their usage patterns. Current prevalence data often uses self-selection sampling,42 leaving representative data scarce. Also, existing studies often do not distinguish between microdosing and medium to high dosing,43 which are very different usage patterns with different risk profiles. Yet, identifying human usage patterns is one critical but under-addressed avenue of research. Especially detailing the specific substances used and under what dosing regimens, for example, contrasting medium to high doses with microdosing, are essential questions when characterizing population-wide usage patterns. Because the category of psychedelics is contested and thus unclear, asking about different substances separately but in the same study may reveal new aspects. Moreover, demographic analyses have been conducted within populations who use,29 while general demographics are also needed.
Current research in Germany is beginning to paint a picture of the general characteristics of today’s users. A recent representative study found a 3.4% lifetime and 0.6% 12-month prevalence for LSD, a 4.5% lifetime and 0.5% 12-month prevalence for hallucinogenic mushrooms and a 4.0% lifetime and 1.3% 12-month prevalence for new psychoactive substances among German adults (18–64 years)44. However, this study was conducted during the COVID pandemic, a time frame that might not be representative of non-pandemic situations45. In 2018, the respective prevalences were 2.1% for lifetime and 0.3% for past 12-month use of LSD, 3.0% for lifetime and 0.4% for past 12-month use of hallucinogenic mushrooms and 2.6% for lifetime and 0.9% for past 12-month use of new psychoactive substances46. While this study provides important insights, it does not differentiate between medium to high doses and microdosing.
Therefore, the aim of this article is to contribute towards characterizing the current patterns of psychedelic use by adults in Germany, using population-based data, focusing on multiple substances that fall under the category of psychedelics as well as on different dosage-types.
Methods
Sample
This study used data from the nationwide web-based ENHANCE-study with adults (18 years and older) in Germany. Recruitment of the initial sample of this four-wave study took place offline and targeted a nationally representative group, regarding sex, age, education, and federal state for the adult residential population having Internet access (applying to 92% of all households)47. We used data of wave four (conducted between October and December, 2022) since only here questions related to psychedelics have been assessed. In this wave, 28,567 individuals were invited to participate in this voluntary and pseudonymized survey, in which the researchers had no access to the respondents’ personal data. We also invited individuals who declined their participation in waves two and/or three as well as a refreshment sample of 10,104 individuals to compensate for demographic imbalances due to selective participation of hard-to-reach participants. In total, 13,452 individuals consented to participate, while 12,168 completed the survey and received bonus points (worth approximately $3.00) as an incentive. These points could be exchanged for vouchers, charity lottery tickets, or donations to UNICEF. The analytic sample included 11,299 individuals who responded to all variables used (49.1% femaleweighted, mean ageweighted: 48.93 years; see Table 1 for all descriptive statistics). This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Erfurt (reference number: EV-20220830). Our work aligns with the Code of Ethics of the American Sociological Association (ASA) and although our study is not a medical study, we adhere to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) to protect human research participants.
Measures
Self-reported use of psychedelics. Respondents were asked to self-report their prior use of psychedelics, including LSD (also labeled “acid” in the questionnaire) or LSD analogues (e.g., 1P-LSD or ETH-LAD), psilocybin (“magic mushrooms”), as well as other psychedelics (e.g., DMT and ayahuasca) and substances that can be used in similar ways (e.g., ketamine and MDMA). We explained to the respondents that these drugs are taken in varying doses. With a medium to high dose, they can cause perceptual changes (e.g., seeing or hearing things in a new way) or hallucinations (e.g., seeing or hearing things that are not present in the environment). With microdosing—that is, a very small dose—some people report subtle changes in their perception over a long period of intermittent use, whereas others detect no noticeable changes. We asked if respondents had ever taken (1) LSD (“acid”) or LSD analogues (e.g. 1P-LSD or ETH-LAD), (2) Psilocybin (“magic mushrooms”), and (3) Other psychedelic drugs (e.g. DMT, ketamine, MDMA, ayahuasca) “in medium to high dose” or “in microdose”. Response options were “no, never”, “yes, more than 6 months ago”, “yes, in the last 6 months but not in the last 30 days”, and “yes, in the last 30 days”. Also “no response” was possible (chosen by 0.8-0.9% of respondents per item). Respondents indicating other psychedelic drug use received a follow-up open-ended question to indicate which drug(s) they had been using “in medium to high dose” or “in microdose”. Based on these questions, six dichotomous variables have been created indicating (a) lifetime and (b) past-six months use of any of these substances, irrespective of the dose, (c) lifetime and (d) past six-month microdosing, as well as (e) lifetime and (f) past six-month medium to high dosing (each time coded as “no” [0] and “yes” [1]).
Sociodemographic characteristics. We assessed the respondents’ sex, age, education, employment status,[cf., 48 and whether they had a partner living in their household (see Table 1). The estimated equivalence household net income was measured with an open-ended question. If no answer was provided, we showed income categories[cf., 48 and used the mean value of these categories, while for the last open-ended category (“20,000 and more”), the value 20,000 was used49. We computed the equivalence income by considering the number of household members with the OECD-modified scale50 for which the first adult received a weight of 1, each additional adult received a weight of 0.5, and each child (under 14 years) received a weight of 0.3. This weight was used as a divisor for the income. Moreover, place of residence was assessed. Therefore, we categorized individuals as living either in a rural or urban settlement structure51, based on their postal codes.
Pretesting
We conducted qualitative cognitive pretests (N = 10) with a think-aloud approach and probing questions52 as well as a quantitative pretest (N = 205) to examine and improve the clarity and accuracy of all measures. The findings of these pretests suggest that the materials used were clear, understandable, and suitable for the nationwide sample.
