Abstract
Understanding the biological phenomena in model organisms typically relies on laboratory studies. However, the ecological validity of these findings is often uncertain when natural behaviors remain understudied. The medaka (Oryzias latipes) is a widely used model in reproductive and behavioral research; however, the timing of spawning in natural settings has rarely been directly observed. Recent fieldwork has suggested that medaka spawn several hours before sunrise, in contrast to the common assumption in laboratory-based studies that spawning occurs within an hour before or after exposure to light. In this study, we conducted continuous 24 h video recordings of medaka pairs under controlled laboratory conditions (14 L:10D photoperiod) to quantify diel variations in courtship and spawning behavior. Spawning occurred mostly between 08:00 and 11:00, peaking immediately after lights were turned on (08:00). Courtship behavior began during the dark period, increased before lights-on, and peaked between 07:00 and 09:00. These results exhibited a consistent delay of 3–4 h compared to (semi-)natural conditions. Our results highlight the influence of environmental conditions on reproductive timing in medaka. The temporal shifts observed in this study underscore the importance of incorporating natural temporal dynamics into experimental protocols to ensure accurate behavioral studies with model organisms.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Model organisms play a central role in elucidating biological mechanisms, often through studies conducted in highly controlled laboratories, which enable the precise control of environmental variables and facilitate fundamental discoveries in biology. However, the extent to which such findings generalize to natural environments remains uncertain, as these controlled conditions often omit key ecological factors, resulting in gaps in our understanding of behavior1,2. This disconnection raises critical concerns regarding the ecological validity and scientific rigor of laboratory-based research. Environmental differences between laboratory and field conditions can induce substantial shifts in behavioral and physiological responses. The relevance of observed patterns to natural systems becomes questionable when experimental settings fail to replicate key ecological variables2. In response, there is a growing recognition of the need for comparative studies in which behavioral and physiological traits are evaluated in both laboratory and field conditions3,4,5,6,7.
These concerns are particularly relevant for chronobiological research using model organisms, where laboratory-imposed light/dark cycles may distort natural timing mechanisms. Circadian rhythms and diel activity patterns are approximately 24 h biological rhythms driven by endogenous clock mechanisms, serving as fundamental regulatory mechanisms for physiological functions and behaviors across animal taxa8,9,10. In many fish, birds, and mammals, various behaviors including foraging, locomotion, predator avoidance, and reproductive activities are synchronized with environmental rhythms such as photoperiod and temperature. These behaviors are controlled by the interactions between internal clocks and external stimuli. Activity patterns such as nocturnal or diurnal behaviors are understood as adaptations to reproduction, predation pressure, and the availability of food resources8,9,10. Therefore, evaluating behavioral rhythms recorded under laboratory conditions within ecological contexts that consider natural conditions and adaptive backgrounds is essential for understanding animal behavior and its underlying mechanisms.
Despite its long history as a model organism, the ecological biology of medaka (Oryzias latipes), particularly its reproductive ecology, remains poorly understood in natural settings4. Medaka are widely used in physiology, genetics, developmental biology, behavioral science, and biomedical research due to their small body size, ease of rearing, pronounced sexual dimorphism, short generation time, transparent eggs, and a compact genome11,12. Due to the ease of observation, numerous studies on mate choice13,14,15,16, mate guarding behavior17,18,19, and alternative reproductive tactics20,21,22 have been conducted in laboratory settings. Importantly, medaka exhibit robust circadian rhythms that regulate physiological and behavioral processes, including reproduction, which are entrained by environmental light-dark cycles23,24,25,26,27,28. However, these features are the products of natural and sexual selection, and their functional significance cannot be fully understood without observations under ecologically relevant conditions.
Conventional laboratory studies have suggested that medaka typically initiate mating within 1 h before or after the onset of light29,30,31,32. However, these conclusions were largely drawn from indirect methods, such as the developmental staging of fertilized eggs, or visual observations restricted to daylight hours, leaving the exact timing of spawning initiation unresolved. Although laboratory studies have shown that ovulation in females is completed at night32, recent studies have demonstrated that general activity levels begin to increase several hours before the onset of light26,27. The results of our recent video-based observations conducted in the field and semi-natural settings have challenged this conventional understanding. In the field, post-spawning females were recorded as early as midnight, before sunrise at approximately 05:0028. Similarly, under semi-outdoor conditions, peak spawning occurred between 02:00 and 04:00, before sunrise at approximately 04:4533. These findings strongly suggest that spawning in natural environments begins several hours before sunrise, in contrary to the assumptions made in laboratory-based studies.
