Abstract
Human perception of predators is extremely important because the decline in biodiversity requires greater involvement from the public in species conservation. We conducted an online study in Slovakia with 225 adult participants (151 women; mean age = 24 years), who viewed a series of short videos depicting predator-prey interactions in which the predator either won or lost the encounter. Most of the predators depicted were large mammals (e.g., leopards, lions), one of which was a snake. We examined participants’ compassion towards both the predator and prey and their willingness to protect these species. Participants exhibited greater compassion towards predators when they lost, whereas prey received higher compassion scores when they lost than when they won. Prey generally received higher compassion scores than predators. Fear of perceived animals was negatively correlated with compassion for predators and prey. Perceived fear and compassion for both predators and prey were positively correlated with the willingness to protect them (WTP), although these correlations were negative for the majority of predator species. WTP was not significantly influenced by the outcome of the encounter. Snakes, often considered prototypical stimuli for human fear, elicited less compassion and lower WTP compared to mammalian predators. We conclude that human sensitivity to physical interactions between predators and prey does not necessarily translate into a willingness to protect these animals. Conservation strategies need to consider the emotional and cognitive biases people hold toward different species. By acknowledging these biases, especially fear and perceived charisma, conservation campaigns may more effectively foster public support for protecting a broader range of species.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Predators have exerted significant selective pressure on human ancestors1,2,3. The primary predators of humans are believed to have been fast-moving mammals, primarily felids and canids4,5. However, snakes hunted small placental mammals, from which the order Primates—including humans—eventually evolved around 85 million years later2. Nonetheless, aversion to snakes may also stem from the more immediate and historically persistent threat that venomous species have posed to humans and livestock3. This means that snakes also posed a significant threat to predation for early human ancestors. Given the long evolutionary history of this threat, emotional adaptations to detect and evade predators likely evolved early in our lineage. One such adaptation is fear, a basic emotional and behavioural response that has evolved to enhance survival by triggering rapid ‘fight or flight’ reactions in the face of potential danger6,7,8,9. For example, research using eye movements showed that participants were quicker to detect predators facing forward (e.g., a lion) among predators facing away than the opposite10. There is a gender difference in the initiation of this adaptation, wherein women are traditionally more afraid of dangerous animals than men11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21.
Negative perceptions of predators and the reluctance to protect them can vary significantly by species22,26,32. Although large carnivores, such as tigers and lions, often evoke admiration and are perceived as charismatic20,33,34, snakes tend to evoke stronger fear responses that are rooted in our evolutionary history2,7,35. This fear is deeply ingrained, making snakes prototypical stimuli for fear, which in turn leads to less empathy towards them compared to other predators and ultimately reduces the public’s willingness to protect them36,37,38. However, human perceptions of predators are not static and may be influenced by the context of their interactions with prey. While a general aversion or admiration exists for certain species, observing a predator engage in a hunt, with varying outcomes, could potentially impact emotional responses; for instance, an outcome where a typically feared predator is unsuccessful might lead to reduced fear and increased compassion towards that animal.
Human willingness to pay (WTP) is often correlated with a broader willingness to protect living organisms39. WTP animals is influenced by emotional responses elicited by animals, which consequently shape attitudes towards wildlife21. Fear often leads to negative perceptions of certain animal species, particularly predators9,11,22,23. In particular, the presence of large carnivores, such as lions or tigers, in human environments can generate strong opposition to the conservation of these species24,25. Media portrayals that emphasise the dangers associated with these animals may contribute to increased public fear26, which contributes to decreased tolerance for predators and reduced support for conservation initiatives22,27,28. Experimental studies have shown that exposed fear-inducing images of predators, such as predators in aggressive postures, exhibited a lower willingness to support conservation efforts than those presented with non-fear-inducing images29,30. In a recent study, Prokop and Fančovičová31 conducted a short-term intervention using positive depictions of wolves (e.g., their crucial role in ecosystems). This intervention significantly improved schoolchildren’s attitudes toward these animals compared to negative depictions, suggesting that the perception of predators is highly sensitive to fear-based cues. Understanding how people emotionally perceive predators may clarify how media portrayals affect conservation willingness.