Statistical analysis
To examine overlaps of patterns of psychedelic use, we computed percentages and Pearson´s correlations (identical to Phi for bivariate measures) for which r > ± 0.2 indicates small effects, r > ± 0.5 medium effects, and r > ± 0.8 large effects53. We used logistic regression models54 to examine the differences in the self-reported prevalence across sociodemographic characteristics. For categorical variables with more than two groups (e.g., age), we rotated the reference group to enable a comparison of all groups (see Tables S1-6, Supplementary Material). We present unadjusted odds ratios (ORs) to indicate the magnitude of bivariate associations between the prevalence measures and each sociodemographic group as well as adjusted ORs, which control for all other variables. An OR ≥ 1.50 (≤ 0.67) indicates a small effect, an OR ≥ 2 (≤ 0.5) a medium effect, and an OR ≥ 3 (≤ 0.33) a large effect53. The ORs are presented along with their 95% confidence intervals. Across all analyses, we use sampling weights to increase the representativity of the sample (see Table 1). The weights are computed with iterative proportional fitting55 on the basis of sex, age, education, and federal state.
Results
Prevalence of use: substances and doses
Panel A in Fig. 1 shows that 5.0% of respondents self-reported any psychedelic use during their lifetime, irrespective of dosing, while 0.7% did so for the past six months. More specifically, roughly 3% used LSD or LSD analogs, psilocybin, or other substances in their lifetime and half a percent during the past six months. Panels B and C show that use of medium to high doses seems more prevalent in Germany than microdosing across all categories.
To investigate the overlap in use of different psychedelics across dosing and time of use, the upper sections of Table 2 (lifetime) and Table 3 (past six-month) provide percentages of overlaps, and the lower sections provide correlation coefficients between the measures. The results suggest that 58.7% of lifetime users of LSD (any dose) also used psilocybin (any dose) during their lifetime, while 55.7% of lifetime users of psilocybin (any dose) also engaged in LSD (any dose) during their lifetime. Of all past six-month medium to high LSD users, 67.8% also engaged in medium to high dosing of psilocybin within this period, while only 55.2% of past six-month users of psilocybin (medium to high dosing) also used LSD (medium to high dosing) in the respective period.
The correlations imply medium-sized positive associations between use of any dose of LSD, psilocybin, and other classic psychedelic substances (for both lifetime and past six-month use). For microdosing and medium to high dosing (lifetime and past six-month use), these associations are of small to medium size. The results further suggest associations of small to medium size between the uses of two doses, i.e., microdose and medium to high dose, for LSD, psilocybin, and other psychedelic substances across the lifetime. For the past six-month use, two associations of medium size were also uncovered, namely, for LSD microdosing and medium to high dosing as well as for psilocybin microdosing and medium to high dosing.
Prevalence of use: demographic groups
Gender
Females were less likely to report the use of psychedelics across all indicators (see Tables 4 and 5). Bivariate effects were small in size for lifetime use of any dose, as well as for lifetime and past six-month microdosing, medium for past six-month use irrespective of dose, and for lifetime medium to high dosing, as well as close to large for past six-month medium to high dosing. After adjustment, most effects minimally increased.
Age
The bivariate analyses suggested higher prevalence rates among the two youngest age groups across all indicators and decreasing use with age (see also Tables S1-S6, Supplementary Material, for further comparisons of the other age categories not highlighted in the main text). The older age groups showed markedly lower prevalence rates; effect sizes were oftentimes large for comparisons against younger cohorts. After adjustment, these patterns remained relatively stable. There were exceptions, however; for example, the age group 30–39 years as compared to age group 18–29 years showed a lower past six-month microdosing with a medium effect size before adjustment, which vanished after adjustment. Moreover, several effects decreased in size, notably for the past six-month indicators.
Education
In the bivariate analyses, respondents with higher levels of education were more likely to report the use of psychedelics across all use indicators. While the effect sizes for all lifetime indicators were small, they were medium for all past six-month indicators. However, none of the effects remained substantial in the adjusted models.
Employment status
In the bivariate models, the lifetime and past six-month prevalence of use across all measures was lowest among those not employed compared to all other groups (see also Tables S1-S6, Supplementary Material). The effect sizes were mainly medium to large. Compared to full-time employment, the past six-month prevalence was higher among part-time employees and respondents in education with small to medium effect sizes. In the adjusted models, several bivariate effects decreased in size and partially turned to non-substantial effect sizes. Remaining substantial differences were, for example, the higher likelihood of reporting a six-month prevalence of any dosing, microdosing, and medium to high dosing for part-time employment compared to full-time, in education, and not employed, with small to large effects. In all analyses for lifetime use, effects turned non-substantial; only respondents in education were less likely to report medium to high dosing as compared to part-time and not employed respondents.
Equivalence income
Bivariate differences were non-substantial for all lifetime indicators across all three income groups (see also Tables S1-S6, Supplementary Material). However, for all past six-month use indicators, individuals in the low-income group were more likely to report any psychedelic use than those in the medium- and high-income group. The effects were mostly medium (with the exception of the comparison to the high-income group for microdosing, which slightly fell below the level of small effects). The medium and high-income group seemed to not substantially differ across all indicators. After adjustment, many effects for differences with the low-income group decreased and became non-substantial. However, few effects were still small in size, namely, the comparisons between the low- and high-income group for any dosing and for medium to high dosing in the past six months. Moreover, the comparison between high- and medium-income groups for medium to high dose, past six-month use showed a small effect size, with use less likely to be reported in the high-income group.
