Introduction

Apolipoprotein E (APOE) is a polymorphic protein involved in neurogenesis, plasticity, and repair mechanisms1. Research has shown that the APOE genotype influences amyloid-β (Aβ) metabolism, cholesterol homeostasis, neurovascular function, and neuroinflammation, which are considered to play significant roles in the pathology of Alzheimer’s disease (AD)2,3,4. Therefore, the ε4 allele is recognized as a significant genetic risk factor for AD5,6. APOE ε4 carriers having 1 and 2 copies of the allele are 3 and 15 times more likely to develop AD respectively7. In addition, there were confirmed associations between ε4 allele and cognitive ability. Both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of healthy people have shown that ε4-carriers demonstrated worse performance or accelerated decline in several cognitive domains (e.g. memory, executive functioning and overall global cognitive ability) compared to non-carriers8,9,10,11. During middle age, the ε4 allele may slightly impair cognition12, while after age 60, this impact becomes more pronounced13.

In recent years, there has been a growing research interest in investigating the impact of the APOE ε4 allele on cognition across the lifespan, extending beyond old age. However, the studies of young adults have yielded inconsistent results14,15,16,17,18. Notably, the impact of the APOE ε4 allele on cognitive function in young individuals remains a subject of considerable debate. Among the most contentious issues is whether APOE ε4 allele acts as an antagonist of pleiotropic genes. Many researchers suggested that the APOE ε4 allele may be an antagonistic pleiotropy gene, conferring a beneficial effect on cognition in early life and a detrimental impact on cognition during later years15,16,19,20. However, some evidence didn’t support this view14,21. Interestingly, one study has shown superior visual working memory in APOE ε4 carriers, indicating that some benefits of this genotype are demonstrable in older age22, which appears to be inconsistent with the notion that APOE ε4 acts as an antagonist of pleiotropic genes. A recent meta-analytic study across seven cognitive domains(intelligence/achievement, attention/working memory, executive functioning, memory, language, processing speed and visuospatial abilities) in younger individuals (participants’ ages ranged from 2 to 40) also did not find statistically significant differences in cognitive performance between APOE ε4 carriers and non-carriers23. Overall, the impact of the APOE ε4 allele on cognitive abilities in young populations remains inconclusive. The inconsistent results across studies may be attributed to differences in age, education level, and other demographic factors, as well as the varying cognitive domains examined.

Given the prominent role of memory impairment as a recognized symptom of dementia, investigating the onset and specific impact of the AD risk gene APOE ε4 on memory function in young adults is particularly compelling. Memory functions include short-term memory, working memory, and long-term memory. Individuals with AD and those in its prodromal stages show more pronounced declines in short-term memory and working memory24,25; these two cognitive domains are also fundamental for assessing learning abilities in young adult students26.To date, there have been relatively few studies exploring the relationship between these two types of memory performance and APOE genotypes15,21,27, as well as the structural basis underlying this association in young adults14,28,29. Cognitive functions require coordination among widely networked brain areas30. A growing body of neuroscience research highlights the importance of functional interactions within and between multiple brain networks for APOE ε4 and memory skills31,32,33,34. Among them, the default-mode network (DMN) has been extensively studied in cognitive neuroscience due to its close relationship with memory processing, particularly working memory. The DMN is typically active during rest and shows reduced activation during externally focused cognitive tasks, a pattern thought to reflect shifts in attention and memory resources35,36. Notably, the functional integrity and connectivity of the DMN, especially in core regions like the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), have been shown to correlate with performance in memory and working memory tasks37. Additionally, evidence suggests that the functional interaction between the DMN and task-positive networks, including the executive control network and salience network, is crucial for maintaining optimal cognitive function38. Unlike working memory networks, short-term memory relies more on primary sensory cortices and the hippocampus-parietal circuits for the temporary storage and processing of information39. However, the precise mechanisms of these large-scale network interactions, especially in the context of APOE genotype differences in young adults, remain poorly understood.