On the contrary, emotions such as compassion and empathy can foster positive attitudes toward wildlife conservation. Compassion is defined as an emotional response to the suffering of others, characterised by a genuine desire to alleviate that suffering40, while empathy involves both cognitive and affective understanding of the feelings of another without necessarily prompting action41,42. Empathy and compassion for animals are strongly correlated43, yet emotional connection to animals has received limited attention in Western psychology44,45. A moderate correlation between empathy for humans and empathy for animals has been reported, suggesting that people who show compassion towards one group tend to extend similar feelings toward the other46,47,48. This suggests that promoting compassion and empathy for certain animals would increase the willingness of people to engage in pro-environmental behaviours, such as conservation efforts49,50, but it should not necessarily be applied to other animals. For example, when participants were presented with film stimuli depicting various animals in victimised circumstances, birds elicited lower empathy than mammalian stimuli46. Compassion is closely related to empathy for animals and nature-relatedness, suggesting that these emotional responses can predict concern for animal welfare and environmental stewardship45. Cultivating compassion and empathy for animals, as well as for humans who live alongside them, is essential to promote a deeper commitment to wildlife conservation and protection of nature51.
Research consistently shows that women exhibit higher levels of compassion and empathy for animals than men52,53,54. On the contrary, men are often socialised to adopt a more utilitarian view of animals, which can manifest itself in less emotional engagement and greater acceptance of practices such as animal research55,56.
To date, little is known about how the outcomes of predator-prey interactions influence compassionate responses and willingness to protect these animals. Research in zoological gardens has shown that most visitors disapprove of feeding cheetahs and tigers live rabbits during exhibits57,58, suggesting that people tend to view predation negatively. The present study aimed to fill this gap by exploring compassion and willingness to protect animals in situations where the prey escapes or is defeated by the predator. We investigated how combat outcomes (victorious vs. non-victorious), self-perceived fear, predator taxonomy (mammal vs. snake), participant sex, compassion towards humans, and target animal (predator vs. prey) influence compassionate responses and willingness to protect these animals. We hypothesized that animals portrayed as losers in predator–prey interactions elicit greater compassion than those portrayed as winners, and that elevated compassion increases the willingness to protect these animals. Women are expected to show significantly more compassion toward prey than men52,53, as prey are more likely to trigger caregiving responses than predators. Individuals with higher levels of dispositional compassion are more likely to express greater compassion toward animals44,45, especially those perceived as losers. In contrast, increased fear toward an animal is expected to suppress the willingness to protect it27,28,30. Finally, mammals are predicted to elicit more compassion than reptiles (e.g., snakes), due to their closer phylogenetic relationship to humans15. Understanding how combat outcomes and emotional responses influence willingness to protect predators and prey can inform targeted interventions to mitigate fear and promote compassion, ultimately increasing public support for conservation initiatives.
Methods
Participants
Participants were 225 Slovak adults (151 women) between the ages of 18 and 62 years (M = 24.1, SE = 0.53). Participants were primarily recruited through advertisements placed on social networking sites, specifically Facebook, and potentially via emails distributed through the university website targeting the student population. The advertisements and information provided to potential participants described the study as investigating human attitudes toward animals. Participation was entirely voluntary, and no financial or other incentive was provided for participation. Following initial recruitment, participants were also asked to recruit additional volunteers from their acquaintances using the snowball sampling method59. We invited individuals to participate in our research project on a voluntary basis without revealing the specific hypotheses involved.
Photographs as visual stimuli
We prepare colourful visual stimuli of predators (N = 6 pictures) and their prey (N = 5 pictures) (Table 1). All visual stimuli were downloaded from Google. We were unable to find predatory interactions between the snake and the hare with the same snake species, therefore, we used means of self-perceived fear from pictures using both snake species. Participants were asked to rate self-perceived fear of each animal using 7-point Likert scales (1 = not at all, 7 = very much).
Videos as visual stimuli
Videos showing predator-prey interactions (N = 10) with the same species as described in Table 1 were downloaded from YouTube. Half of the videos showed victorious attacks of the predator against the prey (predator approach toward the prey, attack and killing). Half of the videos showed non-victorious attacks of the predator against the prey (predator approaching the prey, attacking, and killing). Each video lasted up to 30 s and was played without volume. Participants were asked to rate compassion (To what extent do you feel sorry for the ‘animal that lost the encounter’) and willingness to protect (WTP) each animal (Do you think this species should be protected by laws?) in videos using a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very much). The reliability of compassion and WTP was high (Cronbach α = 0.88 and 0.89, respectively).
