Introduction

The world’s oceans are changing rapidly due to anthropogenic disturbances and human-induced climate change1,2. Already, these changes have included major effects on the emergence and spread of infectious diseases. For example, warming ocean temperatures have increased the growth rates and habitats of pathogens and broadened their repertoire of potential hosts3,4, and high levels of shipping traffic and shore runoff have facilitated the emergence of pathogens in the ocean5,6. Facing these threats on the front lines are delphinid species (i.e., dolphins), ocean predators, and sentinel species that are essential for healthy ocean ecosystems7,8, but among whom disease and mass mortality have surged in recent decades1,9. Our work aims to understand the underlying drivers of disease spread using behavioral data from dolphin populations so that we might effectively and efficiently estimate infection outcomes for highly social wildlife species in response to growing disease threats.

Numerous factors drive heterogeneity in infection outcomes across individuals such as host immunity10 or environmental and anthropogenic conditions11. Indeed, during a deadly viral outbreak in Mediterranean dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba) in 2007, a greater proportion of juveniles died compared to adults12, suggesting that infection vulnerability differs across age classes. However, the role of social behavior across demographics (e.g., age and sex) in structuring infection risk tends to be poorly understood. Highly social species such as bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops spp.) live in fission-fusion societies where members maintain long-term preferential bonds but exhibit associations that change in time and space13. Stable affiliations include mother-calf bonds14 and long-term alliances formed among males15, while various social phenotypes can be found among juveniles and females14,16,17. This stability of strong long-term bonds coupled with social heterogeneity leads us to hypothesize that social structure could drive disease variability and severity in bottlenose dolphins and other gregarious species with similar social structures.

Contact networks are often used to capture heterogeneity in transmission behaviors and to provide a tractable method to represent variation from social and demographic structure18,19. With the contact structure of the population represented in a network model, an epidemiological modeling approach can be an ethical and economical way to test hypotheses regarding potential factors that drive disease spread in wildlife and can play a key role in predicting the risk of epidemics in wild marine populations20. However, in order to study disease transmission and predict epidemic consequences using contact networks, it is important to identify the specific contact that allows for infection propagation between an infected and susceptible individual21. For example, the most deadly diseases in cetacean species tend to be respiratory transmitted viruses22 where transmission likely occurs during synchronized breathing contact, defined as two or more individuals surfacing and breathing simultaneously (e.g., within two seconds) and in close proximity (e.g., within two meters). The expired “blow” that is forcefully released from their blowholes is a combination of mucus, water and air, and is thought to be sufficient for droplet transmission of respiratory viruses22. Synchronized breathing occurs in a variety of contexts in delphinid species, is important for both socialization and parental care23,24,25,26, and is expected to be heterogeneous across both demographic (e.g., age, sex)27, and behavioral states (e.g., foraging, consorting)15,28,29.

Catastrophic die-offs caused by the respiratory pathogen dolphin morbillivirus (DMV) are a global phenomenon22. The most recent and deadliest known mass mortality due to DMV occurred in July of 2013 in bottlenose dolphins along the US Atlantic coast, in which more than 1,650 dolphins were recovered dead over a two year period. The bottlenose dolphins in this area of the northwest Atlantic comprise animals from multiple populations30, some of which are now considered “depleted” (or under optimal population levels) by the National Marine Fisheries Service because of disease mortality30,31, indicating that mass die-offs among dolphin species are detrimental to their populations and ecosystems. Despite this, few studies exist that examine the behavioral drivers of cetacean respiratory diseases such as DMV; most marine mammal disease studies focus on the spread of the similar phocine distemper virus (PDV) in seals, e.g.,32,33,34,35,36,37. Only one study examined the transmission of DMV and focused primarily on large-scale spatial dynamics of the 2013 Atlantic epizootic to estimate important features of DMV, including the infectious period (5–10 days) and the basic reproduction number, R0 (0.9–2.6), but did not explicitly consider contact dynamics between individuals38.