Therefore, we hypothesized that APOE genotypes may influence memory performance in young adults, potentially manifestations in the functional connectivity of a wide range of brain networks including the executive control network, the salience network, and the DMN. To test this hypothesis, we selected a homogeneous sample with matched age and educational background, focusing on short-term and working memory as core memory domains, aiming to elucidate both the relationship between APOE genotypes and memory performance and the associated functional connectivity patterns in young adults, thereby providing new evidence to address ongoing research debates.

Methods

Participants and grouping

All data were obtained at the Southwest University (Chongqing, China), and all participants provided written informed consent and received payment for their time and task participation. The research protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the review committee of the Brain Imaging Center of Southwest University. The data used in this study were extracted from an ongoing research project, the Gene-Brain-Behavior (GBB) project, which has been referred to in several previous studies40,41. GBB is a large sample database that measures multiple behavioral variables, such as creativity, emotion, personality, growth experience, and health, and its recruitment program and exclusion criteria have been detailed in previous publications42. All participants underwent genotype testing for APOE ε4/ε3/ε2, and we divided all participants into three groups according to their genotype: group APOE 2 (ε2/ε2 and ε2/ε3), group APOE 3 (ε2/ε4 and ε3/ε3) and group APOE 4 (ε3/ε4 and ε4/ε4).

Initially, 1069 university students underwent APOE genotyping for this study. However, the subsequent experiments and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanning required additional time, and not all students could complete these procedures. Ultimately, complete data, including MRI scans, were obtained from 516 students who completed the short-term memory experiment and 156 students who completed the working memory experiment. For the working memory analysis, 156 participants were included (3 APOE ε2/ε2, 20 APOE ε2/ε3, 1 APOE ε2/ε4, 102 APOE ε3/ε3, 29 APOE ε3/ε4, and 1 APOE ε4/ε4 carriers). Considering the protective role of APOE ε2 in AD and the dose effect of APOE ε4, 1 APOE ε2/ε4 participant was excluded from our analysis (n = 155). For the short-term memory analysis, 516 participants were included (8 APOE ε2/ε2, 75 APOE ε2/ε3, 1 APOE ε2/ε4, 346 APOE ε3/ε3, 82 APOE ε3/ε4, and 5 APOE ε4/ε4 carriers).

Assessment of working memory

Working memory was measured using the n-back task. We used only one condition (3-back) for the letter n-back task. Participants were asked to identify whether the current item had flashed three items earlier in the sequence. Participants were instructed to press “F” if they thought the current item matched two items earlier and to press “J” otherwise. Participants completed 90 trials. Each trial lasted 3000 ms and the letter for each trial was presented for 750 ms. The Psychophysics Toolbox (http://psychtoolbox.org/) for MATLAB was used to display the stimuli.

The mean reaction time (RT) was calculated after excluding the trials with no response and incorrect response. Accuracy (ACC) was also calculated after excluding the trials with no response. The ACC of n-back task was used as an indicator of of working memory ability.

Assessment of short-term memory

Short-term memory was measured using digit Span assessment43, which is a subscale of the Wechsler Adult Intelligent Scale (WAIS). The WAIS-IV was used in this study, which has a high degree of reliability (reliability coefficient, 0.92) and validity44.

There are two parts to digit span assessment: forward and digits Backward. Each tap represents distinct but interdependent cognitive functions. Digits forward primarily taps short-term auditory memory while digits backward measures the participants’ ability to manipulate verbal information while it is still in temporary storage. In digits forward, the participants listened to and repeated a sequence of numbers spoken aloud by the interviewer. In digits backward, the participants listened to a sequence of numbers and repeated them in reverse order. In both parts, the length of each sequence of numbers increases as the participants responds correctly.

The converted standard score of digit span assessment was used as an indicator of short-term memory ability.