General compassion questionnaire (GCQ)
General compassion was assessed using a 16-item compassion questionnaire developed by Pommier et al.60. Items were rated on a scale from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). Negatively worded indifference items were scored in reverse order. The GCP comprises items such as “I notice when people are upset, even if they don’t say anything”, or “When others feel sadness, I try to comfort them”. The reliability of the scale was high (Cronbach α = 0.86). High scores indicate a high level of general compassion for other people.
Procedure
The online survey was available to participants for a period of approximately six weeks, from November to December 2024, and the estimated time for completion was no more than 10 min. The participants first completed the informed consent form and then rated their fear of animals based on pictures. They responded to 16 items from the GCQ and finally rated the animals in videos on compassion and willingness to protect. The last section of the questionnaire included demographic questions (participant gender and age). The participants received four versions of the questionnaire, in which pictures, items and videos were presented in random order. At the beginning of the questionnaire, participants were instructed to complete it on a computer to ensure that they could view predator-prey interactions on screens of an appropriate size. Completing the questionnaire through the smartphone or mobile phone was not allowed. This version clarifies the sequence of tasks and ensures consistency in verb tense and style.
Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were performed using two separate models, one for each of the two dependent variables (compassionate responses and willingness to protect). A Generalised Linear Mixed Model was used to test whether the following factors influenced compassion toward predators and prey: categorical predictors included predators vs. prey, predator victories vs. prey victories, taxonomic grouping (mammals vs. reptiles), and participant gender (man or woman), while continuous predictors included scores from the General Compassion Questionnaire, and self-perceived fear of animals. If compassion towards predators and prey (ordinal) was the dependent variable, the participant ID and the version of the questionnaire were defined as random effects. When willingness to protect (WTP) was used as the dependent variable, the predictors remained the same, but compassion toward predators and prey was included as an additional continuous predictor. All interactions between factors were included to initial models to capture complex relationships, to improve model accuracy, and to provide deeper insights into the data (combined effects of two or more independent variables on the dependent variable). Nonsignificant interactions were removed from the models. The age of the participants was not included in the models as its effect was not significant. We used post hoc tests to explore specific differences in compassionate responses and willingness to protect across the different combat outcomes, and to examine differences in perceived fear levels between the animal stimuli presented. Post hoc tests were conducted using the Bonferroni method. All statistical tests were performed using the Jamovi project61.
Results
Fear of predators and prey based on picture ratings
Participants showed greater fear of predators (median = 5, ± 95% CI, 4.94, 4.79) than of prey (median = 2, ± 95% CI, 2.67, 2.52); (GLMM, χ2 = 1633.62, df = 1, P < 0.001) (supplementary Table 1). Women showed significantly greater fear of animals than men (median = 4, ± 95% CI, 3.78, 3.94 and median = 3, ± 95% CI, 3.35, 3.56, respectively); (GLMM, χ2 = 7.0, df = 1, P = 0.008).
Compassion for predators and prey
Fear of the two snake species was strongly correlated (Spearman’s r = 0.87, P < 0.001, N = 225). There was no significant influence of predators (vs. prey) on compassion towards predators and prey scores (GLMM, χ2 = 0.29, df = 1, P = 0.59, Table 2, see Supplementary Table 2 for more results). Greater fear of animals was significantly associated with lower compassion toward predators and prey scores (GLMM, χ2 = 81.86, df = 1, P < 0.001). Mammals received higher compassion toward predators and prey scores than reptiles, and prey winners received significantly higher scores than predator winners (Table 2). Videos scored by women received significantly higher compassion scores than videos scored by men (GLMM, χ2 = 17.39, df = 1, P < 0.001). General compassion was positively and significantly associated with compassion toward predators and prey (GLMM, χ2 = 4.99, df = 1, P < 0.05). Two important interaction terms emerged (Table 2). First, predator victories received lower compassion scores than predator losses (post hoc test, P < 0.001), and prey received higher compassion scores when they lost than when they won (post hoc test, P < 0.001). (Fig. 1). Second, women exhibited significantly greater compassion for prey compared to men (post hoc test, P < 0.001), whereas no significant gender difference was observed in compassion for predators (post hoc test, P = 0.17). (Fig. 2).