These findings prompt a critical reassessment of the timing and context of reproductive behavior in medaka and underscore the importance of integrating laboratory and field perspectives. Quantifying the behavioral differences between these settings is essential for optimizing experimental protocols that better reflect natural conditions and for designing studies that elicit ecologically meaningful behaviors. To date, there have been no published reports on the precise timing of spawning initiation in medaka in laboratory aquariums, and systematic comparisons with field data are limited. Bridging this gap is particularly crucial in model organism research, in which both scientific rigor and ecological validity must be maintained to ensure meaningful inferences in both the basic and applied sciences.
This study was designed to investigate the temporal dynamics of courtship and spawning in laboratory-reared medaka. To minimize confounding effects caused by genetic divergence, we used a uniform commercial strain (“himedaka”) commonly used in laboratory research. This allowed us to assess the environmental modulation of reproductive behavior by comparing 24 h behavioral observations conducted under controlled indoor and semi-outdoor conditions28,33. Specifically, 24 h behavioral observations were carried out to (1) identify the female spawning initiation time and (2) elucidate the temporal changes in the intensity of male courtship behavior. By comparing the results obtained in the present study with those of our previous field and semi-field observational studies28,33, we aimed to clarify the differences in courtship and spawning behaviors between laboratory and (semi-)natural environments and to examine the factors that affect these differences. Ultimately, the results of this study contribute to a more ecologically grounded interpretation of laboratory findings and inform the development of experimental protocols that incorporate natural temporal patterns.
Methods
Study fish and rearing
To ensure a direct comparison with our previous semi-outdoor study, the same Himedaka strain was selected from the same local pet shop in Kondo and Awata (2025)33. This commercially available strain is widely used in laboratory studies11,12 to enhance the reproducibility and comparability of our findings with those of other studies. These individuals were maintained under the following conditions for one month before being used in experiments: Eight breeding tanks (90.5 × 60.5 × 21.0 cm, length × width × height) were set up in the laboratory, with approximately 100 individuals housed in each tank. Water temperature was maintained at 26 ± 1 °C, and a 14 L:10D photoperiod was used (lights on at 08:00 and off at 22:00). Fish were fed Tetramin (Tetra, Melle, Germany) three times per day. Spawning activity was monitored daily during the acclimation period.
Experimental settings and video recording
Experiments were conducted from February 5 to 15, 2025, under the same temperature and photoperiod conditions as those described above, following the protocol described by Kondo and Awata (2025)33. Experimental tanks (22.5 × 15.8 × 5.5 cm; L × W × H) were filled with 950 mL of water, resulting in a depth of approximately 2.7 cm.
Each night, between 20:00 and 21:00, one male and one female from each breeding tank were selected and introduced into the experimental tank at 21:00. A total of 35 male–female pairs (2–4 pairs per day) were tested (Supplementary Table S1).
Continuous 24 h video recordings were performed using an AURORA PRO C011300 camera (SiOnyx, Beverly, MA, USA) equipped with a 512 GB SD card (SanDisk, Milpitas, CA, USA). Nighttime recordings (22:00–08:00) were conducted in infrared mode using 940 nm illumination (EnergyPower, Hong Kong, China), which falls outside the visual sensitivity range of medaka and does not affect their behavior34,35,36,37. Daytime recordings (08:00–22:00) were performed in standard mode. No food was provided during the experiments.
At the end of the 24 h period (21:00 the following day), the fish were anesthetized by immersion in a solution of FA100 (DS Pharma Animal Health, Osaka, Japan) diluted 1:2,000 (0.25 mL per 500 mL of water), and body mass was measured with an electronic balance (HT-120, A&D, Tokyo). After the measurements were taken, the individuals were returned to their original breeding tanks. Each animal was examined at least once.