Analyses

ANCOVA was used to compare the differences in working memory and short-term memory among individuals with different genotypes (group APOE 2, group APOE3 and group APOE4). Before the analysis, Levene’s test and the Shapiro–Wilk test were conducted to verify the assumptions of homogeneity of variances and normality required for ANCOVA, which indicated that the data analysis met the prerequisites for ANCOVA. Working memory (ACC in the n-back task) and short-term memory (digit span task) were included as dependent variables. Sex was included as a covariate in all analyses, and RT in the n-back task was additionally controlled when comparing working memory accuracy to account for potential speed–accuracy trade-offs. Furthermore, given the imbalance in group sizes, a non-parametric resampling (bootstrap) method was implemented to ensure robust comparisons. In simply terms, we conducted a random sampling with the replacement of group APOE3 10,000 times. In each sampling process of working memory, 30 participants belonging to group APOE ε3 were randomly selected (due to group APOE4 having a sample size of 30), and their average ACC in the n-back task was calculated and compared with that of group APOE4. In each sampling process of short-term memory, 87 participants belonging to the group APOE3 were randomly selected (due to the group APOE4 having a sample size of 87), and their average score was calculated and compared with those of the APOE4 group. Consequently, bootstrap-based p-values and 95% confidence intervals were obtained. All bootstrap analyses were conducted using the “boot” package in R.

Image acquisition and preprocessing.

All the functional and structural data were obtained using a 3 T SIEMENS PRISMA scanner (Erlangen, Germany) at the Brain Imaging Center of Southwest University. For resting-state fMRI, the functional imaging data were obtained using a multiband T2*-sensitive gradient-recalled single-shot echo-planar imaging pulse sequence: repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, flip angle (FA) = 90°, field of view (FOV) = 220 × 220 mm2, slices = 32, thickness = 3.0 mm, and voxel size = 3.4 × 3.4 × 4.0 mm3. High-resolution, three-dimensional T1-weighted structural images were obtained using a magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence: TR = 1900 ms, TE = 2.52 ms, FA = 9°, slices = 176, FOV = 256 × 256 mm2, thickness = 1.0 mm, and voxel size = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3.

FMRIPrep45,46 based on Nipype47 was used to preprocess the task-fMRI functional imaging data with the following parameters. First, a reference volume and its skull-stripped version were generated using a custom methodology of fMRIPrep. Head-motion parameters with respect to the BOLD reference (transformation matrices, and six corresponding rotation and translation parameters) were estimated before any spatiotemporal filtering using MCFLIRT48. BOLD runs were slice-time corrected to 0.962 s (0.5 of slice acquisition range 0–1.93 s) using 3dTshift from AFNI49. The BOLD time-series (including slice-timing correction when applied) were resampled onto their original, native space by applying the transforms to correct for head-motion. These resampled BOLD time-series are referred to as preprocessed BOLD in original space, or just preprocessed BOLD. The BOLD reference was then co-registered to the T1w reference using mri_coreg (FreeSurfer) followed by flirt50 with the boundary-based registration51 cost-function. Co-registration was configured with six degrees of freedom. Several confounding time-series were calculated based on the preprocessed BOLD: frame wise displacement (FD), DVARS and three region-wise global signals. FD was computed using two formulations following Power47 and Jenkinson48. FD and DVARS were calculated for each functional run, both using their implementations in Nipype47. The three global signals were extracted within the the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), the white matter (WM), and the whole-brain masks. Additionally, a set of physiological regressors were extracted to allow for component-based noise correction52. Principal components were estimated after high-pass filtering the preprocessed BOLD time-series (using a discrete cosine filter with 128 s cut-off) for the two CompCor variants: temporal (tCompCor) and anatomical (aCompCor). tCompCor components were then calculated from the top 2% variable voxels within the brain mask. For aCompCor, three probabilistic masks (CSF, WM and combined CSF + WM) were generated in anatomical space. The implementation differs from that of Behzadi et al. in that instead of eroding the masks by 2 pixels on BOLD space, the aCompCor masks were subtracted a mask of pixels that likely contain a volume fraction of GM. This mask is obtained by thresholding the corresponding partial volume map at 0.05, and it ensures components were not extracted from voxels containing a minimal fraction of GM. Finally, these masks were resampled into BOLD space and binarized by thresholding at 0.99 (as in the original implementation). Components were also calculated separately within the WM and CSF masks. For each CompCor decomposition, the k components with the largest singular values were retained, such that the retained components’ time series were sufficient to explain 50 percent of variance across the nuisance mask (CSF, WM, combined, or temporal). The remaining components were dropped from consideration. The head-motion estimates calculated in the correction step were also placed within the corresponding confounds file. The confound time series derived from head motion estimates and global signals were expanded with the inclusion of temporal derivatives and quadratic terms for each53. Frames that exceeded a threshold of 0.5 mm FD or 1.5 standardized DVARS were annotated as motion outliers. The BOLD time-series were resampled into standard space, generating a preprocessed BOLD run in MNI152NLin2009cAsym space. First, a reference volume and its skull-stripped version were generated using a custom methodology of fMRIPrep. All resamplings were performed with a single interpolation step by composing all the pertinent transformations (i.e. head-motion transform matrices, susceptibility distortion correction when available, and co-registrations to anatomical and output spaces). Gridded (volumetric) resamplings were performed using antsApplyTransforms (ANTs), configured with Lanczos interpolation to minimize the smoothing effects of other kernels54. Non-gridded (surface) resamplings were performed using mri_vol2surf (FreeSurfer).

