Abstract
Although emotions are regarded as essential in automatic cyberbullying detection, the nuanced links between emotion types and roles remain underexplored. The dynamics of cyberbullying are therefore somewhat ambiguous. To address these issues, we analyzed the emotional patterns and connections between five cyberbullying roles (bullies, outsiders, assistants, defenders, and reporters) on a Chinese social media platform. Six emotions were extracted from 11,601 comments using a large pre-trained model for affective computing. Through epistemic network analysis, this study identified three co-occurrence patterns of emotional expressions among these roles, namely, anger-dominated negative pattern, happiness-anger conflicting pattern, and surprise-fear moderate pattern. Beyond just Angry, three emotions (Fearful, Happy, and Surprised) varied significantly among nearly all roles. In addition to the valence of emotions, the position of these roles within the overall network may also be associated with different levels of emotional arousal. Results of subtracted networks for three role pairs further indicated that these emotional co-occurrences may help identify roles for their perceptions, judgments, and intentions regarding others. These insights hold promise for enhancing targeted bullying detection and intervention.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The growing intensity of cyberbullying on social media platforms such as Twitter, Instagram, and Sina Weibo continues to stoke anxiety worldwide (Chan et al., 2021; Zhong et al., 2021). Cyberbullying not only poses grave threats to the well-being of citizens, with detrimental effects including poor self-esteem, low academic performance, and potential suicidality at the individual level (Cowie, 2013), but also impacts the social fabric. For example, it is often accompanied by negative emotions, such as anger, fear, and sadness (Xu et al., 2012a), which disrupts the positive climate of social media and public trust and beliefs towards other groups, society and government (Costello et al., 2023; Papcunová et al., 2023). Efforts to prevent cyberbullying have increased in these years. Yet it remains challenging to identify cyberbullying content within the vast array of social media texts because of its dynamic, context-sensitive and unstructured nature (Dani et al., 2017; Zhong et al., 2022). Recent progress has been made in understanding cyberbullying through the study of communication mechanisms and social behavior patterns on social media, including revealing the psychological process of different roles (Pornari and Wood, 2010; Zhu et al., 2023) and communication mechanisms on social media (Cañas et al., 2020). Building on these advancements, three research areas that necessitate finer-grained analysis are being developed: (1) the context-sensitive linguistic expression in social media, (2) the behavioral characteristics of the various roles within the cyberbullying scenario, and (3) the interplay between them. Such studies are critical to understanding the social complexity of cyberbullying and developing targeted intervention strategies.
The complexity of cyberbullying extends beyond the language to include the diverse and evolving roles involved in cyberbullying incidents (Bowler et al., 2015), which shape patterns of emotional expression. Like traditional bullying, there are three major players in cyberbullying scenarios: bullies, victims, and bystanders (Wang, 2021). Among these, bystanders (the largest group) can further be split into subcategories (e.g., González-Cabrera et al., 2019; Rathnayake et al., 2020; Y. Xu, 2021), critically shaping the evolution of cyberbullying and its impacts on victims (Polanco-Levicán and Salvo-Garrido, 2021). However, users’ roles are not static nor mutually exclusive. Besides, victims and bystanders may even respond with abusive language (Rathnayake et al., 2020). In such cases, the emotions embedded in the language of the roles become similar, highlighting the need to differentiate nuanced variations in emotions (e.g., which kind of emotion is emphasized and how it interacts with other emotions). Yet most research oversimplified the sentiment analysis (i.e., positive or negative emotions), ignoring how role-specific emotion clusters drive collective behavior (e.g., Dani et al., 2017; Choi et al., 2021; Lian et al., 2022). This obscures our focus on the emotional patterns linked to various roles in cyberbullying and, moreover, how these patterns sustain cyberbullying.
This oversight should be valued given the social effects of emotions: individuals gather relevant information from others’ emotions and make decisions (van Kleef and Côté, 2022). They tend to align their emotions with those of the group in social media (Gordijn et al., 2006; van Kleef and Côté, 2022). On social media, such a similarity in emotional responses often manifests as a role-specific pattern, helping in the prediction of emotional expression and cognitive changes in a broader group. However, it remains unclear how these roles vary with regard to emotional expression due to their complex stances and behaviors. As van Kleef & Côté (2022) suggested, behavioral implications of emotions depend on individual (e.g., identity) and situational characteristics (e.g., conflict stage), rather than emotional valence (positive or negative). This revelation exposes a critical limitation in cyberbullying research: while prior studies predominantly analyzing negative emotions (e.g., anger frequency), they glossed over the role-specific emotional profiles.
Building on this premise for viewing online roles as temporary groups, we posit that cyberbullying participants are characterized not only by role-specific linguistic features but also by similar emotions. Acknowledging the richness of involved emotions, we describe patterns of emotional expressions as co-occurrence networks of emotions and structural properties. This study first reviewed existing classifications and extracted five roles instead of only a few roles as has been done elsewhere (Jacobs et al., 2022; Rathnayake et al., 2020). Rather than focusing on the role played by individuals in social processes, as in social network analysis (Gašević et al., 2019; Choi et al., 2021), we attempted to reveal associations and patterns between multiple dynamic concepts (Shaffer et al., 2016; Rolim et al., 2019) by epistemic network analysis (ENA). We further identified how this emotional information explains the behavioral patterns of different roles based on Intergroup Emotional Theory (IET) and Emotional Empathy Theory (EASI). We believe that uncovering more granular features enriches the understanding of participants’ roles in cyberbullying and promotes more effective preventive measures. The following research questions (RQs) informed this effort:
-
RQ1: What are the distinct patterns of emotional expressions exhibited by each cyberbullying role?
-
RQ2: How do patterns of emotional expressions differ across cyberbullying roles?
Participants’ roles in cyberbullying
In the context of cyberbullying on social media platforms, users can be categorized into various participant roles according to their participation behaviors or intentions. All these roles should be valued, rather than solely focusing on the bullies and victims (Bowler et al., 2015). Bystanders play multidimensional roles in cyberbullying, ranging from constructive to aggressive intervention, which cannot be conceptualized as a unidimensional construct (Moxey and Bussey, 2019). Scholars have classified different bystanders using multiple approaches. Salmivalli (1999) argued that, in traditional bullying, bystanders span four categories: assistants of the bully, reinforcers of the bully, defenders, and outsiders. J.-M. Xu et al. (2012b) analyzed bullying on social media and pointed out three bystander groups: reporter, accuser, and other. Psychologists have labeled bystander roles based on questionnaires. For instance, Schultze-Krumbholz et al. (2018) performed latent class analysis and identified prosocial defenders, communicating outsiders, aggressive defenders, bully-victims, and assistants. González-Cabrera et al.’s (2019) categorization echoed that of Salmivalli (1999), although defenders consisted of both the victim’s defenders and supporters. Other studies have uncovered a pair of bystander roles (i.e., bystander-assistant and bystander-defender) via machine learning. It is still challenging to differentiate bullies from aggressive bystanders when detecting cyberbullying roles, as this task normally calls for additional context (Jacobs et al., 2022; Rathnayake et al., 2020).