Behavioral analysis
Video data were analyzed using ELAN version 6.8 annotation software. To assess diel behavioral variation, 10 min video clips were extracted from the 20–30 min segment of each hour, yielding 24 clips per pair of medaka. Previous studies have shown that spawning behavior in medaka typically follows a stereotypical sequence38: (1) following, in which the male follows the female; (2) quick circle, in which the male swims rapidly around the female; (3) wrapping, in which the male encircles the female with its dorsal and anal fins; (4) egg and sperm release; and (5) leaving the female. After spawning, females carry eggs attached to their abdomen and deposit them on the aquatic vegetation. Behavioral sequences during spawning, including egg and sperm release, were classified according to established criteria, and spawning time was defined as the moment of initiation of the spawning act, which was visually identified from the recordings33,39. Furthermore, based on previous work28,33, we quantified two courtship behaviors from each 10 min clip: (1) the total duration of the following and (2) the frequency of quick circles.
Statistical analysis
The mean body mass of males was 0.29 g (SD = 0.01; range: 0.21–0.39; n = 35), and that of females was 0.31 g (SD = 0.01; range: 0.23–0.50; n = 35; see Supplementary Table S1).
All statistical analyses were conducted using R version 4.4.1 (R Core Team 2024)40. To estimate peak spawning timing, we fitted a gamma distribution to the observed spawning events. Temporal changes in courtship behavior (following duration and quick-circle frequency) were analyzed using the generalized additive mixed models (GAMMs) implemented in the mgcv package. Behavioral data were quantified from 10 min video segments extracted hourly. For duration analysis, a Gaussian distribution was used. For the quick-circle frequency analysis, a negative binomial distribution was used to account for overdispersion. In both models, the time of day (hours) was included as a smooth function, and male ID was specified as a random intercept effect. Model significance was assessed using likelihood ratio tests, with statistical significance defined as p < 0.05.
Results
Spawning behavior
A total of 35 spawning events were recorded between 07:23 and 13:47 (Supplementary Movie S1). The majority (25 of 35 events, 71%) occurred between 08:00 and 11:00, with a distinct peak shortly after the lights were turned on at 08:00 (Fig. 1a; Supplementary Table S1). Twelve events (34%) occurred within 1 h of or after lights were turned on. In total, 31 spawning events (89%) occurred after light onset, whereas only four events (11%) occurred before light onset.
(a) Frequency distribution of spawning events observed in each hour over 24 h in medaka, Oryzias latipes (n = 35). The peak of spawning events was determined using gamma distribution fitting. (b) and (c) Changes in courtship behaviors from 21:00 to 20:00 over 24 h. (b) The total following duration (sec/10 min) and (c) the number of quick-circle behaviors (number/10 min). Each plot (green circle: pre-mating male; red triangle: post-mating male) signifies the observed values from the analyzed videos. The regression curves were based on the generalized additive mixed models (GAMMs) using all data (n = 840 observations from 35 males), and the shading indicates the 95% confidence intervals.
Courtship behavior
To examine diel variations in courtship behavior, we analyzed 24 h recordings of all 35 spawning pairs. “Following” was observed consistently throughout the day, with an average duration of 181.8 ± 6.0 s per 10 min video segment (n = 840 observations from 35 males; mean ± SE; Supplementary Movie S2). The following duration increased during the dark period, peaked between 07:00 and 09:00, and then decreased (GAMM: deviance = 4075648; df = 8.75; p < 0.0001; Fig. 1b).
“Quick circle” occurred 476 times in total, averaging 0.57 ± 0.07 occurrences per video segment (n = 840; Supplementary Movie S3). The temporal pattern of the quick circle mirrored the following: it increased before light onset, peaked between 07:00 and 09:00, and declined substantially after 12:00 (GAMM: deviance = 102.56; df = 7.71; p < 0.0001; Fig. 1c). Most post-mating males did not perform a quick circle after 12:00.