Functional connectivity feature extraction.

We used the MATLAB R2016b-based functional connectivity toolbox “Conn toolbox” version 19c55 to extract functional connectivity feature (i.e., 34 participants × 11 trials) on the basis of previously described intrinsic functional network atlases in the MNI space56. First, we performed fMRI denoising based on the linear regression of the following parameters from each node: (a) five noise components each from minimally-eroded WM and CSF (one-node binary erosion of nodes with values above 50% in posterior probability maps), respectively, on the basis of aCompCor procedures52,57; (b) linear BOLD signal trend within session. In a separate step after nuisance regression58, data were then temporally filtered with a bandpass of 0.008–0.09 Hz. Then, we extracted the denoised BOLD time series from the mean across all voxels within each node. We computed a matrix of functional connectivity values between all region pairs on the basis of the Fisher z-transformed Pearson correlation coefficient of time series.

Predictive modeling analysis of working memory

We adapted the connectome-based predictive modeling (CPM) method59 to identify functional connectivity patterns that predicted the working memory.

For each of the 155 participants, we generated model-based working memory scores (ACC of n-back task) prediction based on the independent data form all other participants (i.e., leave-one-participant-out). For each cross-validation fold, after controlling for sex, the mean frame-wise head motion and RT of n-Back task, we calculated the partial correlations between each edge (node pair) in the functional connectivity matrix (derived from the Schaefer400 atlas) and within-participant scores. Then, we ‘masked’ the brain-behavior correlations such that only the edges positively correlated with novelty and appropriateness scores at the suprathreshold level of P ≤ 0.01 (two-tailed) were retained, resulting in positive edge masks. In the held-out-participant, we performed a Pearson correlation between model-predicted working memory scores and observed, within-participant working memory scores.

To determine whether predicted versus observed correlations were statistically significant at the group level, we generated a distribution of null values. To this end, we repeated all of the same CPM procedures, as previously described, except the assignments of functional connectivity matrix were randomly permuted (1,000 iterations) to obtain null correlation values to assess the significance (permutation test, Ppt ≤ 0.05, two-tailed). The code for CPM analyses was adapted from publicly available scripts (https://github.com/YaleMRRC/CPM).

Finally, we retained the edges that positively correlated with working memory scores at every cross-validation fold to constitute the CPM mask of working memory (here after referred to as “WM-CPM”).

