Introduction

Perceptual interactions between color and luminance have been described under various experimental conditions1,2,3,4,5. One method for studying these interactions is visual masking, which involves presenting spatial color noise over a luminance signal6,7. Miquilini et al.4 developed a mosaic stimulus in which a target defined by a contrast in luminance was masked by chromatic noise, allowing the study of the threshold discrimination of the masked target. It was found that the luminance threshold contrasts were influenced by the saturation of the colors present in the color noise, with lower threshold contrasts observed in conditions of low color saturation or in the absence of color noise, indicating a suppressive effect of chromatic noise on luminance discrimination.

The results of Miquilini et al.4 reveal a mechanism of interactions between color and luminance that has been evaluated under conditions of absent or diminished color vision in samples with congenital color vision loss or deterioration of color vision due to the natural aging process8,9.

Despite extensive literature describing that parallel visual processing pathways for luminance and color extend from the retina to the primary visual cortex without interaction between these information streams10,11,12,13,14, there is a growing body of literature that details cortical mechanisms of interaction between color and luminance3,15,16,17,18,19. There are cells in the primary visual cortex that respond only to color, others that respond only to luminance, and still others that respond to both color and luminance, which are thought to form the anatomophysiological substrate for the mechanisms of color and luminance interaction in the primary visual cortex16. Xing et al. (2015) identified the presence of interaction between brightness and color in the primary visual cortex through the recording of chromatic visual evoked potentials, this interaction arises in an inhibitory network generated by local brightness contrast at the boundary between the target and the background.

Moreover, recent evidence also indicates that the relationship between these mechanisms may not be restricted to the primary visual cortex but may involve higher-order cortical regions and be related to illumination processes and global scene analysis20, just as individual variations in luminance sensitivity can predict differences in chromatic axes21.

Various studies indicate different trends of development, maturation, and aging of the achromatic and chromatic visual pathways (red-green and blue-yellow) throughout life (e.g.,22,23,24,25). Studies using visual evoked potentials have assessed the development and maturation of the opponent pathways for luminance and color22,26,27. The results showed that the main components of the achromatic response are already mature by as early as 3 months, while the components of the chromatic responses continue to change throughout the first year.

Similarly, psychophysical studies25,28,29 have demonstrated the presence of chromatic discrimination in the red-green pathways as early as two months of age. However, findings regarding the development of the blue-yellow opponent system remain controversial, with some studies reporting discrimination at two months and others suggesting it emerges at later stages of development.

Psychophysical studies suggest a similar maturation of chromatic pathways starting around the age of 1024. Both electrophysiological and psychophysical studies have suggested that the performance of chromatic pathways in children only resembles that of adults after puberty, indicating a continuous development of color vision from childhood22,23.

Despite the existing knowledge about the development of chromatic and achromatic visual pathways, little is known about the maturation of mechanisms related to the interaction between these two visual subsystems. Considering that protocols for assessing luminance discrimination masked by chromatic noise represent mechanisms of interaction between color and luminance, they can be used to evaluate how chromatic and achromatic pathways interact across different age groups throughout life. In the present study, we compared luminance contrast discrimination under and without chromatic masking in adolescents, young adults, and older adults.

Methods

Ethical aspects

This research was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Pará (#6.546.671). All methods were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Consent from the legal guardians for the participation of minors in the study was obtained through the signing of an informed consent form, and the assent of the adolescents was obtained through their own signatures accompanied by consent of their legal guardians. Participants and their guardians were informed about the research objectives, the protocols to be applied, the potential benefits and risks involved, and the possibility of withdrawing from the study at any time according to their expressed wishes.

Participants

The present study recruited 20 adolescents aged 15–17 years old (mean age: 15.7 ± 0.8 years) and 20 young adult individuals aged 20–40 years old (mean age: 28.5 ± 9.8 years). Additionally, 20 older adults over 60 years (mean age: 60.2 ± 2.4 years) were included. All participants exhibited normal or corrected visual acuity of 20/20, assessed using the Freiburg Visual Acuity Test (FrACT), and had normal color vision based on the Ishihara pseudoisochromatic plate test30. Participants also had no systemic diseases that could influence vision, congenital or acquired color blindness, or frequent exposure to solvents. Individuals were recruited through direct invitation or referrals from selected subjects.