Compassion for snakes and mammals
Additional GLMM with species and participant gender showed that both variables significantly influenced compassion scores (GLMM, χ2 = 328.5 and 21.4, df = 9 and 1, both P < 0.001, respectively) (Fig. 3, see Supplementary Table 3 for more results). Post hoc comparisons showed that snakes received significantly lower compassion scores compared to all other species (all P < 0.001). Videos scored by women received significantly higher compassion scores than videos scored by men. Women rated predator compassion similarly to males, but prey received higher compassion ratings by women than by men (GLMM, χ2 = 64.3, df = 9, P < 0.001). Other details regarding each animal scores can be found in Supplementary Table 4.
Willingness to protect predators and prey
Participants showed significantly higher WTP predators than prey (GLMM, χ2 = 351.52, df = 1, P < 0.001) regardless of whether the prey or predator was victorious or not (Table 3; Fig. 4, Supplementary Table 5). Higher fear ratings were associated with a higher WTP (GLMM, χ2 = 14.17, df = 1, P < 0.001, Supplementary Fig 0.1). Because this result was unexpected, we further analyzed the correlations between WTP and fear of specific species to better interpret the finding. As expected, fear of all predators, except for the lion, showed a slight negative correlation with WTP. All other correlations were positive (Supplementary Fig. 2). Furthermore, participants showed significantly higher WTP for mammals compared to snakes (Table 3). Compassion for animals in videos positively and significantly influenced WTP animals (GLMM, χ2 = 169.02, df = 1, P < 0.001), but general compassion and gender did not influence WTP animals (Table 3; Fig. 5).
Willingness to protect snakes and mammals
An additional GLMM showed a significant effect of species (GLMM, χ2 = 784.12, df = 9, P < 0.001), but not gender (GLMM, χ2 = 3.49, df = 1, P = 0.062), on WTP scores (Fig. 6). Lion received the highest median WTP scores, followed by cheetah. On the contrary, the hare scored the lowest, followed by snakes. Post hoc comparisons showed that hares and snakes received significantly lower compassion scores compared to all other species (all P < 0.001). The only exception was a nonsignificant difference between a hare and guanaco (P = 1.0). The interaction term (GLMM, χ2 = 76.64, df = 9, P < 0.001), means that women showed higher WTP prey than men (Fig. 6). Other details regarding each animal scores can be found in Supplementary Tables 6 and 7.
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate how different characteristics such as gender, compassion towards conspecifics, and fear of predators influence compassion and willingness to protect (WTP) towards animals. This influence was studied depending on the animals’ taxonomy, their status (predator vs. prey), and the result of their interaction in a fight (victorious vs. non-victorious). As far as we know, this is the first study to examine how the outcomes of predator-prey interactions influence compassionate responses and WTP animals. The differences we observed in fear, compassion, and willingness to protect predators and prey offer notable insights into how humans perceive animals. For example, the low levels of compassion shown toward snakes, and the negative link between fear and the willingness to protect predators, could pose challenges for the conservation of these frequently persecuted species. The results can be easily summarised.
First, we found that predator losses generated significantly more compassion than predator victories. This suggests that people may sympathise with predators when they are perceived as vulnerable or struggling, rather than when they succeed in hunting57,58. Compassion for the prey was also higher when the prey lost than when it won, indicating a persistent emotional bias favoring animals that lose. Perhaps the emotional bias favouring prey over predators arises from the significant cost difference between the two. When a prey is caught, it loses its life, whereas a predator that misses its prey experiences temporary hunger. Although both outcomes have survival implications, the loss of life for the prey is generally more severe than the missed opportunity for the predator. This disparity may contribute to the observed bias toward empathy in favour of prey.
Second, self-perceived fear of an animal was negatively associated with compassion towards predators and prey, i.e., adults who are the most fearful toward animals feel the least compassion toward them. This finding is consistent with previous research showing that species perceived as threatening or dangerous tend to receive less empathy from humans37,39. Interestingly, snakes, which are often considered a prototypical fear-inducing stimulus7,9,35, received the lowest compassion scores regardless of their role in the videos. The low compassion for snakes can be attributed to two main factors: (1) an evolutionary fear of them as predators throughout human history2 and/or (2) their significant phylogenetic distance from humans, which leads to a higher preference for mammals43,46,62. Furthermore, in Slovakia, where this study took place, people are much more likely to encounter snakes than apex predators like lions or pumas. This likely strengthens the impact of direct experiences and cultural narratives in shaping negative views and lower levels of compassion toward snakes. Snakes are unique creatures, and their portrayals in the media should be tailored to address the specific emotional biases they evoke, rather than applying the same standards used for large mammalian predators.