Partner
While there were no substantial bivariate differences in any of the lifetime indicators between people with and without partners in their households, those reporting to have such a partner were substantially less likely to report use across all indicators for past six-month use. These bivariate effects were large, but partially decreased to medium in adjusted models.
Place of residence
Generally, respondents from rural areas were less likely to report the use of psychedelics across all indicators. However, the bivariate differences were non-substantial, with the exception of past six-month use of any dose and microdosing. After adjustment, these effects slightly decreased.
Sensitivity analysis
As a sensitivity analysis, we repeated the multivariate regression models by controlling for anonymity perceptions of the survey56 given the potentially sensitive nature of the topic. While most results remained stable (see Tables S7-S10, Supplementary Material), it is worth noting that for any dosing (past six-month), the part-time effect changed from small to medium and the high income effect changed from small to non-substantial; for microdosing (lifetime), the effect of the age group 30–39 changed from small to non-substantial; for microdosing (past six-month), the effect of the age group 40–49 (compared to 50–59) changed from large to medium, the high income effect turned to a small effect from a non-substantial effect; and for medium to high dosing (past six-month), the high income effects changed from small to non-substantial (compared to both low and medium income).
Discussion
Our population-level data point to generally low usage rates of psychedelics in Germany. The rates of lifetime use of LSD (2.8%) and psilocybin (2.9%) in our sample are substantially lower than the rates reported by German respondents in the 2020 Global Drug Survey,42 wherein, 15.7% reported using LSD, and 18.1% reported using psilocybin within the past 12 months. It should be noted, however, that this Global Drug Survey was self-selective in that the target population consists of active drug users invited via media partners (e.g., newspapers and social media), making the data non-representative of any country’s general population57. Consequently, higher prevalence rates of psychedelics are expected among this sample in contrast to a probability-sample of the population. Compared to other studies in Germany using a representative population sample, our six-month prevalence of 0.4% for both LSD and psilocybin is very similar to the 12-month prevalence of 0.6% (2018: 0.3%) for LSD and 0.5% (2018: 0.4%) for hallucinogenic mushrooms found in 202146,58. With regard to lifetime use of LSD, our prevalence of 2.8% is higher than the 2.1% reported by Seitz et al.46 but lower than the 3.4% found by Rauschert et al.44. For psilocybin, our sample showed a lifetime prevalence of 2.9%, which aligns closely with Seitz et al.’s46 finding of 3.0% but is notably lower than Rauschert et al.’s44 reported 4.5%. Overall, the pronounced discrepancy in usage rates between user-samples and representative or probability samples highlights the influence of recruitment methods and their unique role in population-based data collection.
Our data suggest that there are fewer users reporting self-administration of microdoses compared to medium to high doses. While the reason for this difference in prevalence should be further analyzed in future research, it might be that the concept of microdosing is a relatively new phenomenon, whereas taking higher doses is historically more common. The increased interest in microdosing in recent years might be a result of new societal challenges, such as digitalization, flexibilization of the labor market, and the increased prevalence of working environments like the home office that may combine work with childcare;59,60 these challenges might increase the demand for using microdoses of psychedelics as cognitive enhancers and to cope with stressors61,62. This would parallel self-reported reasons for use, which commonly revolve around expansion of awareness, coping, as well as enhancement63. Also, one international study found the most common reason for microdosing was performance enhancement,30 although it is worth noting that a recent double-blind, placebo-controlled study found no evidence for enhancement in participants administered microdoses over the course of two sessions64. Furthermore, microdosing psychedelics may be viewed as a phenomenon distinct enough from medium to high dosing to warrant a change in the way society sees psychedelics. Perhaps the lack of a significant perceptual change at the time of consumption will shift public opinion away from focusing on the use of psychedelics to merely get high and associated moral condemnation and stigma towards more positively viewed use for therapeutic or enhancement purposes.
The medium-sized prevalence of overlap in use of various psychedelics point to users being open to multiple substances and doses, rather than selectively using a single substance at a single dose. This finding is in line with a previous report of increased odds of medium to high dose psychedelic use in microdosers compared to non-microdosers29. Another study found that in their sample of users who had previously used a medium to high dose, almost 80% also reported microdosing30. These findings do not support the idea of a new generation of psychedelic users focused exclusively on microdosing for enhancement. In fact, a recent Danish study found that users of medium to large doses sometimes used microdoses in between the larger doses as a supplementary form of treatment and did not necessarily stay with the same substance33. A study using data from the Global Psychedelic Survey also concluded that co-using substances (e.g., alcohol, cannabis, stimulants, etc.) along with psychedelics is quite common, with other psychedelics being used in about half of the co-using participants65. Similarly, our data indicate that over half of LSD users of any dose also reported use of psilocybin, while over half of psilocybin users also reported using LSD. This finding should be considered when regulations of substances are being discussed, with current trends of decriminalization or legalization focusing slightly more on psilocybin than LSD11. These two substances could be used for similar purposes, although future research should investigate the reasons behind the use of specific psychedelics at specific doses.