In light of extant work, we divided cyberbullying into the following six categories, which have covered the types of roles in existing studies: bullies, victims, outsiders, the bully’s assistants (i.e., assistants), the victim’s defenders (i.e., defenders), and reporters. Outsiders consist of people who ignore or distance themselves from cyberbullying incidents. Bullies’ assistants do not initiate bullying but encourage or even exacerbate it (Salmivalli, 1999; González-Cabrera et al., 2019). Defenders are bystanders who stand up for victims or attempt to stop bullying (Jacobs et al., 2022; Rathnayake et al., 2020). Reporters refer to individuals who report bullying incidents on social media (Xu et al., 2012a) and hold a more neutral attitude towards involved parties.
Emotions in cyberbullying
Social media platform enables users to gather around common goals or identities, thereby forming temporary groups and sharing emotions (Wakefield and Wakefield, 2024). These role clusters are not maintained by formal rules or explicit goals, but by the alignment of emotions. The groups, as IET and many researchers suggested, cultivate shared emotional group norms through divergent social identities and cognitive appraisals (Smith, 1993; Smith and Mackie, 2015; Mackie and Smith, 2018), which direct further attitudes and behaviors. Specifically, the emotions of individuals playing specific cyberbullying roles will be easily affected by the intergroup, ultimately converging on a group emotional pattern for a given role (Parkinson, 2020). For example, when counter-empathic emotions (e.g., schadenfreude) are propagated between groups, they can trigger more social media engagement in groups, which may lead to social media antisocial behaviors, such as cyberbullying (Wakefield and Wakefield, 2024). In a nutshell, IET provides an irreplaceable premise for conducting role-based research, as it reveals how group identities integrate dispersed individual users into clusters of roles with stable emotional patterns.
Despite these close associations, limited studies have explored descriptive and interpretive results of discrete emotions in cyberbullying scenarios (e.g., Balakrishnan & Ng, 2022; J. Xu et al., 2012b). The commonly used categorization is Ekman’s (1992) six major types of emotions, including anger, disgust, fear, joy, sadness, and surprise and Plutchik’s (2001) wheel of emotion, which includes two additional feelings, acceptance and anticipation. Due to social media’s mechanisms, these emotions can be widely expressed and spread through various forms, including aggressive texts, images and emojis (Hettiarachchi and Ranasinghe, 2019), most of which were found to be negative, such as anger and depression (Baroncelli and Ciucci, 2014; Den Hamer and Konijn, 2016; Vranjes et al., 2017; Li and Peng, 2022).
The effects of different emotions on social media vary according to previous observations. Posts expressing intense but diverse emotions, particularly negative ones like anger, disgust, fear, and guilt (Chawla et al., 2022), tend to disseminate more rapidly and widely in both Chinese and English contexts (Stieglitz and Dang-Xuan, 2013; Zhang and Qu, 2020; Chen et al., 2023a). Such different functions can be explained using EASI. This theory posits that emotions affect cyberbullying through a dual pathway: (1) via automatic affective reactions to others’ emotional expressions; (2) via inferential processing of the meaning and implications of the emotional expresser (van Kleef and Côté, 2022). Heerdink et al. (2015) found that anger triggered more exclusion compared to neutrality and happiness. Anger for a particular event can be interpreted as a sign of injustice by others, leading to greater attributions of agency and responsibility to a third person (van Doorn et al., 2015). Furthermore, studies argue that the impact of emotions on users’ information sharing may depend on their level of arousal, rather than valence alone (Song et al., 2016; Pivecka et al., 2022). Arousal provides more information, such as urgency or importance of an incident, influencing observers’ interpretation and thus their behaviors (Storbeck and Clore, 2008; Berger, 2011; van Kleef and Côté, 2022). For example, high arousal may increase accessible judgments, including stereotyped judgments of individuals seen by raters as members of an out-group (Storbeck and Clore, 2008).
However, user posts may present multiple discrete emotions. It is unclear how the coordination of complex emotions affects role behavior. More importantly, even the same emotions work differently in different posts. For example, Jang et al. (2022) identified two groups of Twitter users who expressed negative emotions towards vaccination: those who used it to discourage vaccination, and those who promoted vaccination while criticizing the public health response. Clearly, users’ emotional patterns may vary depending on their intention and stance, or role in an incident. Quantifying the sentiment of cyberbullying posts without differentiating between participant roles can make it challenging to draw meaningful conclusions.
Although emotional patterns among roles in cyberbullying are not explicitly outlined, initial observations suggest distinct trends. Offensive roles, like bullies and their assistants, often express anger, fear, and sadness through accusatory and abusive posts, sometimes disguised with positive emotions (Choi et al., 2021; Lu et al., 2022; Zhong et al., 2022). Victims, however, may not consistently express emotions across different coping strategies, such as seeking support, defending themselves, or seeking revenge (Wright, 2016; Balakrishnan, 2018). Defenders and reporters are positive participants in a role aiming to stop cyberbullying. Defenders actively assist and comfort victims, using positive language to encourage them (Nickerson et al., 2008; Alim and Khalid, 2019). Reporters, on the other hand, may express sadness, sympathy, neutral or other negative emotions to drive the discussion or question the responsible parties (Gu et al., 2021). Outsiders tend to respond more passively or apathetically, reflecting a more objective stance (Jia et al., 2022).
Methods
We applied a four-phase method to explore the emotional characteristics of cyberbullying roles on Chinese social media, as shown in Fig. 1.
Data collection and preparation
Our experiment utilized a corpus of three representative cyberbullying incidents from China’s largest platform for open discussions, Sina Weibo. With mechanisms similar to Twitter, it provides clear hashtags related to an incident, which is ideal for gathering relatively comprehensive data on cyberbullying (Li, 2020). The chosen cases coincided with prior criteria (Zhong et al., 2022), concerned three spheres of daily life: society, sports, and entertainment. The societal case involved an ambiguous sexual assault allegation, surrounded by rumors about women’s safety. The sports case involved gender-biased remarks towards a sports celebrity, sparking public dialog on gender issues. Lastly, the entertainment case was a hate attack triggered by an entertainment star’s depression diagnosis. These incidents have been among the most widely discussed on Sina Weibo over the past five years and have ignited considerable public concern (with over 140 million reads and over 30,000 discussions). Despite contextual differences, they represented typical cyberbullying topics that can be easily found across cultures. They then exhibited ambiguity that drew diverse audiences into discussions, improving the complexity and role diversity of cyberbullying. By analyzing roles across these cases, this study could identify common emotional patterns in cyberbullying, undetectable in isolated case studies, with limited data.
A Python-based crawler was developed to jointly download posts and comments about each incident. Microblog text, usually containing multimedia information, requires special handling for data storage and annotation. We thus established a cleaning pipeline including: (1) removing duplicates, whitespaces, hyperlinks, unknown characters, user mentions, and HTML tags; (2) converting traditional Chinese into simplified Chinese; and (3) retaining emojis with matching words. Eventually, we assembled a raw corpus comprising 11,601 posts.
The data used in this study were collected exclusively from publicly accessible posts and comments on Sina Weibo, in compliance with the platform’s Terms of Service. To protect user privacy, all identifiable information (e.g., usernames, profile links, geolocation tags) was removed prior to analysis, and aggregated results were reported to prevent re-identification. No content from private accounts or restricted communities was included. Data collection protocols adhered to institutional guidelines for ethical use of public social media data.