Discussion
Reproductive time is a key ecological trait that significantly influences fitness. In medaka, spawning has been reported to occur within 1 h before or after light exposure29,30,31,32; however, these conclusions were based on indirect observations rather than direct monitoring under dark conditions. In the current study, we provided the first direct evidence of spawning initiation and courtship rhythms under controlled laboratory conditions, using continuous video surveillance. Our findings are consistent with previous reports that spawning primarily occurs shortly after lights-on; however, we observed that courtship behavior begins during the dark period. This previously undocumented nocturnal initiation of courtship behavior represents a significant revision to the conventional understanding of medaka reproductive behaviors in laboratory settings.
Our 24 h continuous video monitoring methodology revealed that medaka initiate courtship activities during the dark period, a phenomenon that has not been previously reported in laboratory settings. Although it has been reported previously that the general activity level of medaka increases several hours before lights-on26,27, our detailed behavioral analysis has, for the first time, identified specific reproductive behaviors that drive the pre-dawn period. This methodological advancement allowed us to document the complete chronological sequence of reproductive behaviors, providing a behavioral foundation for previously unexplained activity patterns. Moreover, prior research has shown that medaka can spawn in the dark41 and that olfactory cues play an integral role in reproductive process42. The occurrence of nocturnal courtship suggests that medaka can detect and respond to reproductive cues via non-visual modalities, most likely by relying on olfaction.
A key finding of this study was the consistent 3–4 h delay in the timing of courtship and spawning behaviors in laboratory settings compared to field and semi-natural environments28,33, despite similarities in overall behavioral patterns. These results strongly indicate that environmental conditions play a primary role in modulating reproductive timing. Previous studies have shown that medaka populations from different geographic regions exhibit variation in the environmental cues that trigger reproduction, such as temperature and photoperiod43. Genetic analyses have identified candidate genomic regions associated with these circadian rhythms44, and additional studies have reported differences in fecundity among populations along with their genetic underpinnings45. These findings highlight the potential contribution of genetic background to the reproductive traits of medaka. However, in the current study, identical commercial medaka strains (himedaka) from the same source were used under both laboratory and semi-natural conditions33. Similar experimental procedures were applied, including 24 h video monitoring, comparable tank dimensions, and uniform behavioral analysis protocols. The main difference lies in the environmental conditions: artificial lighting with abrupt on/off cycles and constant temperature in the laboratory, versus natural photoperiod with gradual light transitions and fluctuating ambient temperatures in semi-natural conditions. The consistent behavioral delay observed under laboratory conditions, despite genetic and methodological uniformity, supports the conclusion that environmental factors, rather than genetic variation, are responsible for the observed temporal shift in reproductive behavior.
Several factors may explain these observed temporal shifts. The most immediate explanation involves a discrepancy between the artificial light cycle in the laboratory (lights on at 08:00 and lights off at 22:00) and the natural timing of sunrise and sunset. As medaka behavioral rhythms are highly sensitive to photoperiod, entrainment into this artificial schedule likely influences the timing of reproductive behaviors41,43,44. Given the known role of thyroid-stimulating hormones in the photoperiodic regulation of seasonal reproduction in birds, mammals46,47,48, and fish49, such shifts in photoperiodic signaling could also modulate reproductive timing in medaka.
However, photoperiod differences alone may not fully account for temporal shifts. In natural environments, ambient light intensity gradually increases during the pre-dawn period, whereas laboratory lighting typically operates on an abrupt on/off cycle. This qualitative difference may have affected the onset of behavioral expression. Diurnal temperature fluctuations may have also contributed to this phenomenon. Although natural habitats exhibit temperature minima during the night and early morning, our laboratory setup maintained water temperature at a constant 26 ± 1 °C. As temperature influences both physiological processes and behavioral rhythms across taxa, it is plausible that thermal stability affects spawning timing. Given that temperature has been identified as a critical cue in the seasonal reproduction of bony fish24,50, even endothermic species51,52,53, the absence of naturalistic thermal variability in laboratory settings may further distort reproductive timing.