Our findings also point to usage of psychedelics as a phenomenon that may be more prevalent in some population groups or communities (see Table S11, Supplementary Material, for a visual summary). The most robust demographic differences in the multivariate models relate to age and gender, such that, for most analyses, females and older respondents are less likely to have used psychedelics compared to males and 18- to 29-year-olds. Such patterns have often been observed in other studies for different substances and in different countries44,46,66,67,68,69,70. However, some of these studies rely on bivariate analysis (this restriction, along with the fact that effects are not always consistent across all investigated substances, also applies to several comparisons of use patterns for other demographics reported below). In our study, the age pattern is consistent: the effects are, on average, large, since we hardly observe psychedelic use in middle-aged to old respondents. While some of the age effects are medium in size, the effects of gender on consumption are small to large. The underlying mechanisms for the effects of gender and age might be similar. Prior studies on other forms of substance use, for example on (underage) drinking,71,72 cannabis use,73 or the nonmedical use of prescription drugs,74 have revealed that self-control is an important predictor of such behavior. Other work has indicated that males have, on average, lower capabilities to exercise self-control (including to be more risk-prone) and therefore show more criminal or antisocial behavior75. Males with exposure to pharmacological substances have also been found to have higher rates of substance use disorders compared to females with similar levels of substance exposure76. Self-control is also related to age, whereby exercising self-control increases with age77. Also sensation-seeking, a predictor for drug use, might be related to age78. It has further been argued that some measures of self-control explain the correlation between gender, age, and deviant behavior79. Moreover, those seeking to improve their lives through psychedelics might either seek to enhance their self-control or use psychedelics as a form of self-control in relation to health behavior change80.
While another study did not find a statistically significant association between employment and psychedelics use,81 our multivariate analysis indicated that respondents employed part-time had a higher probability to report a higher six-month prevalence rate for all three measures. On the one hand, part-time employment might be associated with more microdosing because the pressure to perform well in fixed-term and not well-paid employment contracts is higher compared to permanent contracts. On the other hand, part-time employment results in more spare time and less opportunity costs for medium to high dose and recreational drug use. A recent U.S. study found being uninsured was related to higher usage rates of psychedelics69. As health insurance is typically linked to one’s employer in the U.S., this could perhaps be considered another measure of secure employment. The authors also noted that these data could represent users who are particularly motivated by self-medication, lacking affordable access to the healthcare system.
A high compared to low household income was associated with a lower probability of a six-month prevalence for any dosage, an effect also seen when isolating medium to large doses (here the prevalence was also lower for high compared to medium income), but not with microdoses. While some other studies also did not observe significant differences in the odds of using certain psychedelics,70 some similarities to our observed effects were found in recent research, reporting that low income was correlated, for example, with LSD and hallucinogen use66,67,82. A possible interpretation is that respondents living in low income households have more stress because of the challenges of relative poverty and accordingly less alternative resources to cope with stress. Another possible interpretation could be that psychedelics are a less expensive solution than conventional therapy, and therefore more attractive to those with less income33. Perhaps, therefore, they present a greater demand for microdoses of psychedelics as cognitive enhancers. However, in the sensitivity analysis, the small income effects slightly decreased in size and turned to non-substantial effects, while, interestingly, past six-month microdosing appeared more likely in the highest compared to the lowest income group. Thus, the findings should be interpreted with caution. Future work on reasons for use and willingness to report use could help explore this relationship.
While we found some effects of education in the bivariate analyses, such that higher education levels were associated with increased rates of psychedelic use,[cf., 66,67,70 these effects do not remain substantial in the multivariate analysis. It is possible that the strong effect of age, with younger respondents generally more likely to have used, could explain this relationship. However, future work should continue to investigate the relationship between education level and psychedelic use. A U.S. study on psychedelic users reported higher accuracy in knowledge about the substances in those with higher levels of education83. This study did not address usage rates, as it focused on psychedelic users, but did uncover some potential effects of education on factors related to use.
Respondents living in a relationship had a lower probability to consume psychedelics in the past six months and for microdosing, in particular,[cf., 67 where the effects were large in the multivariate analysis. The effects of partnership on lifetime prevalence were non-substantial. Similar to income, it might be that being in a partnership provides other resources to cope with stress84,85. It has further been suggested that a partner enhances socialization and so deters risky and illegal behavior86,87. Finally, according to the turning point approach88, engaging in a stable partnership or marital cohesion can be described as a crucial turning point in life, changing the opportunity structure and increasing the opportunity costs of deviant and illegal activities in general.
Moreover, living in rural areas of Germany decreased the probability of using psychedelics only for microdosing within the past six months;[cf., 67 all other effects of place of residence are non-substantial in the multivariate models. While previous research has suggested a general effect of the urban environment supporting greater access to drugs89, our findings suggest a relatively selective increase in use. Perhaps the urban environment allows greater access to the specific venues and groups of individuals who are interested in drug use90, in this case, a newer form of psychedelics use.
Limitations and future research
One limitation of this study is the use of self-report measures since they are prone to social desirability bias. However, the use of web-based surveys may reduce such bias91 since they provide more anonymity92. Moreover, only very few respondents used the response option “no response” (at most 0.9% for any item), although this selection frequency is similar to, or even greater than other selections (e.g., prevalence of past six-month LSD use). Drop-out on this page was very low (N = 10, 0.08%). In addition, sensitivity analyses which control for anonymity perceptions of the survey56 led only to minimal changes of the results. These results thus suggest that social desirability bias might not be severe.
While this study differentiates between microdosing and medium to high dosing as two distinct categories, future research may use more nuanced measures with more categories27,28. However, until full legalization and therefore accurate and measurable titration of psychedelic dose, it might be difficult for users outside of clinical settings to know which exact dose they actually took or should take. Therefore, future studies may also ask about use frequency93 (e.g., daily vs. irregular trips), estimates of specific dosage amounts, intended dose, or types of psychedelic experience (e.g., subperceptual vs. an actual high).