Cyberbullying roles and human coding
As mentioned, we named six cyberbullying roles, all of which represented illocutionary acts based on the posts’ context. Victims’ emotional patterns were not analyzed. Victims are typically defined as individuals “who are the target of repeated harassment” (Salmivalli, 1999) or who experience being harassed. We viewed these persons as the heart of an incident (i.e., by being the people who were initially bullied) rather than as persons who others verbally attacked during an incident. Victims’ posts would also have been difficult to process because few victims regularly speak out on social media.
Rather than coding at the user-level, our coding was conducted at the post-level, i.e., the coding of a complete comment, excluding the commenter’s account details for privacy reasons. That is, in our study, cyberbullying roles were categorized based on their behavioral intent reflected in individual posts rather than being statically assigned to user accounts. Based on our coding scheme (listed in Table 1), five independent coders with a uniform understanding of the three cyberbullying incidents were trained to determine which role each post reflected. Specifically, we implemented a three-stage strategy for this manual coding task. In the first stage, we conducted a two-round consistency test, which included coder training and intercoder agreement discussions. We specifically used Holsti’s coefficient to evaluate inter-rater consistency (Holsti, 1969), which accounts for both agreement and disagreement across multiple coders and categories. It is considered to be useful when coding requires holistic judgments of context, such as in discourse analysis or frame interpretation (Nili et al., 2020). The coefficient in the second round was 0.93, indicating high consistency among annotators. In the second stage, each coder was assigned to annotate 2320 posts independently over a one-month period. The coding proceeded sequentially by case, with posts from each case randomized before being presented to the coders. In the final stage, we conducted circular member-checking, where each coder was assigned to cross-check the annotations of another coder in a closed loop. This approach not only reduced individual workload but also mitigated potential biases, enhancing the overall quality of the annotations. Our final dataset contained 11,601 labeled instances. Table 2 displays the basic statistics for these data.
Fine-tuning ERNIE 3.0 for emotion detection
To handle social media posts with multiple emotions, we treated emotion detection as a multi-label classification task for short text. Challenges included the exponential explosion of label combinations and the computational costs associated with constructing brief-text classification models (Gibaja and Ventura, 2015). Few studies have extracted diverse emotions from Chinese social media in this manner. However, recent advances in text-based emotion detection, particularly with transformer-based models (Acheampong et al., 2021), have shown promising results with improved efficiency and reduced dependency on labeled data. We therefore fine-tuned a transformer-based model to enhance fine-grained emotion detection.
This goal was realized through three steps: (1) selecting a transformer-based model; (2) feeding a corpus of social media posts into this model for fine-grained emotion detection; and (3) evaluating emotion detection from social media. The ERNIE 3.0 pretrained model (Zhang et al., 2019; Sun et al., 2021) is founded on auto-encoding transformers. It was trained using a 4TB corpus consisting of plain text and a large-scale knowledge graph. Compared with BERT and GPT, ERNIE 3.0 has demonstrated superior performance on Chinese NLP tasks. We fine-tuned ERNIE 3.0 on the public corpus of Wang et al. (2014) to adapt its parameters for social media emotion detection. The fine-tuned ERNIE 3.0 was then employed to classify multiple labels in our sample using a simple discriminant function. A pragmatic assumption of this function is that an emotional label will be assigned to a social media post when its predicted probability is greater than the threshold of 5% (Storey and Tibshirani, 2003; Read et al., 2011; Liu and Chen, 2015; Alotaibi and Flach, 2021). To ensure consistent performance of the fine-tuned ERNIE 3.0 in our research context, we implemented a domain adaptation framework inspired by Ben-David et al.‘s (2010) theoretical work on dataset shift mitigation. We constructed an independent test set rigorously partitioned from our dataset (stratified 20% split, n = 2320). Consequently, evaluation on the test set revealed encouraging results with an average precision of 77.7%, a recall rate of 77.2%, and an F1 score of 77.3%. We fully acknowledge that an over 20% misclassification rate might introduce certain biases in the analysis of role-specific emotional patterns. To address this, we performed bootstrap resampling with 1000 iterations on our dataset to construct 95% confidence intervals. Results confirmed that the significance levels (p < 0.05) of emotional differences between roles remained stable despite misclassification errors (Appendix has more details). This indicates that the misclassification did not fundamentally alter the statistical significance of our findings, thus not leading to significant discrepancies in the conclusions and ensuring the robustness of the results.
Data visualization and analysis
After fine-grained emotion detection, we statistically analyzed the data and applied ENA to address our RQs. For RQ1, we first summarized the counts of emotional expressions for each role and plotted an “affective flower” chart. Specifically, to mitigate potential bias from highly active users, we calculated the proportion of each emotional expression type relative to each role’s total post count, thereby normalizing frequency measurements and reducing overrepresentation risks. Then, we performed a pairwise multiple comparison test, Conover’s test, to uncover differences in emotional expressions across roles. Here, basic emotions and cyberbullying roles were regarded as dependent variables and independent variables, respectively. This is one of the most powerful procedures for post hoc evaluation (Conover and Iman, 1979) and has been implemented in a Python package, scikit-posthocs (Terpilowski, 2019).
For RQ2, ENA was exploited to detect fine-grained differences that existed in emotional patterns among roles. ENA is capable of analyzing discourse data in groups and revealing associations between multiple dynamic concepts (Rolim et al., 2019; Shaffer et al., 2016). This capability contributes to its popularity for extracting cognitive patterns from social media data. Misiejuk et al. (2021) theorized that integrating sentiment analysis in ENA allowed for a clearer understanding of how groups saw the components of a stimulus issue by separating the elements by sentiment. We entered our data into the ENA web tool. First, we chose six fundamental emotions as codes, with each cyberbullying role serving as a unit of analysis. The stanza was then set to 1, given our interest in cumulative connections among these emotions. Nodes within our epistemic network represented basic emotions, and link thickness indicated co-occurrence frequency; the thicker the link, the more often the emotions co-occurred.
Results
Distributions and differences in emotions among cyberbullying roles
To discern the distributional characteristics of cyberbullying roles among the six emotions, emotion analysis was performed on posts typifying each role to analyze the associated feeling. Figure 2 depicts the results. The value in the center of an affective flower represents the emotion evenness index (EEI; Huang et al., 2020). Basic emotions were evenly distributed within a specific role. Each flower’s oval petal represents the number of posts in which a particular emotion appeared. Expressions of the chosen emotions varied across roles. Unsurprisingly, Angry was most pronounced among bullies, with a similar pattern among assistants and reporters. Surprised, Neutral, and Fearful were most common among outsiders. Happy was prevalent for defenders, followed by Angry and Neutral. Sad ranked the lowest across all roles except for reporters.
As shown in Fig. 3, Conover’s test was carried out to ascertain potential significant differences between roles tied to these emotions. For Fearful, Happy, and Surprised, all roles exhibited a significant difference (p < 0.05) except for bullies and assistants. Significant differences between roles were largely apparent for Angry, Fearful, Happy, and Surprised; fewer pairs of significant differences accompanied Neutral. Regarding Angry, no significant difference was observed in bully–assistant and reporter–assistant pairs. Significant differences in Sad were observed for most pairs, except for bully–assistant, outsider–defender, and outsider–reporter. Variation was also evident between outsiders’ neutral emotions and those of other roles.