Luminance contrast discrimination test

The experiment was conducted using a MacBook Pro LCD 1700 (1690 × 1050 pixel spatial resolution, frame rate of 75 Hz, and 10-bit color resolution). A program was written in MATLAB R2017b (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA) to conduct the test. A CS-100A colorimeter (Konica Minolta, Osaka, Japan) was used to calibrate the monitor and for all color calculations in the test with all chromaticities calculated for a 2° observer angle and a D65 illuminant.

The test stimulus consisted of mosaics made up of 428 circles with diameters ranging from 0.12 to 0.49° of visual angle, viewed at a distance of 234 cm from the monitor. Sixteen different mosaic arrangements were created, with one of these arrangements randomly selected each time the stimulus was presented.

Four configurations of stimuli were presented similarly to previous studies by our group8,9. In three of the four conditions, a luminance contrast in the shape of a Landolt C (external diameter 4.4°, internal diameter 2.2°, gap 1°) was shown in the mosaic, masked by chromatic noise. Three types of chromatic noise were used (protan, deutan, tritan) (Fig. 1. Each type of chromatic noise consisted of 10 chromaticities with a vector magnitude of 0.04 u’v’ units, radially projected from a reference chromaticity (CIE 1976: u’ = 0.1947; v’ = 0.4639. To ensure a more uniform distribution of chromaticities along each confusion line, five chromaticities were spaced by 2 degrees in hue angle on either side of each color confusion line, resulting in five chromatic axes per protocol (see Fig. 2 for chromaticity distribution in the u’v’ color space). These axes were centered on the protan confusion line (protan protocol), the deutan confusion line (deutan protocol), and the tritan confusion line (tritan protocol). An additional condition without chromatic noise (non-noise protocol) was included, using only the reference chromaticity across the mosaic. These confusion lines represent directions in color space along which individuals with color vision deficiencies have difficulty discriminating chromatic differences. They correspond to the main axes of color confusion for protan (associated with L-cones), deutan (M-cones), and tritan (S-cones). These noise types were chosen because they align with the red-green and blue-yellow opponent channels, which are fundamental to human color perception.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Stimuli utilized in each experimental protocol: (A) protan, (B) deutan, (C) tritan, and (D) no-noise protocol. The luminance contrast of the stimuli was represented by a C-shaped target, with chromatic noise applied in protocols (A), (B), and (C) to mask the luminance contrast. In protocol (D), chromatic noise was absent.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Chromaticities used to compose the color noise for protan (red line), deutan (green line), and tritan (yellow line) in the CIE 1976 chromaticity diagram. The black crosses represent chromaticities spaced by 2 degrees in hue angle on either side of each color confusion line.

The initial luminance of the target was set at 4 cd/m2, while the background had a fixed luminance of 40 cd/m2. A four-alternative forced-choice method (up, down, right, left) was applied to feed a two down/one up adaptive staircase procedure, as seen in Sousa et al.9 and Brito et al.8, corresponding to approximately 71% correct responses. Briefly, an error would lead to a decrease in the target’s luminance, increasing the contrast relative to the background, while two correct responses would result in an increase in the target’s luminance, decreasing the contrast between target and background. A total of 10 reversals were considered for the completion of the test in each protocol, with the last six reversals used to calculate the threshold contrast.

More details on the methodology can be found in previous studies by our group that utilized similar approaches8,9.

Data analysis

To evaluate the effects of age group and protocol (protan, deutan, tritan, and achromatic) on estimated contrast thresholds, we applied Linear Mixed-Effects Models (LMMs). The main model included age group, protocol, and their interaction as fixed effects, with subject ID as a random effect to account for repeated measures. p-values were calculated using Type-III Wald chi-square tests. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were performed using estimated marginal means, and Tukey’s method was applied to correct for multiple comparisons. Model assumptions were evaluated through visual inspection of residuals using diagnostic plots along with the Shapiro–Wilk normality test and a Bartlett test of homogeneity of variance. Separate models were also fitted for threshold differences, following the same structure of fixed and random effects.

All statistical analyses and visualizations were performed using R software version 4.4.2, along with the following packages: lme4 for model building, lmerTest for p-value computation, car for Type-III Wald chi-square tests, performance for residual diagnostics, emmeans for post-hoc comparisons, and ggplot2 for data visualization. The significance level was set at α = 0.05.