The analysis concerning the socio-demographic variation in the past six-month prevalence needs to be interpreted with caution given the partially very low number of psychedelic users, especially for microdosing. Although our sample size can be considered large, future studies may use even larger samples to replicate and expand upon the findings. Moreover, we present cross-sectional data in one country, while future studies should also use longitudinal designs in different socio-cultural contexts to examine similarities and differences in the pattern found here. Also, the influence of the recent legalization of cannabis in Germany on the use of other substances might be worth investigating94.
Given the evidence that younger individuals engage in psychedelic use, it is also important to examine possible socio-demographic associations with such use. Future studies should include youth (under the age of 18), who may show different patterns of use than adults. These findings may have implications for educational interventions tailored towards pediatric and young adult populations.
Intensive studies have identified myriad reasons for use, including recreational, spiritual, therapeutic, and self-enhancement6. To better understand these patterns of desired outcomes as well as the actual vs. desired effects (including whether incorrect microdosing may result in unpleasant full-blown trips),59 population-level details regarding use are critical. Questions remain regarding psychedelics’ efficacy for the range of outcomes people anticipate when they use the substances.
Conclusion
Despite increasing research interest in psychedelics and potential therapeutic applications in controlled settings, in the German context, psychedelic use seems currently relatively low. Still, we encourage further monitoring of potential use trends to understand if factors such as research interest and more therapeutic uses together with increasing media attention also increase the prevalence in the future. Moreover, our analyses uncovered high probabilities of using multiple forms of psychedelics as well as variation in use across socio-demographic groups. In sum, these findings provide foundational information, key to understanding the state and pattern of current use of psychedelics, that should be considered when developing policies and interventions for psychedelic use. The great deal of overlap found in users of psilocybin and LSD suggests that both substances should be given consideration when deciding upon new potential regulations, rather than excluding LSD from the discussion.
Data availability
The data analyzed during the current study are available at the repository of Bielefeld University95.
References
Sessa, B. The 21st century psychedelic renaissance: heroic steps forward on the back of an elephant. Psychopharmacology 235, 551–560 (2018).
Hall, W. Why was early therapeutic research on psychedelic drugs abandoned? Psychol. Med. 52, 26–31 (2022).
Kuiper, H. et al. Psychedelic public health: state of the field and implications for equity. Soc. Sci. Med. 357, 1–11 (2024).
Carhart-Harris, R. L. & Goodwin, G. M. The therapeutic potential of psychedelic drugs: past, present, and future. Neuropsychopharmacol 42, 2105–2113 (2017).
Hadar, A. et al. The psychedelic renaissance in clinical research: A bibliometric analysis of three decades of human studies with psychedelics. J. Psychoactive Drugs. 55, 1–10 (2023).
Holm, S., Petersen, M. A., Enghoff, O. & Hesse, M. Psychedelic discourses: A qualitative study of discussions in a Danish online forum. Int. J. Drug Policy. 112, 1–9 (2023).
Polito, V. & Stevenson, R. J. A systematic study of microdosing psychedelics. PLoS ONE. 14, e0211023 (2019).
Sandison, R. A., Spencer, A. M. & Whitelaw, J. D. A. The therapeutic value of lysergic acid diethylamide in mental illness. J. Ment Sci. 100, 491–507 (1954).
Walsh, C. A., Gorfinkel, L., Shmulewitz, D., Stohl, M. & Hasin, D. S. Use of lysergic acid diethylamide by major depression status. JAMA Psychiatry. 81, 89 (2024).
Yang, K. H., Satybaldiyeva, N., Allen, M. R., Ayers, J. W. & Leas, E. C. State Cannabis and psychedelic legislation and microdosing interest in the US. JAMA Health Forum. 5, e241653 (2024).
Chesak, J. What psychedelics legalisation and decriminalisation looks like around the world. BBC (2024). https://www.bbc.com/future/article/20240320-legal-status-of-psychedelics-around-the-world
Aday, J. S., Bloesch, E. K. & Davoli, C. C. : A year of expansion in psychedelic research, industry, and deregulation. Drug Science, Policy and Law 6, 2050324520974484 (2020). (2019).
Phelps, J., Shah, R. N. & Lieberman, J. A. The rapid rise in investment in Psychedelics - Cart before the horse. JAMA Psychiatry. 79, 189–190 (2022).
Siegel, J. S., Daily, J. E., Perry, D. A. & Nicol, G. E. Psychedelic drug legislative reform and legalization in the US. JAMA Psychiatry. 80, 77 (2023).