This study has certain limitations. First, our findings were based solely on a single laboratory strain (himedaka), and caution should be exercised when extrapolating these results to wild populations. Second, although our results demonstrated temporal patterns in reproductive behavior, we did not assess the day-to-day repeatability of these behaviors in individuals. Repeated observations of the same individual over multiple days are valuable to evaluate the consistency and robustness of individual reproductive rhythms. Third, although we determined the timing of spawning, the exact timing of ovulation was not directly measured. Although previous studies have suggested that ovulation is completed at night32, the precise timing under laboratory conditions remains unknown. Further studies employing fine-scale hormonal or physiological monitoring are needed to elucidate the relationship between ovulation and courtship. Finally, the adaptive value of nocturnal reproduction, such as the avoidance of visual predators, warrants further investigation. Assessing the risk of predation on medaka and their eggs across diel cycles in natural environments would provide insights into the potential selective pressures shaping reproductive timing.
By showing that courtship initiation during the dark period occurs in both laboratory and field environments, albeit with a consistent time lag, this study highlights the need to bridge experimental and ecological contexts1,2. The 3–4 h delay in reproductive timing observed in this study exemplifies such a gap between laboratory and field conditions. These temporal mismatches may have far-reaching implications for experimental design, particularly for the investigation of diel variations in hormone secretion, gene expression, and reproductive behavior in medaka. Our findings suggest that sampling schedules and analytical frameworks should account for field-based temporal dynamics to avoid systematic biases in model organisms. In conclusion, our study demonstrates that medaka initiate reproductive behaviors earlier in (semi-)natural environments than in laboratory conditions, primarily because of differences in environmental cues such as light transitions and temperature. These findings underscore the need to integrate ecologically relevant parameters into experimental protocols involving model organisms.
Data availability
Data are included as Supplementary Information.
References
Heard, E. Molecular biologists: Let’s reconnect with nature. Nature 601, 9–9 (2022).
West-Eberhard, M. J. Animal behaviour and the new natural history. Anim. Behav. 221, 122791 (2024).
Alfred, J. & Baldwin, I. T. New opportunities at the wild frontier. Elife 4, e06956 (2015).
Hilgers, L. & Schwarzer, J. The untapped potential of medaka and its wild relatives. Elife 8, e46994 (2019).
Parichy, D. M. The natural history of model organisms: Advancing biology through a deeper Understanding of zebrafish ecology. Evol. Elife. 4, e05635 (2015).
Cheng, J., He, H., Zheng, L., Niu, H. & Zhang, H. Are the personality measures for laboratory mice applicable for wild rats? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 79, 1–11 (2025).
Neff, E. P. Where the wild zebrafish are. Lab. Anim. 49, 305–309 (2020).
Nakane, Y. & Yoshimura, T. Photoperiodic regulation of reproduction in vertebrates. Annu. Rev. Anim. Biosci. 7, 173–194 (2019).
Ikegami, K. & Yoshimura, T. Circadian clocks and the measurement of daylength in seasonal reproduction. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 349, 76–81 (2012).
Ikegami, K., Refetoff, S., Van Cauter, E. & Yoshimura, T. Interconnection between circadian clocks and thyroid function. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 15, 590–600 (2019).
Iwamatsu, T. The Integrated Book for the Biology of the Medaka (Daigaku kyouiku, 2018).
Kirchmaier, S., Naruse, K., Wittbrodt, J. & Loosli, F. The genomic and genetic toolbox of the teleost Medaka (Oryzias latipes). Genetics 199, 905–918 (2015).
Okuyama, T. et al. A neural mechanism underlying mating preferences for familiar individuals in Medaka fish. Science 343, 91–94 (2014).
Fujimoto, S., Miyake, T. & Yamahira, K. Latitudinal variation in male competitiveness and female choosiness in a fish: Are sexual selection pressures stronger at lower latitudes? Evol. Biol. 42, 75–87 (2015).
Fujimoto, S., Kawajiri, M., Kitano, J. & Yamahira, K. Female mate preference for longer fins in medaka. Zool. Sci. 31, 703–708 (2014).
Kondo, Y., Kohda, M. & Awata, S. Male medaka continue to mate with females despite sperm depletion. R Soc. Open. Sci. 12, 241668 (2025).
Yokoi, S. et al. Mate-guarding behavior enhances male reproductive success via familiarization with mating partners in medaka fish. Front. Zool. 13, 1–10 (2016).
Yokoi, S. et al. An essential role of the arginine vasotocin system in mate-guarding behaviors in triadic relationships of Medaka fish (Oryzias latipes). PLoS Genet. 11, 1–25 (2015).