Motivated by the devastating consequences of the 2013 western North Atlantic DMV epizootic and the urgent need for data-driven work in gregarious wildlife populations, we leverage a study of wild bottlenose dolphins (recently classified as Tursiops erebennus39) to investigate the role of demographic variability in relevant contact on pathogen transmission in gregarious species. These dolphins utilize the region where the plurality of 2013 DMV deaths occurred (Fig. S1), and are currently below optimal sustainable population levels from disease outbreaks31. We hypothesize that infection risk will vary based on individual differences in synchronized breathing contact among age and sex classes, which could impact population-level vulnerability. With this work, we aim to characterize the impact of social structure and dynamics on disease risk using empirical data and inform future data collection on behavior and disease. A better understanding of disease transmission in this complex marine species may also allow us to develop strategies that minimize the detrimental effects of perturbations to this vulnerable population and to the overall ecosystem.

Results

Our central hypothesis is that social structure drives disease dynamics in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops spp.). To assess this empirically, we collected data from a wild dolphin research site in the Potomac-Chesapeake Bay region, USA (PC). Since it is not feasible to observe complete contact networks, we collected data and estimated the average number of synchrony contacts over a DMV infectious period, μ, for the following demographic groups, d: adult males, adult females and calves, and juveniles (synchrony degree, \({k}_{d}(\mu )\)). We then estimated the degree of mixing among these groups. We developed a generative network model to construct realistic contact networks informed by these empirical data. To predict disease consequences, we used an epidemiological model on these generated contact networks and examined the epidemic consequences across age and sex classes. We test the generalizability of our behavioral findings by comparing to empirical data from a second long-term study of wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) from Shark Bay, Australia (SB). Finally, to support our disease models, we compared our results to National Marine Fisheries Service mortality data from the 2013 DMV epidemic in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops erebennus.) along the western North Atlantic. The specifics of our methods are summarized in Fig. 1, with a more detailed summary in Fig. S2.

Fig. 1: Summary of methods.
figure 1

We first collected data on synchronized breathing behavior in bottlenose dolphins to estimate how respiratory droplets might transmit disease in cetaceans (Step 1). We then use the data to estimate synchronized breathing behavior over the course of a DMV infectious period (Step 2) and use these results to model a contact network that is representative of synchronized breathing and its variation across individuals (Steps 3–4). Finally we simulate the spread of disease across this contact network to characterize epidemic outcomes (Step 5). Photo credit: Potomac Chesapeake Dolphin Project, NMFS Permit No. 19403. Created in BioRender. Collier, M. (2025) https://BioRender.com/ytc9ehb.

Bottlenose dolphin synchronized breathing is structured demographically

We found that synchrony degree for PC dolphins, \({k}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\), and the variation in degree varies significantly across demographic groups for a DMV infectious period (F = 1152; d.f. = 2; p < 0.0001) (Fig. 2AB). Juveniles had the highest average degree (\({k}_{{juv}}\left(\mu \right)\) = 34.7), followed by adult males (\({k}_{{male}}\left(\mu \right)\) = 3058), while adult females and calves had the lowest (\({k}_{{fem}}\left(\mu \right)\) = 22.3). Our estimate of demographic-specific degree structure over an infectious period is also supported by our empirical measurement of demographic-specific degree (Fig. 2A) where juveniles have a significantly higher observed degree than adult females (p = 0.008), though the higher degree of males compared to adult females is not statistically significant (p = 0.295).

Fig. 2: The degree distributions and mixing matrices generated from PC data.
figure 2

A shows the average observed sync degree over the course of an average PC focal follow (n = 99) using a GLMM to control for different variables where points show the raw data, letters indicate significant differences, error bars represent standard error from the mean. We demonstrate how this observed degree scales up to an average DMV infectious period by generating a demographic specific degree distribution (B) for n = 1000 infectious period estimates using our methodology from Fig. 1. We then estimate a (C) demographic mixing matrix, where each cell represented the proportion of all observed synchrony dyads that existed between a focal demographic (row) and each demographic group (columns) (termed proportion of population mixing) for the PC over a DMV infectious period.

PC dolphins are also significantly more assortative in their synchronized breathing interactions than what would be expected at random (Newman’s assortativity = 0.47; z score = −5.09; p = <0.0001), suggesting that individuals breathe synchronously most often with individuals of their own demographic (Fig. 2C). Indeed, we saw that our predicted mixing values and confidence intervals were higher along the diagonal of our mixing matrices (where assortative mixing is represented) compared to the expected values for all three demographic groups (Fig. S3).