Third, WTP animals were positively associated with fear of animals, further supporting the idea that human emotions, particularly fear, play a crucial role in conservation attitudes22,29,30,32. However, most research has shown a negative association between fear and WTP. Although the overall correlation between WTP and fear was positive, it is noteworthy that the associations were negative for all predators except the lion. The strong charisma of the lion likely drives WTP despite the presence of fear22. Interestingly, while the compassion for predators and prey in the videos positively influenced WTP of these animals, the general levels of compassion did not. The lack of influence of general compassion on WTP suggests that conservation attitudes are driven by immediate emotional responses to specific species rather than an individual’s overall empathetic disposition. These results also imply that compassion depends on empathy. Large predators induce ambivalent emotional and cognitive responses, since admiration for their intelligence is often mixed with fear of their aggression20,63. Thus, large, charismatic predators such as lions may be seen as more deserving of protection, possibly due to their perceived ecological importance or symbolic status in wildlife conservation mediated by personal admiration, rather than non-charismatic animals like snakes. The low WTP scores for rabbits, despite their perceived innocence13, might be influenced by their common role as a food source, which could lead people to value them differently than non-hunted species. It’s also likely that participants in this study were more exposed to information about the conservation threats facing large predators, through media or education, than they were about snakes or rabbits. This greater awareness of the vulnerability of apex predators may have contributed to their higher WTP scores. Conservation campaigns could benefit from emphasising the ecological importance of less charismatic species in changing public attitudes.
Fourth, the differences in compassion between the genders were more pronounced for prey than for predators, suggesting that women may be particularly sensitive to the suffering of prey animals. This finding is consistent with previous research showing that women tend to express greater empathy towards animals and are generally more supportive of conservation initiatives52,53. Women’s greater compassion for prey may reflect evolutionary pressures that have promoted greater empathy and care in women64,65. Furthermore, women’s increased fear of predators may stem from their historically lower survival rates during predatory attacks compared to men66, which may increase their identification with the vulnerability of prey.
Several limitations of the present study can be identified, suggesting avenues for future research. In particular, our investigation is based on self-reported measures of compassion and willingness to pay (WTP), rather than direct behavioural observations. Future studies could benefit from incorporating more direct indicators of these constructs. However, evidence shows a link between individuals’ stated willingness to pay and willingness to protect living organisms39, as well as its connection to their actual support for conservation initiatives in real life67. We therefore assume that willingness to protect is a reliable tool for estimating actual support for conservation initiatives68. In addition, participants perceived prey/predator interactions through video stimuli, raising questions about whether similar responses would be observed with real-life observations. However, practical and ethical constraints make it challenging to carry out such experiments. For example, comparing reactions to a bullfight in person versus on television highlights the potential impact of observation mode on emotional responses. Furthermore, it would be valuable for future research to explore how relationships identified in adults are established during childhood.
To conclude, we have shown that people are emotionally engaged in physical interactions between large predators and prey, but this does not necessarily translate into conservation motivation. Media portrayals must address emotional biases, such as fear and negative perceptions of less charismatic species, such as snakes, to foster greater support for biodiversity protection. Given that predator losses elicited greater compassion, wildlife documentaries and social media content may unintentionally reinforce sympathy for predators only when they are perceived as vulnerable. Including depictions of predators facing starvation or experiencing the loss of their young as a result of predation by heterospecifics could help balance emotional responses toward dangerous animals. This approach can foster greater public support for biodiversity protection, even for less charismatic species such as snakes.
Data availability
Data is provided within the supplementary information files.
References
Hart, D. & Sussman, R. W. in Man the Hunted: Primates, Predators, and Human Evolution. (eds Hard, D. & Sussman, R. W.) (Westview, 2008). 357 pp
Isbell, L. A. The Fruit, the Tree, and the Serpent: why We See so Well (Harvard University Press, 2009).
Babo Martins, S. et al. Ruiz de castañeda, R. Snakebite and its impact in rural communities: the need for a one health approach. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 13, e0007608. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pntd.0007608 (2019).
Barrett, H. C. Adaptations to predators and prey. In The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (ed. Buss, D. M.) 200–223 (John Wiley & Sons Inc, 2015).
Treves, A. & Palmqvist, P. Reconstructing hominin interactions with mammalian carnivores. In Primate Anti-Predator Strategies (eds Gursky, S. L. & Nekaris, K. A. I.) 355–381 (Springer, 2007).