Analysis of functional neuroanatomical patterns contributing to the CPM of working memory

We visualized the edges comprising the WM-CPM (see Fig. 2C) using the BioImage Suite Connectivity Visualization Tool (https://bioimagesuiteweb.github.io/webapp). To observe the neuroanatomical patterns that contributed to the WM-CPM, we assigned each node to one of the 7 canonical Yeo-Krienen60 intrinsic functional networks. For these analyses, we used the Schaefer atlas of 400 cortical regions, which includes a Yeo-Krienen network label for each node56.

Based on the Schaefer atlas, the WM-CPM included 208 edges in the positive masks. We assigned each of these edges to one of 28 within- or between-network Yeo-Krienan pairs.

Mediation analysis

To explore whether the relationship between genotype and working memory is mediated by the WM-CPM, we performed a mediation analysis. For each participant, the edges of WM-CPM were summed as mediating variable. Three general linear regression models were defined to test (1) the total effect of genotype on working memory, (2) the effect of genotype on WM-CPM, and (3) the direct effect of genotype on working memory, controlling for WM-CPM. The significance of the indirect effect of WM-CPM on the relationship between genotype and working memory was tested via the quasi-Bayesian Monte Carlo simulation as implemented in the “mediation” package in R. Specifically, 1000 simulations were performed to compute the 95% confidence interval of the average causal mediation effects.

Results

In our study, we assessed the impact of APOE genotypes on memory performance in young adults. Specifically, we evaluated working memory using the n-back task and short-term memory using the digit span test. For the working memory assessment, a total of 155 participants (43 males, mean age = 19.2 years, SD = 1.14 years) were included in the analysis. For the short-term memory assessment, 516 participants (153 males, mean age = 19.7 years, SD = 1.54 years) were included in the analysis. The demographic and performance data for each APOE group are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and performance data by APOE genotype.

Main effects of APOE genotypes on working memory

To explore the effect of APOE ε4 on memory, two ANCOVAs were performed on working memory (n-back task) and short-term memory (digit span assessment) separately. Significant effects were noted on working memory (F(2152) = 3.08, P < 0.05, Fig. 1A). The Tukey Post-Hoc tests indicated that group APOE 4 performed worse than group APOE3 in the n-back task (Fig. 1A). However, there was no significant effect on short-term memory (F(2513) = 2.13, p > 0.05, Fig. 1B).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Comparison of differences in memory among different APOE genotypes. (A) ANCOVA for working memory. (B) ANCOVA for short-term memory. (C) Robust analysis for working memory. *P < 0.05.

Considering the large differences in sample sizes between participant groups, we employed non-parametric resampling (10,000 times) procedures61 to prove the robustness of our results. The results showed that in 10,000 comparisons, group APOE4 performed better in fewer than 10 instances in working memory performance (P < 0.001, Fig. 1C). However, there is no significant difference between group APOE 4 and group APOE3 in short-term memory performance (p > 0.05, Fig. 1D).