Yokoi, S. et al. Sexually dimorphic role of oxytocin in medaka mate choice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 117, 4802–4808 (2020).
Kondo, Y., Kohda, M., Koya, Y. & Awata, S. Sperm allocation in relation to male–male aggression and courtship in an externally fertilizing fish, the medaka. Anim. Behav. 202, 9–19 (2023).
Koya, Y., Koike, Y., Onchi, R. & Munehara, H. Two patterns of parasitic male mating behaviors and their reproductive success in Japanese medaka, Oryzias latipes. Zool. Sci. 30, 76–82 (2013).
Grant, J. W. A., Casey, P. C., Bryant, M. J. & Shahsavarani, A. Mate choice by male Japanese medaka (Pisces, Oryziidae). Anim. Behav. 50, 1425–1428 (1995).
Yokota, T. & Oishi, T. Seasonal change in the locomotor activity rhythm of the medaka, Oryzias latipes. Int. J. Biometeorol. 36, 39–44 (1992).
Koger, C. S., Teh, S. J. & Hinton, D. E. Variations of light and temperature regimes and resulting effects on reproductive parameters in medaka (Oryzias latipes). Biol. Reprod. 61, 1287–1293 (1999).
Takahashi, T., Fujimori, C., Hagiwara, A. & Ogiwara, K. Recent advances in the understanding of teleost medaka ovulation: The roles of proteases and prostaglandins. Zool. Sci. 30, 239–247 (2013).
López-Olmeda, J. F. et al. Long photoperiod impairs learning in male but not female medaka. iScience 24, 102784 (2021).
Fujisawa, K. et al. Circadian variations in the liver metabolites of medaka (Oryzias latipes). Sci. Rep. 6, 20916 (2016).
Kondo, Y., Okamoto, K., Kitamukai, Y., Koya, Y. & Awata, S. Medaka (Oryzias latipes) initiate courtship and spawning late at night: Insights from field observations. PLoS One. 20, e0318358 (2025).
Takano, K., Kasuga, S. & Sato, S. Daily reproductive cycle of the medaka, Oryzias latipes under artificial photoperiod. Bull. Fac. Fish. Hokkaido Univ. 24, 91–99 (1973).
Ueda, M. & Oishi, T. Circadian oviposition rhythm and locomotor activity in the medaka, Oryzias latipes. J. Interdiscipl Cycle Res. 13, 97–104 (1982).
Weber, D. N. & Spieler, R. E. Effects of the light-dark cycle and scheduled feeding on behavioral and reproductive rhythms of the cyprinodont fish, medaka, Oryzias latipes. Experientia 43, 621–624 (1987).
Egami, N. Effects of exposure to low temperature on the time of oviposition and the growth of the oocytes in the fish, Oryzias latipes. J. Fac. Sci. Tokyo Univ. Sec Zool. 8, 539 (1959).
Kondo, Y. & Awata, S. Courtship and spawning behaviour of medaka in a semi-outdoor environment initiating at midnight. Sci. Rep. 15, 17057 (2025).
Homma, N., Harada, Y., Uchikawa, T., Kamei, Y. & Fukamachi, S. Protanopia (red color-blindness) in medaka: A simple system for producing color-blind fish and testing their spectral sensitivity. BMC Genet. 18, 1–10 (2017).
Shimmura, T. et al. Dynamic plasticity in phototransduction regulates seasonal changes in color perception. Nat. Commun. 8, 412 (2017).
Matsuo, M., Kamei, Y. & Fukamachi, S. Behavioural red-light sensitivity in fish according to the optomotor response. R Soc. Open. Sci. 8, 210415 (2021).
Matsuo, M., Ando, Y., Kamei, Y. & Fukamachi, S. A semi-automatic and quantitative method to evaluate behavioral photosensitivity in animals based on the optomotor response (OMR). Biol. Open. 7, bio033175 (2018).
Ono, Y. & Uematsu, T. Mating ethogram in Oryzias latipes. J. Fac. Sci. Hokkaido U Zool. VI Zool. 13, 197–202 (1957).