We compared these results to contact structure in the SB population and found consistent patterns (F = 1788; df = 2; p < 0.0001): adult females and mother calf pairs had the lowest synchrony degree (\({k}_{{fem}}\left(\mu \right)\,\)= 8.0) followed by juveniles (\({k}_{{juv}}\left(\mu \right)\) = 12.3) and adult males (\({k}_{{male}}\left(\mu \right)\) = 14.6) (Figs. S4 and S5); and synchrony degree was more assortative than expected at random (Newman’s assortativity = 0.39; z score = −8.65; p = <0.0001) (Fig. S6).

PC dolphins have higher infection risks than SB dolphins

We considered the spread of disease on networks representative of demographic degree and assortativity for bottlenose dolphins. The average reproductive number \({R}_{0}\) experienced by a host population during an outbreak is a function of the population’s contact structure and the transmissibility of the pathogen. We found that that in the PC dolphins \({R}_{0}=\,1.9\), which is the average reproductive number observed for past morbillivirus outbreaks in marine mammal populations, was achieved by a weakly transmissible pathogen with transmissibility \(T=0.055\). To achieve the same \({R}_{0}\) in the SB population required a moderately transmissible pathogen of \(T=0.115\) (Fig. S7). With these transmissibility values, we simulated epidemics representative of DMV spread in dolphin populations. We found that this resulted in about 63% of the total population infected in PC and 57% in SB (t = 25.525, df = 44.891, p-value < 0.0001), with a 63% likelihood of an epidemic occurring after a random introduction of disease into the population in PC and 57% liklihood in SB (t = 4.3235, df = 45.122, p-value < 0.0001) (Fig. S8). Additionally, our sensitivity analysis showed our results are robust to potential errors in demographic group assignment (Fig. S9).

Infection risk is age- and sex-structured

We simulated the spread of disease on networks representative of demographic degree and assortativity. We calculated the proportion of each demographic group that was infected across all networks and simulations for which an epidemic occurred. We found that our models predicted juveniles to be disproportionately infected in DMV epidemics compared to adults (t = 66.143, df = 36.63; p < 0.0001) (Fig. 3A). Within adults, males were predicted to be significantly more at risk for infection than females (t = 72.812, df = 44.28; p-value < 0.0001) (Fig. 3D). To compare to epidemic outcomes in the SB population, we performed the same epidemic simulation experiments, and found highly consistent patterns of transmission (juveniles more at risk for infection than adults (t = 11.309, df = 38.075; p < 0.0001); adult males more at risk for infection than adult females (t = 171.28, df = 45.415; p < 0.0001)) (Fig. 3B, E).

Fig. 3: Epidemiological model results compared to the stranding data.
figure 3

The top row shows the model results for the proportion of each age class infected in the PC (A) and SB (B) based on 100 disease simulations on 25 network models for each population. (C) shows the estimated proportion of each age class that was recovered during the 2013 DMV epizootic on the Atlantic Coast, derived from the stranding data (n = 504 strandings). The bottom row shows the proportion of each adult sex class that was infected in our models in the PC (D) and SB (E) based on 100 disease simulations on 25 network models for each population. F shows the proportion of adults of each sex class recovered on the Atlantic Coast, derived from the mortality data (n = 242 strandings). Differences were significant across all comparisons (asterisks) based on a t-test, and uncertainty represents standard deviation.

We show support for our results with empirical mortality data from strandings that occurred during the Atlantic DMV outbreak of 2013–2015. We found that the demographic biases in infection predicted by our model results were reflected in the observed stranding data where juveniles stranded at significantly higher rates than adults when controlling for the estimated population size for each age class (t = 554.43; df = 540.51; p < 0.0001) (Figs. 3C and S14), and adult males stranded at higher rates than adult females (t = 343.04; df = 995.92; p < 0.0001) (Figs. 3F and S10).

Thus, our results showed age- and sex-specific biases in infection risk. To better understand whether this risk bias is structured by demographic-specific degree distributions (the average and variation in the number of contacts per demographic group) or demographic assortativity (the tendency of demographic groups to mix more within groups), we ran epidemic experiments on null models. We found that in a population with only demographic-specific degree structure, epidemic sizes (the proportion of the total population infected) were significantly lower (PC: F = 10830, d.f. = 2, p < 0.0001; SB: F = 4232, d.f. = 2, p < 0.0001) (Fig. S11). Importantly, we also found that with only demographic assortativity present, the demographic biases in infection outcomes no longer existed (PC: F = 0.735, d.f. = 2, p = 0.483; SB: F = 1.527, d.f = 2, p = 0.224) (Fig. S12). This suggests that degree is the most important driver of the demographic-specific infection risks, and assortative mixing drives the overall population infection burden.