Mordor Intelligence. Psychedelic Drugs Market Size & Share Analysis - Growth Trends & Forecasts (2024–2029). (2024). https://www.mordorintelligence.com/industry-reports/psychedelic-drugs-market
Adams, B. These three drugs could make up over half of forecasted psychedelic sales by 2029. Forbes (2023). https://www.forbes.com/sites/benjaminadams/2023/09/29/these-three-drugs-could-make-up-over-half-of-forecasted-psychedelic-sales-by-2029/
Zullino, D. Die Psychedelische Renaissance - Psychiatrie. Universimed - Medizin Im Fokus. (2024). https://www.universimed.com/ch/article/psychiatrie/die-renaissance-176374
Gavarini, L. Psychedelika in der Psychotheraphie. Den Hype erforschen. taz (2023). https://taz.de/Psychedelika-in-der-Psychotherapie/!5960391/
Penzel, V. M. Forscher wollen medizinische Nutzung von Psychedelika vorbereiten. Frankfurter Allgemeine (2024). https://www.faz.net/aktuell/wissen/medizin-ernaehrung/ecstasy-und-lsd-als-medikamente-forscher-wollen-erprobung-vorbereiten-19483285.html
Köckritz, A. Psychedelische Drogen: Ist das gut für die Seele? Zeit Online (2023). https://www.zeit.de/2023/16/psychedelische-drogen-forschung-retreat-psilocybin
von Wilmsdorff, J. Jenke. Experiment. Alltagsdrogen: Besser, schneller, schlauer – wie gedopt ist Deutschland? ProSieben (2024). https://www.prosieben.de/serien/jenke/news/jenke-experiment-alltagsdrogen-besser-schneller-schlauer-wie-gedopt-ist-deutschland-am21-oktober-auf-prosieben-440191
Ehlers, M. LSD und MDMA feiern Comeback in Therapie, Coaching und Training. Focus Online (2024). https://www.focus.de/experts/experte-sieht-welle-lsd-und-mdma-feiern-comeback-in-therapie-coaching-und-training_id_219540493.html
Garcia-Romeu, A., Kersgaard, B. & Addy, P. H. Clinical applications of hallucinogens: A review. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 24, 229–268 (2016).
Morris, H. & Wallach, J. From PCP to MXE: a comprehensive review of the non-medical use of dissociative drugs. Drug. Test. Anal. 6, 614–632 (2014).
Nichols, D. E. Differences between the mechanism of action of MDMA, MBDB, and the classic hallucinogens. Identification of a new therapeutic class: entactogens. J. Psychoactive Drugs. 18, 305–313 (1986).
Freeman, T. P., Lorenzetti, V. & ‘Standard THC units’: a proposal to standardize dose across all cannabis products and methods of administration. Addiction 115, 1207–1216 (2020).
Liechti, M. E. & Holze, F. Dosing psychedelics and MDMA. in Disruptive Psychopharmacology (eds Barrett, F. S. & Preller, K. H.) vol. 56 3–21 (Springer International Publishing, Cham, (2021).
Kuypers, K. P. et al. Microdosing psychedelics: more questions than answers? An overview and suggestions for future research. J. Psychopharmacol. 33, 1039–1057 (2019).
Rosenbaum, D. et al. Microdosing psychedelics: demographics, practices, and psychiatric comorbidities. J. Psychopharmacol. 34, 612–622 (2020).
Hutten, N. R. P. W., Mason, N. L., Dolder, P. C. & Kuypers, K. P. C. Motives and Side-Effects of microdosing with psychedelics among users. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 22, 426–434 (2019).
Johnstad, P. G. Powerful substances in tiny amounts: an interview study of psychedelic microdosing. Nordic Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 35, 39–51 (2018).
Lea, T., Amada, N., Jungaberle, H., Schecke, H. & Klein, M. Microdosing psychedelics: motivations, subjective effects and harm reduction. Int. J. Drug Policy. 75, 1–9 (2020).
Petersen, M. A., Smith, A., Kristensen, D. B. & Hoeyer, K. Subject in the making: technologies of the self and aspirations for a good life in contemporary Denmark. Med. Anthropol. 41, 431–445 (2022).
Nichols, D. E. & Psychedelics Pharmacol. Rev. 68, 264–355 (2016).
Bremler, R., Katati, N., Shergill, P., Erritzoe, D. & Carhart-Harris, R. L. Case analysis of long-term negative psychological responses to psychedelics. Sci. Rep. 13, 15998 (2023).
Schlag, A. K., Aday, J., Salam, I., Neill, J. C. & Nutt, D. J. Adverse effects of psychedelics: from anecdotes and misinformation to systematic science. J. Psychopharmacol. 36, 258–272 (2022).
Winstock, A. Global drug survey. GSD2019 Key findigs report. GDS Core Res. Team (2019). https://www.drugsandalcohol.ie/30537/1/Exec-Summary.pdf
Romeo, B. et al. Safety and risk assessment of psychedelic psychotherapy: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Psychiatry Res. 335, 115880 (2024).
Katz, E. G. et al. Benefit–Risk assessment of Esketamine nasal spray vs. Placebo in Treatment-Resistant depression. Clin. Pharma Ther. 109, 536–546 (2021).
Wolff, K., Winstock, A. R. & Ketamine From medicine to misuse. CNS Drugs. 20, 199–218 (2006).
Rigg, K. K. & Sharp, A. Deaths related to MDMA (ecstasy/molly): prevalence, root causes, and harm reduction interventions. J. Subst. Use. 23, 345–352 (2018).
Winstock, A. R. et al. Global Drug Survey (GDS) 2020 Psychedelics Key Findings Report. (2021). https://www.globaldrugsurvey.com/gds-2020-psychedelics-report/
Jahn, Z. W., Lopez, J., de la Salle, S., Faber, S. & Williams, M. T. Racial/ethnic differences in prevalence of hallucinogen use by age cohort: Findings from the 2018 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. JPS 69–82 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1556/2054.2021.00166
Rauschert, C. et al. Kurzbericht epidemiologischer Suchtsurvey 2021. Tabellenband: trends der Prävalenz des (problematischen) Konsums illegaler Drogen Nach geschlecht und alter 1990–2021. IFT Inst. Für Therapieforschung (2023). https://www.esa-survey.de/fileadmin/user_upload/Literatur/Berichte/ESA_2021_Tabellen_illegale_Drogen.pdf
Kuitunen-Paul, S., Schwarzbach, C. P. & Atzendorf, J. Trends in der verbreitung des Konsums psychoaktiver substanzen in Deutschland: Ein deskriptives review von bevölkerungsstudien Vor, während und Nach der COVID-19 pandemie. Suchttherapie 25, 92–98 (2024).