Epistemic networks of emotions for cyberbullying roles
Overall characteristics of epistemic network
Figure 4 shows the overall network for the six emotions based on five roles. Along the x-axis, the space is distinguished by Angry on the left and Happy on the right. The y-axis is defined by Surprised at the bottom and Angry at the top. Based on these distributions of emotions and roles, the y-axis may represent how strongly different emotions contribute to cyberbullying behaviors, distinguishing more active roles from passive ones. For example, the roles situated above the x-axis are more aggressive (i.e., showing Angry), being the major contributors of cyberbullying, while outsiders who were not involved in cyberbullying show the lowest y-values. It seems that the x-axis is related to emotional valence (Berger and Milkman, 2012), where most negative emotions are on the left and positive emotions on the right, except for Sad. Dots denote emotions, and lines denote connections between these feelings. Larger dots and thicker lines indicated more prominent emotions and stronger connections, respectively. In the overall epistemic network, connections between Angry and other discrete emotions (particularly Surprised, Neutral, and Fearful) were stronger compared to other emotion pairs (e.g., Sad-Neutral), evident from the concentration of lines on the left side of Fig. 4.
Figure 5 visualizes the centroids of different roles in the ENA space. These centroids’ average values are displayed as squares, and the 95% confidence interval for each dimension is shown as a rectangle. Each centroid considers the weights of connections between emotions and can be depicted as a matching plotted point (Bressler et al., 2019). Obviously, the co-occurrence pattern of conflicting emotions among defenders (red) resulted in a greater occupancy in the ENA space with the centroid to the left of the x-axis. Conversely, the emotions of outsiders (purple) appear to be concentrated in a smaller area. Moreover, the centroids of bullies (yellow) and assistants (green) nearly overlap, with similar confidence interval sizes, suggesting a high degree of consistency in their emotional expressions.
Figure 6a–e present each role’s epistemic network. The lines link different points (emotions), such that the thicker the connecting line, the greater the co-occurrence frequency of the associated emotions. Bullies, assistants, and reporters similarly expressed emotions stronger associations between negative emotions, whereas reporters were less associated with Happy than the other two roles. Specifically, defenders primarily displayed a conflicting emotion co-occurrence pattern. Outsiders tended to express themselves neutrally.
Differences in role categories
When examining networks, it is useful to compare a pair of network models by subtracting their connection weights from each other to create a difference network graph (i.e., a “subtracted network”). Our graph provided additional insight into the distinctions among cyberbullying roles, as pictured in Fig. 7.
The subtracted networks illustrate emotional differences and co-occurrences among roles, shedding further light on parties’ behavioral motivations around cyberbullying. For brevity, we focus on three key pairs that showed the most significant differences in all discrete emotions except Neutral: bully–assistant, defender–assistant, and outsider–reporter. Figure 7c shows that the subtracted network of bullies and assistants had thin edges, implying a slight difference between their networks. The bullies’ emotional network contained stronger connections between Angry and Sad, while the assistants’ network had stronger links between Angry and Fearful, Angry and Neutral, and Fearful and Neutral. Figure 7h reveals that the defenders’ network mainly occupied the right side of the axis, with stark connections between Angry and Happy and Neutral and Happy. Defenders’ emotions were generally positive, while assistants frequently appeared Angry and Fearful or Surprised. Figure 7g indicates that outsiders’ emotions were easily distinguishable from those of reporters. Angry and Surprised, and Angry and Fearful appeared more frequently in the defenders’ network. Neutral and Surprised, Fearful and Neutral, and Fearful and Surprised were more common among outsiders.
Discussion
The picture of emotions in cyberbullying: more than anger and valence
The emotional expressions of different roles are complex and multifaceted. Angry, Fearful, Happy, and Surprised varied significantly among nearly all roles, while Sad was comparatively less prevalent and less likely to vary significantly among roles. These findings align with the popular view of Angry, Fearful and Sad on social media (Xu et al., 2012a; Baroncelli and Ciucci, 2014; Li et al., 2020; Chawla et al., 2022; Zhong et al., 2022).
Our overall epistemic network further indicated that emotional valence may be an unreliable indicator of cyberbullying, which is consistent with research by van Kleef & Côté (2022) that the positive or negative impact of emotions depends on individual and situational characteristics, not just their valence. Findings suggested that Angry, which exhibits high levels of arousal, contributed prominently to cyberbullying. Conversely, low arousal emotions such as Sad and Surprised had a weaker association with cyberbullying. Thus, we presume that the arousal level of emotions may also be linked to core and non-core roles in cyberbullying. Emotions with higher valence and arousal were found to last longer on microblog (Sener et al., 2023). Anger is considered a dominant high-arousal negative emotion, leading to more engagement in harassing, bullying or hurting perceived threats, whereas fear may result in passive, harmful responses, such as ignoring and avoiding these threats (Clobert et al., 2022). As for the unexpected result for Sad, it could be attributed to posts filled with sadness tend to be less shared and disseminated on social media (Chawla et al., 2022). Therefore, future research should broaden its focus to include other high-arousal emotions like Fearful and Happy, and explore their correlations.
Three types of emotional patterns: bridging IET and EASI
As reviewed above, IET posits that shared social identities (e.g., ‘bully’ as a transient identity) bring emotion convergence on social media (Mackie and Smith, 2018). This convergence, we argue, implies that different discrete emotions may manifest distinct patterns of co-occurrence with different roles. The phenomenon of group emotional convergence then tends to strengthen internal cohesion, allowing group members to develop a consensual interpretation of future events (Smith and Mackie, 2015). This essentially involves the mechanism proposed by EASI—whether the users will have automatic affective processing or deliberate inferential analysis. Notably, the type of emotion may affect the depth of processing (Tiedens and Linton, 2001). Studies indicated emotions associated with uncertainty, such as sadness, produced more thorough processing, while anger emotions with clear attributional directions may result in heuristic processing and stereotype use (Tiedens and Linton, 2001; Small and Lerner, 2008; Tugade et al., 2014). Together, we believe these two theories provide a useful lens for decoding behavioral patterns across different cyberbullying roles.
In the networks of emotion co-occurrence among different roles, three types of patterns were identified, including an anger-dominated negative pattern, a happiness-anger conflicting pattern, and a surprise-fear moderate pattern:
-
(1)
Anger-dominated negative pattern was observed in bullies, assistants and reporters, with Angry being the most prevalent, appearing alongside other negative emotions, in line with previous studies (Mackie et al., 2004, p. 200; Baroncelli and Ciucci, 2014). Reporters, as interveners, may perceive crisis incidents on social media as threats to fairness (Zhang et al., 2018), forming group norms emphasizing condemnation of injustice. They might vent negative emotions, especially anger marked by clear attribution and high arousal level. They enable reporters to rapidly assign responsibility and advocate accountability (Small and Lerner, 2008), as reflected in their concise and punitive comments. In contrast, threat-induced fear and anxiety, characterized by low certainty and minimal emotional polarization (Renström et al., 2023), conflicted with their action goals.