Kondo, Y., Kohda, M., Koya, Y. & Awata, S. Sperm allocation strategies depending on female quality in medaka (Oryzias latipes). Zool. Sci. 37, 203–209 (2020).
R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (Version 4.4.1) (2024).
Egami, N. Effect of artificial photoperiodicity on time of oviposition in the fish, Oryzias latipes. Annot Zool. Jpn. 27, 57–62 (1954).
Hayakawa, Y., Takita, S., Kikuchi, K., Yoshida, A. & Kobayashi, M. Involvement of olfaction in spawning success of medaka Oryzias latipes. Jpn. J. Ichthyol. 59, 111–124 (2012).
Shinomiya, A. et al. Variation in responses to photoperiods and temperatures in Japanese medaka from different latitudes. Zool. Lett. 9, 16 (2023).
Nakayama, T. et al. A transcriptional program underlying the circannual rhythms of gonadal development in medaka. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 120, e2313514120 (2023).
Fujimoto, S. et al. Evolution of size-fecundity relationship in medaka fish from different latitudes. Mol. Ecol. 33, e17578 (2024).
Ono, H. et al. Involvement of thyrotropin in photoperiodic signal transduction in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 18238–18242 (2008).
Nakao, N. et al. Thyrotrophin in the pars tuberalis triggers photoperiodic response. Nature 452, 317–322 (2008).
Yoshimura, T. et al. Light-induced hormone conversion of T4 to T3 regulates photoperiodic response of gonads in birds. Nature 426, 178–181 (2003).
Nakane, Y. et al. The saccus vasculosus of fish is a sensor of seasonal changes in day length. Nat. Commun. 4, 2108 (2013).
Huber, M. & Bengtson, D. A. Effects of photoperiod and temperature on the regulation of the onset of maturation in the estuarine fish Menidia beryllina (Cope) (Atherinidae). J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 240, 285–302 (1999).
van Rosmalen, L. et al. Mechanisms of temperature modulation in mammalian seasonal timing. FASEB J. 35, e21605 (2021).
Caro, S. P., Schaper, S. V., Hut, R. A., Ball, G. F. & Visser, M. E. The case of the missing mechanism: How does temperature influence seasonal timing in endotherms? PLoS Biol. 11, e1001517 (2013).
van Rosmalen, L., Riedstra, B., Beemster, N., Dijkstra, C. & Hut, R. A. Differential temperature effects on photoperiodism in female voles: A possible explanation for declines in vole populations. Mol. Ecol. 31, 3360–3373 (2022).
Percie du Sert. The ARRIVE guidelines 2.0: Updated guidelines for reporting animal research. PLOS Biol. 18, e3000410 (2020).
Acknowledgements
We extend our gratitude to the members of the Laboratory of Animal Sociology at the Osaka Metropolitan University for their valuable support throughout this study. We acknowledge Editage (www.editage.jp) for providing English language editing services.
Funding
This study was funded by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI (22K20666 to Y. Kondo and 23H03868 to S.A.), Sasakawa Scientific Research Grant from the Japan Science Society (2023–5013 and 2024–5010 to Y. Kondo), Tokyo Zoological Park Society Wildlife Conservation Fund (to Y. Kondo), and the Kurita Water and Environment Foundation (24H083 to Y. Kondo).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Y. Kondo, R. K., Y. Kobayashi and S. A. designed the study. Y. Kondo conducted aquarium experiments and collected data. Y. Kondo and S. A. performed the analyses. Y. Kondo, R. K., Y. Kobayashi and S. A. wrote and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethical standard
All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the ARRIVE guidelines54 and with the approval of Osaka Metropolitan University (Approval Number: S0092). All procedures involving animals adhered to the most recent ASAB/ABS ethical guidelines and were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the Osaka Metropolitan University, Japan. Housing and experimental methodologies were developed to minimize stress in medaka. After the experiment, the fish were returned to their original tanks. We did not observe any behaviors indicative of stress.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Supplementary Material 2
Supplementary Material 3
Supplementary Material 4
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Kondo, Y., Kobayashi, R., Kobayashi, Y. et al. Temporal dynamics of courtship and spawning in medaka under laboratory conditions revealed by 24 h video monitoring. Sci Rep 15, 26576 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-11082-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-11082-y