Seitz, N. N., John, L., Atzendorf, J., Rauschert, C. & Kraus, L. Kurzbericht Epidemiologischer Suchtsurvey 2018. Tabellenband: Konsum Illegaler Drogen, Multiple Drogenerfahrung Und Hinweise Auf Konsumabhängigkeit Und -missbrauch Nach Geschlecht Und Alter Im Jahr 2018 (IFT Institut für Therapieforschung, 2019).
Statista Anteil Der Haushalte in Deutschland Mit Internetzugang von 2002 Bis 2022 [Proportion of Households in Germany with Internet Access]. (2023). https://de.statista.com/statistik/daten/studie/153257/umfrage/haushalte-mit-internetzugang-in-deutschland-seit-2002/.
Beckmann, K. et al. Demographische Standards. Statistik und Wissenschaft 17 (Statisiches Bundesamt, Wiesbaden, 2016).
Celeste, R. K. & Bastos, J. L. Mid-point for open-ended income category and the effect of equivalence scales on the income-health relationship. Rev. Saude Publica. 47, 168–174 (2013).
Hagenaars, A. J., De Vos, K. & Asghar Zaidi, M. Poverty Statistics in the Late 1980s: Research Based on Micro-Data. (1994).
Federal Institute for Research on Building; Urban Affairs and Spatial Development. Referenz Gemeinden, Kreise, NUTS. (2017).
Van Someren, M. W., Barnard, Y. F. & Sandberg, J. A. The Think Aloud Method: A Practical Approach to Modelling Cognitive Processes. (Academic Press, London, 1994).
Sullivan, G. M. & Feinn, R. Using effect Size—or why the P value is not enough. J. Graduate Med. Educ. 4, 279–282 (2012).
Long, J. S. & Freese, J. Regression Models for Categorical Dependent Variables Using Stata (3rd Edition Ed.). (Stata Press, College Station, 2014).
Kolenikov, S. Calibrating survey data using iterative proportional fitting (raking). Stata J. 14, 22–59 (2014).
Patrzek, J., Sattler, S., van Veen, F., Grunschel, C. & Fries, S. Investigating the effect of academic procrastination on the frequency and variety of academic misconduct: A panel study. Stud. High. Educ. 40, 1014–1029 (2015). https:/doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2013.854765
Barratt, M. J. et al. Moving on From Representativeness: Testing the Utility of the Global Drug Survey. Subst-Abuse 11, 1–17 (2017).
Rauschert, C. et al. The use of psychoactive substances in Germany. Deutsches Ärzteblatt Int. https://doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.m2022.0244 (2022).
Brühl, A. B., d’Angelo, C. & Sahakian, B. J. Neuroethical issues in cognitive enhancement: modafinil as the example of a workplace drug? Brain Neurosci. Adv. 3, 1–8 (2019).
Brühl, A. B. & Sahakian, B. J. Drugs, games, and devices for enhancing cognition: implications for work and society: drugs, games, and devices for enhancing cognition. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1369, 195–217 (2016).
Bagusat, C. et al. Pharmacological neuroenhancement and the ability to recover from stress – a representative cross-sectional survey among the German population. Subst Abuse Treat. Prev. Policy 13, 37 (2018).
Baum, M., Sattler, S. & Reimann, M. Towards an Understanding of how stress and resources affect the nonmedical use of prescription drugs for performance enhancement among employees. Curr. Psychol. 42, 4784–4801 (2023). https:/doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01873-7
Basedow, L. A. & Kuitunen-Paul, S. Motives for the use of serotonergic psychedelics: A systematic review. Drug Alcohol Rev. 41, 1391–1403 (2022).
Cavanna, F. et al. Microdosing with psilocybin mushrooms: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. Transl Psychiatry. 12, 1–11 (2022).
Lake, S. & Lucas, P. Co-use of psychedelics with other substances: findings from the global psychedelic survey. J. Psychopharmacol. 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/02698811241292956 (2024).
Davis, A. K. et al. Race, ethnic, and sex differences in prevalence of and trends in hallucinogen consumption among lifetime users in the united States between 2015 and 2019. Front. Epidemiol. 2, 876706 (2022).
Killion, B. et al. LSD Use in the united States: trends, correlates, and a typology of Us. Drug Alcohol Depend. 223, 108715 (2021).
Kňažek, F. et al. Typology of users of psychedelics and alike psychoactive substances in Czechia: results of the National online survey. J. Psychoactive Drugs. 55, 601–611 (2023).
Matzopoulos, R., Morlock, R., Morlock, A., Lerer, B. & Lerer, L. Psychedelic mushrooms in the USA: knowledge, patterns of use, and association with health outcomes. Front. Psychiatry. 12, 780696 (2022).
Yockey, A. & King, K. Use of psilocybin (mushrooms) among US adults: 2015–2018. J. Psychedelic Stud. 5, 17–21 (2021).
Foster, D. W., Young, C. M. & Bärnighausen, T. W. Self-Control as a moderator of the relationship between drinking identity and alcohol use. Subst. Use Misuse. 49, 1340–1348 (2014).
Gibson, C., Schreck, C. J. & Miller, J. M. Binge drinking and negative alcohol-related behaviors: A test of self-control theory. J. Criminal Justice. 32, 411–420 (2004).