-
(2)
Happiness-anger conflicting pattern was found in defenders. This finding correlates with the two defender behavioral patterns observed in the corpus. From an IET perspective, defenders may simultaneously contain two subgroups with different norms: one required to stand up for the victim and the other to confront bullies. Specifically, the former may adopt a strategy of solidarity with victims, conveying happiness or empathy for emotional support, while the latter may express anger to advocate for justice and counteract cyberbullying (Vranjes et al., 2017; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018). This dual identity could cause conflicting emotions, reflected in the largest confidence intervals in the ENA space. Furthermore, defenders frequently employed sarcasm over direct aggression, reflecting Chinese social media’s prevalent implicit cyberbullying strategies of mockery and ridicule (Zhong et al., 2022).
-
(3)
Surprise-fear moderate pattern, which presented more Surprised, Neutral and Fearful, with these emotions showing co-occurrences with Happy and Angry, respectively. This pattern was identified in outsiders who were conceptualized as neutral observers in this study. They regularly expressed surprise at aggressive comments due to not expecting such behavior. Mameli et al. (2022) came to similar conclusions. According to IET (Mackie and Smith, 2018), their emotional expressions were more balanced, rather than convergent, as they were unaffiliated with either group involved in an incident. These detached identities enabled outsiders to keep themselves from being emotionally drawn into a particular group. Instead, they expressed a certain degree of rationality (as indicated by neutral emotions).
Fine-grained variations among cyberbullying roles
We compared each pair of roles and highlighted three pairs with similar social effects (i.e., bully-assistant, defender-assistant, and outsider-reporter). The findings indicate that analyzing emotional co-occurrences can reveal the positive or negative social effects of emotions in distinct roles, as well as their behaviors and intentions, aiding in role identification. This differentiation helps to distinguish between similar roles.
Key findings revealed significant emotional overlap between bullies and assistants, as evidenced by centroid proximity and confidence interval convergence. Drawing on IET and EASI (Mackie and Smith, 2018; van Kleef and Côté, 2022), this alignment may arise from shared group identification: assistants, by supporting bullies, adopted similar group norms and maintained a unified stance toward the same goal. This also explains why interventions targeting only bullies or haters often fail, as the emotional pattern persists through assistants’ unconscious compliance. Nevertheless, their different co-occurrence between Angry and other negative emotions facilitated their differentiation. This result may be due to their different aggressive behaviors. Sad in bullies’ epistemic network may stem from public reprimand for aggressive behavior by one party to the incident (Pornari and Wood, 2010) or from realizing someone had been hurt (Mameli et al., 2022). In contrast, assistants’ anger often co-occurred with Fearful, Surprised, and Neutral, as they usually conceal themselves within groups, projecting objectivity and impartiality but in fact share alarming comments to encourage aggression and cyberbullying (Snakenborg et al., 2011).
Being supporters of the victim and cyberbullying, respectively, comparing the epistemic networks of defenders and assistants offered valuable insights into two distinct approaches to cyberbullying intervention. The assistant displayed an aggressive style, which increased conflict by negative emotions, while defenders showed more pro-social behaviors by comforting and explaining to the victims. Previous studies revealed that expressing more pleasant emotions can promote positive social effects, such as pro-social bystander behaviors (Miles and Upenieks, 2018; García-Vázquez et al., 2020; van Kleef and Côté, 2022). Therefore, their epistemic network mainly occupied the right side of the axis with clear connections (i.e., Happy-Angry and Happy-Neutral), suggesting predominantly positive emotions. On the contrary, unpleasant feelings about cyberbullying expressed by assistants compel people to support victims (Costa Ferreira et al., 2022). This transmission of anger may boost assistants’ level of influence and promote cyberbullying (Barsade, 2002; Sarmiento et al., 2019).
As for the outsider-reporter comparison, although they both engaged in the cyberbullying events as observers, their emotional co-occurrences varied. For outsiders, Neutral, Surprised, and Happy have a strong co-occurrence between them, while reporters’ emotional responses were predominantly negative, consistent with J.-M. Xu et al.’s (2012b) study. Except for the low perceived threat, some users may be afraid to post negative comments about others and become the target of bullies’ revenge. These circumstances can impede outsiders from intervening in cyberbullying (Wang, 2021; Costa Ferreira et al., 2022). Reporters were apt to express anger in order to motivate others to exert more cognitive energy attending to the unjust news (Stevens et al., 2021). Such expressions also have the potential to instigate change or extract concessions in conflict (van Kleef and Côté, 2022).
From patterns to interventions: suggestions for cyberbullying prevention
These emotion-based patterns not only deepen our understanding of interpersonal dynamics in cyberbullying but also offer some practical suggestions for prevention. The high involvement of anger-based negative emotions in most roles has again highlighted the importance of anger (Rathje et al., 2021). However, focusing solely on anger may overlook posts from defenders and reporters, whose roles could differentially influence the trajectory of cyberbullying. In addition, the discussion on the similar emotional patterns exhibited by assistants and bullies indicated that effective interventions must weaken the shared stance that binds them into a cohesive unit. As noted earlier, prior cyberbullying research has predominantly focused on anger detection in isolation. Consequently, automated detection systems often demonstrate suboptimal performance owing to their inability to incorporate crucial contextual cues. The observation of co-occurrence patterns between anger and other high-arousal emotions suggests that emotion pairs—rather than isolated emotions—could serve as potential features for identifying cyberbullying content.
Conclusion and implications
This paper has presented a novel exploration of emotional patterns among five roles in cyberbullying scenarios. We leveraged the ERNIE 3.0 model for emotion detection and ENA for data visualization. Our analysis of 11,601 comments about three cyberbullying incidents on Chinese social media revealed three emotional patterns among five cyberbullying roles: bullies, assistants and reporters presented an Anger dominated negative pattern, namely Angry co-occurred with Surprised and Fearful; defenders showed a Happiness-Anger conflicting pattern and outsiders displayed a Surprise-Fear moderate pattern with balanced distribution of different emotions in their networks. Furthermore, although the overall epistemic network featured more robust connections to negative emotions for all groups, participant roles in cyberbullying may not only depend on valence but also be related to arousal level. Discussions were presented on the basis of IET and EASI, which collectively explained why roles developed distinct emotional patterns (IET) and how these differences correlated with role-specific behaviors (EASI). Ensuing fine-grained analysis of the subtracted networks for three role pairs (i.e., bullies-assistants, defenders-assistants, and outsiders-reporters) showed variations in these pairs’ emotional co-occurrence, facilitating further understanding of their perceptions, inferences, and intentions related to cyberbullying incidents.
This study offers valuable insights into the diverse roles in cyberbullying, contextualizing interpersonal interaction through emotional patterns (e.g., how aggressors may respond to others or how bystanders evaluate ongoing incidents). Our application of two theories to role identities in cyberbullying can also transfer to other online contexts, such as emotionally polarized political discussion and crisis-driven public debates. Moreover, the findings also highlight the role of emotions in cyberbullying prevention and governance. For instance, interventions should not solely target bullies; assistants, who often mimic and thereby extend bullies’ negative emotional influence, require attention as well. A psychosocial approach, considering affective factors, may inform targeted preventative measures. Lastly, further studies can focus on aspects of group cohesion and collective cognitive evaluation in cyberbullying and establish associations with intense co-occurring emotions rather than discrete emotions.