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In Handbook of Emotions (eds Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M.) 91–115 (Guilford, 2000).
Öhman, A. & Mineka, S. Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108, 483–522. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483 (2001).
O’Connell, K., Rhoads, S. A. & Marsh, A. A. ‘Fear: An Evolutionary Perspective on Its Biological, Behavioral, and Communicative Features’, in Laith Al-Shawaf, and Todd K. Shackelford (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Evolution and the Emotions ; online edn, Oxford Academic, New York, NY, 22 May 2024), (2024). https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780197544754.013.25
Seligman, M. E. Phobias and preparedness. Behav. Th. 2, 307–320. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(71)80064-3 (1971).
Yorzinski, J. L., Tovar, M. E. & Coss, R. G. Forward-facing predators attract attention in humans (Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol. 132, 410–418. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000126 (2018).
Kaltenborn, B. P., Bjerke, T. & Nyahongo, J. Living with problem animals: self-reported fear of potentially dangerous species in the Serengeti region, Tanzania. Hum. Dimens Wildl. 11, 397–409. https://doi.org/10.1080/10871200600984323 (2006).
Røskaft, E., Bjerke, T., Kaltenborn, B. P., Linnell, J. D. C. & Andersen, R. Patterns of self-reported fear toward large carnivores among the Norwegian public. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 184–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00011-4 (2003).
Prokop, P. & Kubiatko, M. Bad Wolf kills lovable rabbits: children’s attitudes toward predator and prey. El J. Sci. Educ. 12, 55–70 (2008).
Prokop, P. & Fančovičová, J. Perceived body condition is associated with fear of a large carnivore predator in humans. Ann. Zool. Fenn. 47, 417–425. https://doi.org/10.5735/086.047.0606 (2010).
Prokop, P., Zvaríková, M., Zvarík, M., Pazda, A. & Fedor, P. The effect of animal bipedal posture on perceived cuteness, fear, and willingness to protect them. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9, 681241. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2021.681241 (2021).
Pereira, H. M., Braga-Pereira, F., Azeredo, L. M. M., Lopez, L. C. S. & Alves, R. R. N. Assessing factors influencing students’ perceptions towards animal species conservation. Peer J. 11, e14553. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.14553 (2023).
Suryawanshi, K. R., Bhatia, S., Bhatnagar, Y. V., Redpath, S. & Mishra, C. Multiscale factors affecting human attitudes toward snow leopards and wolves. Conserv. Biol. 28, 1657–1666. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.12320 (2014).
Polák, J. et al. Scary and nasty beasts: Self-reported fear and disgust of common phobic animals. Br. J. Psychol. 111, 297–321. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12409 (2020).
Zvaríková, M. et al. What makes spiders frightening and disgusting to people? Front. Ecol. Evol. 9, 694569. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2021.694569 (2021).
Prokop, P., Zvaríková, M., Zvarík, M., Ježová, Z. & Fedor, P. Charismatic species should be large: the role of admiration and fear. People Natur. 6, 945–957. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10504 (2024).
Castillo-Huitrón, N. M., Naranjo, E. J., Santos-Fita, D. & Estrada-Lugo, E. The importance of human emotions for wildlife conservation. Front. Psychol. 11, 1277. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01277 (2020).
De Pinho, J. R., Grilo, C., Boone, R. B., Galvin, K. A. & Snodgrass, J. G. Influence of aesthetic appreciation of wildlife species on attitudes toward their conservation in Kenyan agropastoralist communities. PLoS ONE. 9 (2), e88842. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088842 (2014).
Prokop, P. & Randler, C. Biological predispositions and individual differences in human attitudes toward animals. In Ethnozoology: Animals in our Lives (eds Alves, R. R. N. & Albuquerque, U. P.) 447–466 (Academic, 2018). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809913-1.00023-5.
Røskaft, E., Händel, B., Bjerke, T. & Kaltenborn, B. P. Human attitudes toward large carnivores in Norway. Wildl. Biology. 13, 172–185. https://doi.org/10.2981/0909-6396(2007)13[172:HATLCI]2.0.CO;2 (2007).
Zimmermann, B., Wabakken, P. & Dötterer, M. Human-carnivore interactions in norway: how does the re-appearance of large carnivores affect people’s attitude. For. Snow Lands Res. 76 (1/2), 137–153 (2001).