A connectome-based predictive model predicts working memory across brains

To further investigate the differences in the functional neuroanatomical basis between different genotype groups, we used CPM59 to construct functional connectivity patterns from the whole-brain functional connections that could successfully predict working memory (n = 155, 43 males, mean age = 19.2 years, SD = 1.14 years). For each participant, we calculated the functional connectivity matrix within resting-state fMRI based on the whole-brain functional map of 400 nodes56. Within each cross-validation fold, we identified all node pairs (edges) exhibiting suprathreshold-level (P < 0.01) positive partial correlations with ACC of working memory (RT of working memory and mean frame-wise head motion were controlled). Based on the positive edge sum scores for each trial, we constructed a linear model to predict ACC of working memory based on all participants within a given cross-validation fold. For the held-out participant, we applied this linear model to compute predicted ACC of working memory. Then, we then correlated the predicted value with the observed ACC of working memory. Finally, we randomly shuffled the functional connectivity matrix 1,000 times and ran the above prediction pipeline for each time to obtain a null distribution of correlation coefficients between the predicted and observed scores to assess the significance (permutation test, Ppt ≤ 0.05, two-tailed) (Fig. 2B). We conducted the same analysis to explore edges exhibiting suprathreshold-level negative partial correlations with working memory.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Functional connectivity-based predictive modeling of working memory. (A) A linear model, based on summary scores, was used to correlate 155 predicted versus observed novelty ACC scores of working memory. (B) Correlation value (indicated with red line) between predicted and observed scores was compared with a null distribution of r values derived from 1000 permutations of shuffled functional connectivity matrix. (C) The perspectives of the top ten nodes contributing to the predictive functional connectivity pattern of working memory. (D) The number of edges, among those within the WM-CPM positive mask, assigned to each within- or between-network pair based on the Schaefer400 and Yeo-Krienen 7-network atlases. Nodes: IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; dmpfc = dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; dlpfc = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; STG = superior temporal gyrus; MTG = middle temporal gyrus. Networks: VIS = visual; SMN = somatomotor; DAN = dorsal attention; SAL = salience; LIM = limbic; FPCN = frontoparietal control; DMN = default mode.

For positive functional connectivity, the permutation results showed that CPM of working memory (r = 0.171, Ppt = 0.046) was effective (Fig. 2A). The edges contributing to the models included 208 edges positively associated with working memory. These edges of the WM-CPM were distributed widely throughout the brain, with high-degree nodes (i.e., nodes involved in multiple contributing edges) situated in parietal, temporal and prefrontal (Fig. 2C). For negative functional connectivity, the permutation results showed that CPM of working memory was not effective.

To better explain the functional neuroanatomical basis of patterns contributing to the WM-CPM, we examined the relationships with the functional networks previously associated with working memory. According to the Schaefer400 atlas, with each node assigned to 1 of 7 standard Yeo-Krienen networks56,60, we quantified the number of WM-CPM mask edges belonging to each intra- or inter-network pair.

The highest number of edges positively correlated with working memory (Fig. 2D) derived from Default mode network (DMN) between-network connections. The few network pairs that contributed the most to positive edges were the DMN-sensorimotor network (SMN), DMN-salience network (SAL), DMN-frontoparietal control network (FPCN) and the SMN-visual network (VIS). The DMN-SMN connections contributed the most positive edges (29 total pairs in total).

Mediation analysis

In previous analyses, we found a relationship between genotype and working memory and furthermore, we used CPM to construct whole-brain functional connectivity of working memory. In the final step, we analyzed whether the relationship between genotype and working memory is mediated by the WM-CPM.

To this end, using mediation analysis, we used genotype as the independent variable, working memory as the dependent variable and the sum of edges of the WM-CPM as the mediator variable to establish mediation model. We observed a significant indirect impact of the genotype on working memory through the mediation of the WM-CPM (average causal mediation effects = 0.014, p = 0.042, 95% CI  [0.000, 0.030], Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mediation analysis These mediation models show that the WM-CPM significantly mediate genotype and working memory. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. The paths (path a, b, c and c’) are labeled with path coefficients, and their standard errors are shown in parenthesis.

Discussion

By examining the differences in working memory and short-term memory among individuals with different APOE genotypes, the current study found that APOE ε4 carriers performed worse than the individuals with APOE ε3/ε3 genotype in terms of working memory, while no significant differences in short-term memory were found between them. Meanwhile, the resting-state brain functional connections that are closely related to working memory were identified using the CPM method. Subsequently, a mediation analysis was performed to verify the mediating role of these functional connections in the relationship between APOE genotypes and working memory performance. This study extends the knowledge on the relationship between APOE genotypes and cognitive functions in early adulthood and uncovers the underlying neural basis. These findings contribute to a better understanding of the role of APOE genotypes in influencing individuals’ lifelong cognitive development.