Limitations
The current study has several limitations. First, our dataset is limited in its ability to capture the full spectrum of cyberbullying variations, including cross-cultural and cross-platform differences, where larger and more diverse samples are often required. This suggests the need for enhancing AI-assisted methods to balance data size and reliability. Implementing hybrid human-AI validation frameworks could strengthen emotion classification reliability (Chen et al., 2023b). Besides, it must be acknowledged that the combination of human and machine methods does not preclude the possibility of a single post with multiple intentions, and that misclassification of the model would affect our conclusions to some extent. Third, our application of IET and EASI remains at a theoretical level. Empirical validation, such as time-series analysis or longitudinal tracking of individuals, is needed to examine specific causal mechanisms. Nonetheless, the findings of this exploratory study offer a foundational framework for tackling real-world intervention challenges.
Data availability
Raw data and the emotional coding results can be found in the GitHub link: https://github.com/kimpink98/Cyberbullying-Post-Role-Dataset.
References
Acheampong FA, Nunoo-Mensah H, Chen W (2021) Transformer models for text-based emotion detection: a review of BERT-based approaches. Artif Intell Rev 54:5789–5829. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10462-021-09958-2
Alim S, Khalid S (2019) Support for cyberbullying victims and actors: a content analysis of Facebook groups fighting against cyberbullying. Int J Technoethics 10:35–56. https://doi.org/10.4018/IJT.2019070103
Alotaibi R, Flach P (2021) Multi-label thresholding for cost-sensitive classification. Neurocomputing 436:232–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucom.2020.12.004
Balakrishnan V (2018) Actions, emotional reactions and cyberbullying—from the lens of bullies, victims, bully-victims and bystanders among Malaysian young adults. Telemat Inform 35:1190–1200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2018.02.002
Balakrishnan V, Ng SK (2022) Personality and emotion based cyberbullying detection on YouTube using ensemble classifiers. Behav Inf Technol 0:1–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2022.2116599
Baroncelli A, Ciucci E (2014) Unique effects of different components of trait emotional intelligence in traditional bullying and cyberbullying. J Adolesc 37:807–815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.05.009
Barsade SG (2002) The ripple effect: emotional contagion and its influence on group behavior. Adm Sci Q 47:644–675. https://doi.org/10.2307/3094912
Ben-David S, Blitzer J, Crammer K, Kulesza A, Pereira F, Vaughan JW (2010) A theory of learning from different domains. Mach Learn 79:151–175. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10994-009-5152-4
Berger J (2011) Arousal increases social transmission of information. Psychol Sci 22:891–893. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611413294
Berger J, Milkman KL (2012) What makes online content viral? J Mark Res 49:192–205. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.10.0353
Bowler L, Knobel C, Mattern E (2015) From cyberbullying to well-being: a narrative-based participatory approach to values-oriented design for social media. J Assoc Inf Sci Technol 66:1274–1293. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.23270
Bressler DM, Bodzin AM, Eagan B, Tabatabai S (2019) Using epistemic network analysis to examine discourse and scientific practice during a collaborative game. J Sci Educ Technol 28:553–566. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-019-09786-8
Cañas E, Estévez E, Martínez-Monteagudo MC, Delgado B (2020) Emotional adjustment in victims and perpetrators of cyberbullying and traditional bullying. Soc Psychol Educ 23:917–942. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-020-09565-z
Chan TKH, Cheung CMK, Lee ZWY (2021) Cyberbullying on social networking sites: a literature review and future research directions. Inf Manag 58:103411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.im.2020.103411
Chawla S, Mehrotra M, Rani S (2022) Understanding the dynamics of Cyberbully content diffusion using Textual Emotion Mining. In: 2022 IEEE 7th International Conference for Convergence in Technology (I2CT). IEEE, Mumbai, India, pp. 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1109/I2CT54291.2022.9824773
Chen Y, Chen AX, Yu H, Sun S (2023a) Unraveling moral and emotional discourses on social media: a study of three cases. Inf, Commun Soc 0:1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2023.2246551
Chen Y, Clayton EW, Novak LL, Anders S, Malin B (2023b) Human-centered design to address biases in artificial intelligence. J Med Internet Res 25:e43251. https://doi.org/10.2196/43251
Choi Y-J, Jeon B-J, Kim H-W (2021) Identification of key cyberbullies: a text mining and social network analysis approach. Telemat Inform 56:101504. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2020.101504
Clobert M, Sasaki J, Hwang K-K, Tsai JL (2022) Valuing high arousal negative states increases negative responses toward outgroups across cultures. Emot (Washington, DC) 22:1450–1472. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001101
Conover WJ, Iman RL (1979) Multiple-comparisons procedures. Informal report. Los Alamos National Lab. (LANL), Los Alamos, NM (United States) https://doi.org/10.2172/6057803
Costa Ferreira Pda, Veiga Simão AM, Martinho V, Pereira N (2022) How beliefs and unpleasant emotions direct cyberbullying intentions. Heliyon 8:e12163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2022.e12163
Costello M, Vishwamitra N, Liao S, Cheng L, Luo F, Hu H (2023) COVID-19 and Sinophobia: detecting warning signs of radicalization on Twitter and Reddit. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 26:546–553. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2022.0200
Cowie H (2013) Cyberbullying and its impact on young people’s emotional health and well-being. Psychiatrist 37:167–170. https://doi.org/10.1192/pb.bp.112.040840
Dani H, Li J, Liu H (2017) Sentiment informed cyberbullying detection in social media. In: Ceci M, Hollmén J, Todorovski L, Vens C, Džeroski S (eds). Machine learning and knowledge discovery in databases. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Springer International Publishing, Cham, pp. 52–67. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71249-9_4
Den Hamer AH, Konijn EA (2016) Can emotion regulation serve as a tool in combating cyberbullying? Personal Individ Differ 102:1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.033
Ekman P (1992) Are there basic emotions? Psychol Rev 99:550–553. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.550
García-Vázquez FI, Valdés-Cuervo AA, Martínez-Ferrer B, Parra-Pérez LG (2020) Forgiveness, gratitude, happiness, and prosocial bystander behavior in bullying. Front Psychol 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02827
Gašević D, Joksimović S, Eagan BR, Shaffer DW (2019) SENS: Network analytics to combine social and cognitive perspectives of collaborative learning. Comput Hum Behav 92:562–577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.07.003
Gibaja E, Ventura S (2015) A tutorial on multilabel learning. ACM Comput Surv 47:52:1–52:38. https://doi.org/10.1145/2716262
González-Cabrera JM, León-Mejía A, Machimbarrena JM, Balea A, Calvete E (2019) Psychometric properties of the cyberbullying triangulation questionnaire: a prevalence analysis through seven roles. Scand J Psychol 60:160–168. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12518
Gordijn EH, Yzerbyt V, Wigboldus D, Dumont M (2006) Emotional reactions to harmful intergroup behavior. Eur J Soc Psychol 36:15–30. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.296
Gu M, Guo H, Zhuang J (2021) Social media behavior and emotional evolution during emergency events. Healthcare 9:1109. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare9091109
van Kleef GA, Côté S (2022) The social effects of emotions. Annu Rev Psychol 73:629–658. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-020821-010855
Hettiarachchi H, Ranasinghe T (2019) Emoji powered capsule network to detect type and target of offensive posts in social media. In: Proceedings of the International Conference on Recent Advances in Natural Language Processing (RANLP 2019). INCOMA Ltd., Varna, Bulgaria, pp. 474–480. https://doi.org/10.26615/978-954-452-056-4_056