Nanni, V. et al. Social media and large carnivores: sharing biased news on attacks on humans. Front. Ecol. Evol. 8, 71. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2020.00071 (2020).
Johansson, M., Sjöström, M., Karlsson, J. & Brännlund, R. Is human fear affecting public willingness to pay for the management and conservation of large carnivores? Soc. Natur. Resour. 25 (6), 610–620. https://doi.org/10.1080/08941920.2011.622734 (2012).
Treves, A. et al. Predators and the public trust. Biol. Rev. 92 (1), 248–270. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12227 (2017).
Prokop, P. & Fančovičová, J. Animals in dangerous postures enhance learning, but decrease willingness to protect animals. Eurasia J. Math. Sci. Tech. Edu. 13 (9), 6069–6077. https://doi.org/10.12973/eurasia.2017.01000a (2017).
Notaro, S. & Grilli, G. How much fear? Exploring the role of integral emotions on stated preferences for wildlife conservation. Environ. Manag. 69 (3), 449–465. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00267-022-01593-z (2022).
Prokop, P. & Fančovičová, J. A positive presentation of wolves affects the explicit and implicit attitudes of schoolchildren towards them. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 47 (3), 422–439. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2024.2327272 (2024).
Prokop, P. & Fančovičová, J. Does colour matter? The influence of animal warning coloration on human emotions and willingness to protect them. Anim. Conserv. 16, 458–466. https://doi.org/10.1111/acv.12014 (2013).
Albert, C., Luque, G. M. & Courchamp, F. The Twenty most charismatic species. PLoS One. 13, e0199149. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199149 (2018).
Berti, E., Monsarrat, S., Munk, M., Jarvie, S. & Svenning, J. C. Body size is a good proxy for vertebrate charisma. Biol. Conserv. 251, 108790. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2020.108790 (2020).
Janovcová, M. et al. Human attitude toward reptiles: A relationship between fear, disgust, and aesthetic preferences. Animals 9, 238. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9050238 (2019).
Knight, A. J. Bats, snakes and spiders, oh my! How aesthetic and negativistic attitudes, and other concepts predict support for species protection. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 94–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.10.001 (2008).
Onyishi, I. E., Nwonyi, S. K., Pazda, A. & Prokop, P. Attitudes and behaviour toward snakes on the part of Igbo people in southeastern Nigeria. Sci. Tot Environ. 763, 143045. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.143045 (2021).
da Silva, A. R. et al. Bioecological representations and social characteristics of students influence their attitudes toward wild vertebrates. Jour Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 19 (1), 25. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-023-00593-5 (2023).
Prokop, P. & Belzárová, K. Tomanová čergeťová, I. Compassion and the perceived rarity of plants can increase plant appreciation. People Natur. 7, 387–397. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10775 (2025).
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., Simon-Thomas, E. & Compassion An evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychol. Bull. 136, 351–374. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018807 (2010).
Cuff, B. M., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., Howat, D. J. & Empathy A review of the concept. Emot. Rev. 8, 144–153. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466 (2016).
Jolliffe, D. & Farrington, D. P. Development and validation of the basic empathy scale. J. Adoles. 29, 589–611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010 (2006).
Miralles, A., Raymond, M. & Lecointre, G. Empathy and compassion toward other species decrease with evolutionary divergence time. Sci. Rep. 9 (1), 19555. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-56006-9 (2019).
Wallach, A. D., Batavia, C., Bekoff, M., Alexander, S., Baker, L., Ben-Ami, D., …Ramp, D. Recognizing animal personhood in compassionate conservation. Conserv Biol.34, 1097–1106; https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13494 (2020).
Khoury, B. & Vergara, R. C. Compassion questionnaire for animals: scale development and validation. Jour Environ. Psychol. 100, 102470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2024.102470 (2024).
Westbury, H. R. & Neumann, D. L. Empathy-related responses to moving film stimuli depicting human and non-human animal targets in negative circumstances. Biol. Psychol. 78, 66–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.12.009 (2008).
Paul, E. S. Empathy with animals and with humans: are they linked? Anthrozoös. 13, 194–202 ; (2000). https://doi.org/10.2752/089279300786999699
Preylo, B. D. & Arikawa, H. Comparison of vegetarians and non-vegetarians on pet attitude and empathy. Anthrozoös 21 (4), 387–395. https://doi.org/10.2752/175303708X371654 (2008).