For the statistical analysis, consistent with the approved work plan, the luminance discrimination thresholds in relation to the saturation of the color noise were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post hoc test to compare the estimated thresholds in the protan, deutan, tritan, and achromatic protocols of the luminance contrast discrimination test with color masking. A significance level of 0.05 was considered.

Results

The Fig. 3 displays box plots of the estimated luminance contrast thresholds across different stimulus conditions for each age group.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Comparison of luminance contrast thresholds across different stimulus conditions for each age group. Box plots illustrate the distribution of luminance contrast thresholds. Each color corresponds to the protocol under which the thresholds were measured: gray for the no-noise protocol, red for the protan protocol, green for the deutan protocol, and blue for the tritan protocol. *Luminance contrast thresholds are significantly lower than those estimated for all three protocols with chromatic noise (p < 0.05). **Luminance contrast thresholds in the tritan protocol are significantly lower than those measured in the protan and deutan protocols (p < 0.05).

Table 1 shows the results of the comparison of discrimination thresholds considering the factors of stimulation protocols and age group. The results indicated a significant interaction between the age group and stimulation protocol factors.

Table 1 Results of the Linear Mixed-Effects Model (LMM) for comparing luminance threshold across age groups and protocols.

Post-hoc comparisons showed that for all three age groups significantly lower thresholds were observed in the absence of noise compared to the other stimulus conditions. In the elderly group, not only were significantly lower thresholds noted in the absence of noise compared to the other stimulus conditions, but also a difference was found between thresholds estimated using the tritan protocol and those estimated using the protan and deutan protocols, with thresholds from the tritan protocol being lower than those from the other conditions with chromatic noise.

When considering the fixed factor of the type of stimulation protocol and comparing thresholds across age groups, it was observed that elderly participants exhibited higher threshold contrasts than both young adults and adolescents across all stimulation conditions (p < 0.05). In all stimulation conditions with the presence of chromatic noise, the adolescent group also demonstrated higher threshold contrasts compared to the adult group (p < 0.001); however, this was not observed in the no-noise condition, where adolescents and adults presented threshold contrasts without significant difference (p = 0.738). Figure 4 shows the comparison of luminance thresholds among the different age groups within each stimulation condition.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Intergroup comparison of luminance contrast thresholds across different age groups within each protocol. *Luminance contrast thresholds in older participants were significantly higher than those in adolescent and adult participants (p < 0.05).

When assessing the difference between the thresholds estimated in conditions with and without chromatic noise, it was observed that there was a significant effect for the factors age and stimulation protocol, but no significance for the interaction of these factors, as seen in Table 2. Adults exhibited smaller differences in thresholds between the conditions with and without noise compared to participants in the adolescent and elderly groups (p < 0.05). Additionally, a significant difference was noted between the threshold differences when considering the protan and tritan protocols (p = 0.007).

Table 2 Results of the Linear Mixed-Effects Model (LMM) for comparing luminance threshold differences between conditions with and without chromatic noise across age groups and protocols.

Discussion

This study presented results on the interaction between color and luminance across different age groups (adolescents, adults, and elderly individuals), showing that the mechanisms of color-luminance interaction differ across these age ranges. The findings indicate that the presence of chromatic noise significantly elevated the luminance discrimination thresholds in all age groups, with the largest increase observed in the adolescent and elderly groups. This pattern of results reflects the complexity of the development and aging of the visual systems responsible for processing luminance and color information, which is consistent with previous studies conducted by our research group.

Our group has previously investigated the influence of chromatic masking on luminance threshold discrimination using mosaic stimuli4,8,9. The findings indicate that discrimination thresholds are strongly modulated by the saturation of colors in the noise4, Sousa et al., 2020), and are significantly elevated under mosaic conditions compared to non-mosaic conditions4.This effect may reflect the perceptual strategy employed by observers, as mosaic patterns likely require a scanning process to identify the C-gap orientation. This process could lead to loss of information within the target stimulus, as proposed by Watanabe et al.31. Additionally, the elevated thresholds may also be influenced by the task demands, which involve integrating visual information across spatially distributed elements. This integration likely engages visual grouping mechanisms, and the interaction of these processes with chromatic noise may impose additional perceptual demands, further contributing to the increased thresholds observed.

In previous investigations, we explored how age might affect color-luminance interaction mechanisms, finding that especially under blue-yellow chromatic noise conditions, aging can interfere with chromatic suppression on luminance discrimination. The current study extended the investigation of color-luminance interaction to adolescents. In addition to confirming results previously observed in Brito et al.8 for adult and elderly data, we showed here that adolescents differ significantly from adults in quantitative parameters indicating the interaction between color and luminance.