Holsti OR (1969) Content analysis for the social sciences and humanities. Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 264 pp
Huang Y, Li J, Wu G, Fei T (2020) Quantifying the bias in place emotion extracted from photos on social networking sites: a case study on a university campus. Cities 102:102719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cities.2020.102719
Jacobs G, Van Hee C, Hoste V (2022) Automatic classification of participant roles in cyberbullying: can we detect victims, bullies, and bystanders in social media text? Nat Lang Eng 28:141–166. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135132492000056X
Jang H, Rempel E, Roe I, Adu P, Carenini G, Janjua NZ (2022) Tracking public attitudes toward COVID-19 vaccination on Tweets in Canada: using aspect-based sentiment analysis. J Med Internet Res 24:e35016. https://doi.org/10.2196/35016
Jia Y, Wu Y, Jin T, Zhang L (2022) How are bystanders involved in cyberbullying? A latent class analysis of the cyberbystander and their characteristics in different intervention stages. Int J Environ Res Public Health 19:16083. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192316083
Li L, Wang Z, Zhang Q, Wen H (2020) Effect of anger, anxiety, and sadness on the propagation scale of social media posts after natural disasters. Inf Process Manag 57:102313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2020.102313
Li W (2020) The language of bullying: social issues on Chinese websites. Aggression Violent Behav 53:101453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2020.101453
Li W, Peng H (2022) The impact of strain, constraints, and morality on different cyberbullying roles: a partial test of Agnew’s general strain theory. Front Psychol 13:980669. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.980669
Lian Y, Zhou Y, Lian X, Dong X (2022) Cyber violence caused by the disclosure of route information during the COVID-19 pandemic. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 9:1–18. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-022-01450-8
Liu SM, Chen J-H (2015) A multi-label classification based approach for sentiment classification. Expert Syst Appl 42:1083–1093. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eswa.2014.08.036
Lu S, Zhao L, Lai L, Shi C, Jiang W (2022) How do Chinese people view cyberbullying? A text analysis based on social media. Int J Environ Res Public Health 19:1822. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19031822
Mackie DM, Silver LA, Smith ER (2004) Intergroup emotions: emotion as an intergroup phenomenon. In: Tiedens LZ, Leach CW (eds) Intergroup emotions: emotion as an intergroup phenomenon. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 227–245
Mackie DM, Smith ER (2018) Intergroup emotions theory: Production, regulation, and modification of group-based emotions. In: Advances in experimental social psychology. Adv Exp Social Psychol. Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 1–69
Mameli C, Menabò L, Brighi A, Menin D, Culbert C, Hamilton J, Scheithauer H, Smith PK, Völlink T, Willems RA, Purdy N, Guarini A (2022) Stay safe and strong: characteristics, roles and emotions of student-produced comics related to cyberbullying. Int J Environ Res Public Health 19:8776. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19148776
Miles A, Upenieks L (2018) An expanded model of the moral self: Beyond care and justice. Soc Sci Res 72:1–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2018.02.004
Misiejuk K, Scianna J, Kaliisa R, Vachuska K, Shaffer DW (2021) Incorporating sentiment analysis with epistemic network analysis to enhance discourse analysis of Twitter data. In: Ruis AR, Lee SB (eds) Advances in quantitative ethnography. Springer International Publishing, Cham, pp. 375–389
Moxey N, Bussey K (2019) Styles of bystander intervention in cyberbullying incidents. Int J Bullying Prev 2:6–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42380-019-00039-1
Nickerson AB, Mele D, Princiotta D (2008) Attachment and empathy as predictors of roles as defenders or outsiders in bullying interactions. J Sch Psychol 46:687–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2008.06.002
Nili A, Tate M, Barros A, Johnstone D (2020) An approach for selecting and using a method of inter-coder reliability in information management research. Int J Inf Manag 54:102154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2020.102154
Papcunová J, Martončik M, Fedáková D, Kentoš M, Adamkovič M (2023) Perception of hate speech by the public and experts: insights into predictors of the perceived hate speech towards migrants. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 26:489–498. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2022.0191
Parkinson B (2020) Intragroup emotion convergence: beyond contagion and social appraisal. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 24:121–140. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868319882596
Pivecka N, Ratzinger RA, Florack A (2022) Emotions and virality: social transmission of political messages on Twitter. Front Psychol 13:931921. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.931921
Plutchik R (2001) The nature of emotions: human emotions have deep evolutionary roots, a fact that may explain their complexity and provide tools for clinical practice. Am Scientist 89:344–350. https://doi.org/10.1511/2001.4.344
Polanco-Levicán K, Salvo-Garrido S (2021) Bystander roles in cyberbullying: a mini-review of who, how many, and why. Front Psychol 12:676787. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.676787
Pornari CD, Wood J (2010) Peer and cyber aggression in secondary school students: the role of moral disengagement, hostile attribution bias, and outcome expectancies. Aggress Behav 36:81–94. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20336
Rathje S, Van Bavel JJ, van der Linden S (2021) Out-group animosity drives engagement on social media. In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. e2024292118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2024292118
Rathnayake G, Atapattu T, Herath M, Zhang G, Falkner K (2020) Enhancing the identification of cyberbullying through participant roles. In: Proceedings of the Fourth Workshop on Online Abuse and Harms. Association for Computational Linguistics, Online, pp. 89–94. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/2020.alw-1.11
Read J, Pfahringer B, Holmes G, Frank E (2011) Classifier chains for multi-label classification. Mach Learn 85:333–359. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10994-011-5256-5
Renström EA, Bäck H, Carroll R (2023) Threats, emotions, and affective polarization. Political Psychol 44:1337–1366. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12899
Rolim V, Ferreira R, Lins RD, Gǎsević D (2019) A network-based analytic approach to uncovering the relationship between social and cognitive presences in communities of inquiry. Internet High Educ 42:53–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.iheduc.2019.05.001
Salmivalli C (1999) Participant role approach to school bullying: implications for interventions. J Adolesc 22:453–459. https://doi.org/10.1006/jado.1999.0239
Sarmiento A, Herrera-López M, Zych I (2019) Is cyberbullying a group process? Online and offline bystanders of cyberbullying act as defenders, reinforcers and outsiders. Comput Hum Behav 99:328–334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.05.037
Schultze-Krumbholz A, Hess M, Pfetsch J, Scheithauer H (2018) Who is involved in cyberbullying? Latent class analysis of cyberbullying roles and their associations with aggression, self-esteem, and empathy. Cyberpsychol J Psychosoc Res Cyberspace 12:2. https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2018-4-2
Sener B, Akpinar E, Ataman MB (2023) Unveiling the dynamics of emotions in society through an analysis of online social network conversations. Sci Rep. 13:14997. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-41573-9
Shaffer DW, Collier W, Ruis AR (2016) A tutorial on epistemic network analysis: analyzing the structure of connections in cognitive, social, and interaction data. J Learn Analyt 3:9–45. https://doi.org/10.18608/jla.2016.33.3