Tam, K. P. Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: similarities and differences. Jour Environ. Psychol. 34, 64–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.004 (2013).
Young, A., Khalil, K. A. & Wharton, J. Empathy for animals: a review of the existing literature. Curator: Museum J. 61, 327–343. https://doi.org/10.1111/cura.12257 (2018).
Pooley, S. The challenge of compassion in predator conservation. Front. Psychol. 13, 977703. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.977703 (2022).
Herzog, H. Gender differences in attitudes toward animals: A review of the literature. Anthrozoös 20, 7–22. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279307780216687 (2007).
Taylor, N. & Signal, T. D. Empathy and attitudes to animals. Anthrozoös 18, 18–27. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279305785594342 (2005).
Colombo, E. S., Crippa, F., Calderari, T. & Prato-Previde, E. Empathy toward animals and people: the role of gender and length of service in a sample of Italian veterinarians. Jour Veter Behav. 17, 32–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.10.010 (2017).
Pifer, L. Exploring the gender gap in young adults’ attitudes about animal research. Soc. Anim. 4, 37–52 (1996).
Herzog, H. A. Jr, Betchart, N. S. & Pittman, R. B. Gender, sex role orientation, and attitudes toward animals. Anthrozoös 4, 184–191. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279391787057170 (1991).
Ings, R., Waran, N. K. & Young, R. J. Attitude of zoo visitors to the Idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals. Zoo Biol. 16, 343–347. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2361(1997)16:4%3C343::AID-ZOO6%3E3.0.CO;2-A (1997).
Cottle, L., Tamir, D., Hyseni, M., Bühler, D. & Lindemann-Matthies, P. Feeding live prey to zoo animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland. Zoo Biol. 29 (3), 344–350. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20261 (2010).
Goodman, L. A. Snowball sampling. Annal Mathemat Stat. 32, 148–170. https://doi.org/10.1214/aoms/1177705148 (1961).
Pommier, E., Neff, K. D. & Tóth-Király, I. The development and validation of the compassion scale. Assessment 27 (1), 21–39. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191119874 (2020).
The jamovi project. jamovi (Version 2.5) Computer Software. (2024). https://www.jamovi.org
Batt, S. Human attitudes towards animals in relation to species similarity to humans: a multivariate approach. Biosci. Horizon. 2 (2), 180–190. https://doi.org/10.1093/biohorizons/hzp021 (2009).
Sevillano, V. & Fiske, S. T. Warmth and competence in animals. Jour Appl. Soc. Psychol. 46, 276–293. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12361 (2016).
Archer, J. The reality and evolutionary significance of human psychological sex differences. Biol. Rev. 94, 1381–1415. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12507 (2019).
Greenberg, D. M., Warrier, V., Allison, C. & Baron-Cohen, S. Testing the empathizing-systemizing theory of sex differences and the extreme male brain theory of autism in half a million people. Proc. Nat. Acad. Scien USA. 115, 12152–12157. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1811032115 (2018).
Treves, A. & Naughton-Treves, L. Risk and opportunity for humans coexisting with large carnivores. Jour Hum. Evol. 36 (3), 275–282. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1998.0268 (1999).
Dörge, L., Büscher, M., Drews, J., Eylering, A. & Fiebelkorn, F. German laypeople’s willingness to donate toward insect conservation: application of an extended protection motivation theory. Front. Psychol. 12, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.773913 (2022).
Randler, C. & Koch, S. Willingness to protect bird species depends on individual respondents’ demographic and species traits. Cons Sci. Pract. 7, e13277. https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.13277 (2025).
Acknowledgements
Two anonymous referees provided helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. This study was partially funded by grant VEGA no. 1/0372/24.
Funding
This study was funded by the Slovak Grant Agency VEGA no. 1/0211/25 and 1/0372/24.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
P.P., J.B., Z.P., M.Z. and P.F. designed the study, J.B. performed the experiment, P.P., P.B and G.T. wrote the manuscript. P.P. and M.Z. performed statistical analyses, M.Z. visualized the results, P.F. obtained funding for publication.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethics declarations
This research was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Natural Sciences, Comenius University (Licence no. ECH19036). All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations, including the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Prokop, P., Balcerčík, J., Bonin, P. et al. The effect of fear and compassion on human willingness to protect predators and prey. Sci Rep 15, 23278 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-06861-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-06861-6