The similarity in luminance threshold discrimination between adolescents and adults in the absence of chromatic noise suggests that the achromatic mechanism is fully developed, as indicated by earlier studies22,26,27,32. However, it was observed that when exposed to chromatic masking, adolescents exhibited greater chromatic suppression on luminance threshold discrimination than adults, as evidenced by higher threshold contrasts in as evidenced by higher threshold contrasts in the color noise stimulation conditions. This indicates that the mechanisms of color or the interaction between color and luminance in adolescents’ brains have not yet reached full development, as suggested by some studies22,23,24,33.

These findings are also corroborated by studies like those by Fiorentini et al.34, who demonstrated that aging leads to a general decline in contrast sensitivity for both luminance and chromatic signals. In our study, we observed a significant increase in luminance discrimination thresholds in the presence of chromatic noise, especially in elderly individuals, which may be explained by the deterioration of the visual pathways responsible for processing these signals over time. This suggests that the impact of aging on visual perception mechanisms, both for luminance and color, may be interconnected, and the interaction between these mechanisms may be similarly affected with age.

The study by Richardson et al.21, on the other hand, highlighted that individual differences in spectral sensitivity can influence both luminance and chromatic processing. Their findings suggest that these variations may impact the interaction between chromatic axes and luminance, which aligns with our observation that sensitivity to chromatic noise may be mediated by individual differences, especially between adolescents and adults. This interdependence between luminance and color is an important characteristic for understanding how different age groups process chromatic and achromatic stimuli.

Furthermore, Conway20’s research on the role of S-cones in the visual pathways, particularly in the dorsal and ventral cortical areas, offers a crucial perspective for understanding our findings. Conway argues that the activation of S-cones, involved in chromatic contrast perception, is essential for analyzing illumination variations, such as shadows. Aging and the incomplete development of the visual system during adolescence may affect the ability to perceive these illumination variations, which would explain the differences in luminance discrimination thresholds observed between adolescents and adults.

These results indicate that while the achromatic mechanisms are already consolidated in adolescence, the chromatic systems and the interaction between color and luminance continue to develop throughout adolescence and into adulthood, as suggested by Knoblauch et al.23 and Paramei & Oakley33. The gradual deterioration of chromatic sensitivity observed in aging may impact luminance discrimination, particularly under chromatic masking conditions, as observed in our study.

There is an agreement in the literature that throughout adolescence, the brain undergoes significant changes in its volume, gray and white matter, plasticity, and neurotransmitter systems35,36,37. More specifically regarding the visual pathways, studies show that the ventral temporal cortex, which processes information related to faces, size, and color, may undergo important changes during adolescence, such as reduced volume compared to the same region in adults38. Although we do not have direct anatomical or physiological data from the brains of the adolescents tested, the structural and biochemical differences in the adolescent brain may potentially indicate changes in the mechanisms of color and luminance interaction that we have been studying.

A limitation of the present study is the restricted age range of adolescent participants, which was 15–17 years. Future studies should include a broader age range within childhood and adolescence to determine whether this finding represents a continuum of developmental changes. In addition, the differences observed in luminance discrimination thresholds may be attributed to general differences in noise sensitivity rather than specific interactions between color and luminance. Further investigations are needed to explore this possibility, particularly by considering different types of masking.

The mechanisms of color and luminance interaction studied through color masking on the luminance threshold perception show potential for evaluating adolescent neurodevelopment, paving the way for the development of new evaluative methods for assessing the integrity of pathways across different age groups.

Conclusion

The present study, by comparing luminance contrast sensitivity with and without color masking, allowed for a better understanding of how interactions between chromatic and achromatic pathways occur throughout different life stages, extending the investigation to the adolescent age range.

The findings reinforce the hypothesis that the mechanisms of interaction between color and luminance undergo changes throughout life, highlighting the occurrence of a decrease in the suppressive effect of color on the luminance contrast threshold discrimination between adolescence and adulthood. This result suggests that while the achromatic mechanisms (related to luminance perception) are fully developed in adolescence, chromatic mechanisms are still maturing during this phase and may later show a decline due to aging. These findings are important for understanding how visual development and aging impact visual perception and pave the way for future research on visual neurodevelopment and age-related sensory processes.