Small DA, Lerner JS (2008) Emotional policy: personal sadness and anger shape judgments about a welfare case. Polit Psychol 29:149–168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00621.x
Smith E, Mackie D (2015) Dynamics of group-based emotions: insights from intergroup emotions theory. Emotion Rev 7. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915590614
Smith ER (1993) Social identity and social emotions: toward new conceptualizations of prejudice. In: Mackie DM, Hamilton DL (eds) Affect, cognition and stereotyping. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 297–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-088579-7.50017-X
Snakenborg J, Van Acker R, Gable RA (2011) Cyberbullying: prevention and intervention to protect our children and youth. Prevent Sch Fail Alternat Educ Child Youth 55:88–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/1045988X.2011.539454
Song Y, Dai X-Y, Wang J (2016) Not all emotions are created equal: expressive behavior of the networked public on China’s social media site. Comput Hum Behav 60:525–533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.086
Stevens H, Acic I, Taylor L (2021) Uncivil reactions to sexual assault online: linguistic features of news reports predict discourse incivility. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Network 24. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2021.0075
Stieglitz S, Dang-Xuan L (2013) Emotions and information diffusion in social media—sentiment of microblogs and sharing behavior. J Manag Inf Syst 29:217–248. https://doi.org/10.2753/MIS0742-1222290408
Storbeck J, Clore GL (2008) Affective arousal as information: how affective arousal influences judgments, learning, and memory. Soc Personal Psychol compass 2:1824–1843. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00138.x
Storey JD, Tibshirani R (2003) Statistical significance for genomewide studies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:9440–9445. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1530509100
Sun Y, Wang S, Feng S, Ding S, Pang C, Shang J, Liu J, Chen X, Zhao Y, Lu Y, Liu W, Wu Z, Gong W, Liang J, Shang Z, Sun P, Liu W, Ouyang X, Yu D, Tian H, Wu H, Wang H (2021) ERNIE 3.0: large-scale knowledge enhanced pre-training for language understanding and generation. https://arxiv.org/abs/2107.02137v1. Accessed 22 Sept 2023
Terpilowski MA (2019) scikit-posthocs: Pairwise multiple comparison tests in Python. J Open Source Softw 4:1169. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01169
Tiedens LZ, Linton S (2001) Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: the effects of specific emotions on information processing. J Personal Soc Psychol 81:973–988. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.973
Tugade MM, Shiota MN, Kirby LD (2014) Handbook of positive emotions. Guilford Publications, New York, 544 pp
Van GA, Côté S (2022) The social effects of emotions. Annu Rev Psychol 73:629–658. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-020821-010855
van Doorn EA, van Kleef GA, van der Pligt J (2015) Deriving meaning from others’ emotions: attribution, appraisal, and the use of emotions as social information. Front Psychol 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01077
Vranjes I, Baillien E, Vandebosch H, Erreygers S, De Witte H (2017) The dark side of working online: towards a definition and an Emotion Reaction model of workplace cyberbullying. Comput Hum Behav 69:324–334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.055
Wakefield R, Wakefield K (2024) How intergroup counter-empathy drives media consumption and engagement. Internet Res 34:1602–1628. https://doi.org/10.1108/INTR-07-2022-0552
Wang S (2021) Standing up or standing by: Bystander intervention in cyberbullying on social media. N Media Soc 23:1379–1397. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444820902541
Wang M, Liu M, Feng S, Wang D, Zhang Y (2014) A novel calibrated label ranking based method for multiple emotions detection in Chinese microblogs. In: Zong C, Nie J-Y, Zhao D, Feng Y (eds) Natural Language Processing and Chinese Computing. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp. 238–250. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-45924-9_22
Wright MF (2016) Cybervictims’ emotional responses, attributions, and coping strategies for cyber victimization: a qualitative approach. Safer Commun 15:160–169. https://doi.org/10.1108/SC-03-2016-0006
Xu Y (2021) The invisible aggressive fist: features of cyberbullying language in China. Int J Semiotics Law Rev Int de Sémiotique Jurid 34:1041–1064. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11196-020-09746-1
Xu J, Jun K, Zhu X, Bellmore A (2012a) Learning from bullying traces in social media. In: Proceedings of the 2012 Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies. NAACL HLT’12. Association for Computational Linguistics, USA, pp. 656–666
Xu J-M, Zhu X, Bellmore A (2012b) Fast learning for sentiment analysis on bullying. In: Proceedings of the First International Workshop on Issues of Sentiment Discovery and Opinion Mining. WISDOM ’12. Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, pp. 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1145/2346676.2346686
Zhang H, Qu C (2020) Emotional, especially negative microblogs are more popular on the web: evidence from an fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav 14:1328–1338. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-018-9998-6
Zhang XA, Borden J, Kim S (2018) Understanding publics’ post-crisis social media engagement behaviors: an examination of antecedents and mediators. Telemat Inform 35:2133–2146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2018.07.014
Zhang Z, Han X, Liu Z, Jiang X, Sun M, Liu Q (2019) ERNIE: Enhanced language representation with informative entities. https://arxiv.org/abs/1905.07129v3. Assessed 22 Sept 2023
Zhong J, Zheng Y, Huang X, Mo D, Gong J, Li M, Huang J (2021) Study of the influencing factors of cyberbullying among Chinese college students incorporated with digital citizenship: from the perspective of individual students. Front Psychol 12:621418. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.621418
Zhong J, Qiu J, Sun M, Jin X, Zhang J, Guo Y, Qiu X, Xu Y, Huang J, Zheng Y (2022) To be ethical and responsible digital citizens or not: a linguistic analysis of cyberbullying on social media. Front Psychol 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.861823
Zhu H, Hsu C, Zhou Z (2023) Bystander pro-celebrity cyberbullying: an integrated perspective of susceptibility to retaliation and social capital gains. Inf Manag 60:103797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.im.2023.103797
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China (Grant Number: 21FJKB018), the Postdoctoral Research Foundation of China (Grant Number: 2021M701273), and Humanities and Social Sciences Youth Foundation of the Chinese Ministry of Education (Grant Number: 22YJC880021).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
JZ: data analysis, data curation, methodology, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; YM: data analysis, data curation, methodology, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; JZ: data analysis, data curation, writing—original draft; PL: data analysis, data curation, writing—original draft; XL: data analysis, literature search, writing—review and editing; LL: literature search, writing—review and editing; RD: data analysis, writing—review and editing; JH: conceptualization, funding acquisition, methodology, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; YZ: conceptualization, data analysis, funding acquisition, methodology, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethical approval
Ethical approval is not required prior to conducting the research reported in this paper, as the present study does not have experiments on human subjects and animals, and does not contain any sensitive and private information.
Informed consent
This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Zhong, J., Mo, Y., Zhang, J. et al. Beyond anger: uncovering complex emotional patterns between cyberbullying roles through affective computing and epistemic network analysis. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 12, 1281 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-025-05689-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-025-05689-9