Dvorak, R. D. & Day, A. M. Marijuana and self-regulation: examining likelihood and intensity of use and problems. Addict. Behav. 39, 709–712 (2014).
Ford, J. A. & Blumenstein, L. Self-Control and substance use among college students. J. Drug Issues. 43, 56–68 (2013).
Gibson, C. L., Ward, J. T., Wright, J. P., Beaver, K. M. & Delisi, M. Where does gender fit in the measurement of Self-Control? Criminal Justice Behav. 37, 883–903 (2010).
Lev-Ran, S., Le Strat, Y., Imtiaz, S., Rehm, J. & Le Foll, B. Gender differences in prevalence of substance use disorders among individuals with lifetime exposure to substances: results from a large representative sample. Am. J. Addict. 22, 7–13 (2013).
Kray, J., Eber, J. & Lindenberger, U. Age differences in executive functioning across the lifespan: the role of verbalization in task Preparation. Acta. Psychol. 115, 143–165 (2004).
Willoughby, T., Heffer, T., Good, M. & Magnacca, C. Is adolescence a time of heightened risk taking? An overview of types of risk-taking behaviors across age groups. Dev. Rev. 61, 1–18 (2021).
Tittle, C. R., Ward, D. A., Grasmick, H. G. & Gender Age, and crime/deviance: A challenge to Self-Control theory. J. Res. Crime. Delinquency. 40, 426–453 (2003).
Teixeira, P. J. et al. Psychedelics and health behaviour change. J. Psychopharmacol. 36, 12–19 (2022).
Lyke, J. A. & Kuti, J. Demographic characteristics and frequency of use among current users of classic hallucinogens. Int. J. Clin. Psychiatry Mental Health. 7, 3–9 (2019).
Weleff, J., Anand, A., Dewey, E. N. & Barnett, B. S. LSD use in the united States: examining user demographics and their evolution from 2015–2019. JPS https://doi.org/10.1556/2054.2024.00309 (2024).
Kruger, D. J., Glynos, N. G., Fields, C. W., Herberholz, M. & Boehnke, K. F. An assessment of psychedelic knowledge among people using psychedelics naturalistically. J. Psychoactive Drugs. 55, 420–424 (2023).
Carr, D. & Springer, K. W. Advances in families and health research in the 21st century. J. Marriage Family. 72, 743–761 (2010).
Thoits, P. A. Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 52, 145–161 (2011).
Jang, B. J., Schuler, M. S., Evans-Polce, R. J. & Patrick, M. E. Marital status as a partial mediator of the associations between young adult substance use and subsequent substance use disorder: application of causal inference methods. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 79, 567–577 (2018).
Laub, J. H., Nagin, D. S. & Sampson, R. J. Trajectories of change in criminal offending: good marriages and the desistance process. Am. Sociol. Rev. 63, 225–238 (1998).
Laub, J. H. & Sampson, R. J. Turning points in the life course: why change matters to the study of crime. Criminology 31, 301–325 (1993).
Galea, S., Rudenstine, S. & Vlahov, D. Drug use, misuse, and the urban environment. Drug Alcohol Rev. 24, 127–136 (2005).
Metzler, C. Epidemiologie des Substanzkonsums bei Jugendlichen: Eine Analyse unter Stadt-Land-Perspektive. (2003) doi:10.23668/psycharchives.8536
Kreuter, F., Presser, S. & Tourangeau, R. Social desirability Bias in CATI, IVR, and web surveys: the effects of mode and question sensitivity. Pub. Opin. Q. 72, 847–865 (2008).
Ong, A. & Weiss, D. The impact of anonymity on responses to sensitive questions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 30, 1691–1708 (2000).
Syed, O. A. et al. Global trends in psychedelic microdosing: demographics, substance testing behavior, and patterns of use. J. Psychoactive Drugs. 1–11 https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2024.2424284 (2024).
Manthey, J., Rehm, J. & Verthein, U. Germany’s cannabis act: a catalyst for European drug policy reform? Lancet Reg. Health - Europe. 42, 100929 (2024).
Sattler, S., Wood, S., Petersen, M. A., Seiffert, F., Mehlkop, G. Data publication for: Population-based estimates ofdifferent dosage types of psychedelic use across socio-demographic groups in Germany. Bielefeld University.(2025). https://doi.org/10.4119/unibi/3003733.
Acknowledgements
We thank those who helped conduct this study, especially Fabian Hasselhorn, Floris van Veen, Jonas Jakubassa, Lea Becher, and the forsa-team.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
This work was supported by the German Research Foundation [SA 2992/2 − 1 and SA 2992/2–2 to Sebastian Sattler; ME 2082/8 − 1 and ME 2082/8 − 2 to Guido Mehlkop].
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization: S.S., S.W., M.P., G.M.; Data curation: S.S.; Formal analysis: F.S.; Funding acquisition: S.S., G.M.; Investigation: S.S., G.M.; Methodology: S.S., S.W., M.P., G.M.; Project administration: S.S., G.M.; Visualization: S.S., S.W., F.S.; Writing – original draft: S.S., S.W., M.P., F.S., G.M.; Writing – Review and Editing: S.S., S.W., M.P., F.S., G.M.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethics approval
The questionnaire and methodology for this study was approved by the Ethics committee of the University of Erfurt (reference number: EV-20220830). Informed consent was obtained from all study participants.
Consent to participate
Informed consent was obtained from all study participants.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Sattler, S., Wood, S., Petersen, M.A. et al. Population-based estimates of different dosage types of psychedelic use across socio-demographic groups in Germany. Sci Rep 15, 18952 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-03873-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-03873-0