Notably, as mentioned numerous genetics studies62, the role of genotype in influencing the onset and development of individuals’ psychological behavior is complex and influenced by extensive environmental factors. In the present study analyzed the working memory performance of different gene carriers using an ANOVA and found differences among carriers. However, the existence of such differences cannot be taken as proof of the genotype’s determining role on working memory. Indeed, we tend to attribute this discrepancy to the influence role of genotype. That is to say, as in the case of executive function63 and attentional modulation64, genotypes also an influential factor on working memory, and this influence is clearly crucial. The decline in late-life cognitive function among APOE ε4 carriers is associated with structural brain changes65, and were more likely to develop AD66. A meta-analysis revealed that APOE ε4 carriers were at a disadvantage compared to non-carriers in terms of overall cognitive function, situational memory and executive function67. Therefore, in line with previous studies68, the present study found that APOE ε4 carriers performed worse in terms of working memory, which may be because APOE ε4 is associated with lower cognitive performance and higher risk of cognitive deficits. In addition, APOE ε2 was associated with a better short-term memory performance than APOE ε4 in our results, but the effect was not significant (Fig. 1B). However, this trend approaching a significant difference also seems to illustrate the protective effect of APOE ε2 on short-term memory.

CPM results show that resting-state brain functional connectivity, which is closely related to working memory, comes primarily from connections between the DMN and other networks. The next highest active network is the SMN. The DMN has been widely proven to be associated with working memory29,36,69,70,71. A study found that participants with reduced default network reactivity performed worse on a multilevel working memory task72. In addition, the DMN is also involved in a various of higher mental processes, including working memory, through flexible interactions with other networks73. Baddeley’s model of working memory identifies the phonological loop as a component of working memory74, a process that involves encoding verbal information and maintaining auditory information, which are both related to the SMN75. Besides, verbal working memory is reportedly related to the frontotemporal sensorimotor circuits76 and cerebellum77. Furthermore, the embodied cognition perspective suggests that many higher cognitive processes are closely related to perceptual and motor processes78 Therefore, the SMN may be involved in working memory by participating in information representation and manipulation79,80,81.

The results of the mediation analyses suggest that the WM-CPM mediates the relationship between the genotype and working memory scores. As found in previous studies, the genotype can influence specific mental behavioral performance by triggering certain changes in the brain82,83. Therefore, APOE ε4 may affect individuals’ working memory performance by altering the basis of brain functional connectivity that is closely related to working memory, including the DMN and SMN.

Our study has some limitations. First, the effects of a genotype on individuals are complex and may act in various ways; the present study revealed one possibility on how genes affect mental behavioral scores by influencing functional brain connectivity. However, this cannot be understood as a determinative causal relationship. It is not possible to enumerate all the possibilities of gene action on cognitive outcomes, or to include all environmental variables in the analyses. Second, a limitation of our study is the relatively small sample size, particularly among APOE ε4 carriers, with only 30 participants in our working memory analysis. This geographic and genetic limitation may affect the generalizability of our findings. Future research should expand the sample size and include participants from diverse regions, especially by recruiting more APOE ε4 carriers to provide a more accurate understanding of how APOE genotypes influence memory performance in young adults. Thirdly, while our study focused on short-term and working memory as key areas of interest, it did not cover the long-term memory domain, including episodic memory, which is notably affected in older APOE ε4 carriers. Future studies should extend their scope to include these additional memory domains to explore the full spectrum of memory functions influenced by APOE genotypes in young adults. Lastly, our study did not differentiate between APOE ε4 heterozygotes and homozygotes. Prior research indicates that the APOE ε4 allele’s impact may be dose-dependent, with ε4/ε4 carriers facing a higher Alzheimer’s disease (AD) risk and more significant cognitive and neural changes than ε3/ε4 carriers. A recent study by Fortea et al. identified ε4 homozygosity as a distinct genetic profile associated with elevated AD risk and altered biomarkers even in preclinical stages84. Future research should separately examine ε4 homozygotes and heterozygotes to clarify the effects of APOE ε4 dosage on cognitive function and its neural basis.