Population infection risks are amplified when disease is introduced by adult males or juveniles

We examined how population infection burden differs depending on which demographic group begins the infection in our 25 PC network models. We found that when infection is introduced to the population by a juvenile, disease burden was higher for both age classes compared to when infection is introduced by an adult (F = 80.69, d.f = 3, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 4A). Similarly, when the index case is an adult male, we saw that both adult males and adult females had higher infection burdens than when the index case is an adult female (F = 132.5, d.f. = 3, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 4B). This suggests that demographically structured social behavior not only drives infection risk within demographic groups, but also epidemic risk at the population level.

Fig. 4: The effect of pathogen introduction on epidemic outcomes.
figure 4

The proportion of each age class (A) and adult sex class (B) that is infected in our epidemiological model for the PC, based on in which demographic group the infection began or was introduced (colors) for n = 100 disease simulations on 25 PC network models. Uncertainty represents standard deviation.

Discussion

Our analysis shows that there is heterogeneity in synchronized breathing contact dynamics across age and sex in bottlenose dolphins. While infection risk can be structured demographically due to a variety of other factors such as environmental conditions or host immunity40,41, our goal in this work is to present a parsimonious explanation for observed heterogeneity in infection risk based on social behavior only.

Our comparative analysis of two wild bottlenose dolphin studies, one in the heavily disturbed waters of an Atlantic Ocean bay42 and the other in the protected waters of an Indian Ocean bay, highlights that relative infection risk is structured similarly despite notable ecological differences between the two research sites. However, we also demonstrate a substantially higher absolute risk of epidemics occurring in PC dolphins relative to the SB population, an idea supported by the fact that certain populations of the Atlantic bottlenose dolphins have decreased by 40–50% twice in 25 years from morbillivirus31,43,44, while SB has no recorded mass mortality events from infectious diseases. Our work explains differences in vulnerability based on the fundamental social behavior of synchronized breathing and quantifies the risk in terms of a higher number of and more homophilic transmission contacts in the PC. The substantial difference in social connectivity can be explained by a number of differences. First, the dolphins in the PC are thought to be a part of at least three different populations of Atlantic bottlenose dolphins30, meaning there are likely more individuals (~10,000) compared to SB (~2700)45. Indeed, the average group size for SB based on our data was 3.7 compared to 18.7 in the PC. Second, the SB population is a year-round, resident population, while the animals in the PC are considered seasonal and migratory46,47. These factors drive notable changes in the observed activity budgets of individuals, with the SB population spending ~35% of their time foraging (a largely solitary activity13,48) and ~22% time traveling49, while the PC preliminary estimates suggest individuals spend a large majority of their time traveling (a largely social activity) when present at the PC study area (~7 months). While it is possible that activity budgets for PC dolphins might change in other parts of their habitat that we have not observed, we suggest that for at least half the year, their contact rates might be elevated due to long times spent traveling. Our comparative approach thus highlights consistencies in demographic contact patterns and disease risk structure despite ecological differences, and generates hypotheses about the role of metapopulation structure and seasonal changes in distribution, like migration, on population vulnerability to threats for many wildlife species.

We also find that there is significant assortativity for synchronized breathing among demographic groups. This is consistent with past findings27 and suggests a preference for social associations within similar age and sex classes. This effect was stronger in the PC than in SB, which may also contribute to increased infection risk in the former. Increased assortativity by trait (e.g., age or sex) can increase infection burden in networks if the number of contacts is also structured by that trait50. Indeed, we found that demographic assortative mixing had a significant impact on epidemic risk in the PC, but less so in SB. It is possible these differences are due to an observation bias in that PC focal follows were conducted over a shorter period of time and a different life-history stage compared to SB focal follows. In adults, male-male synchronized breathing is likely higher during breeding seasons when males are consorting females15. Synchronized breathing has also been shown to increase with tourist boat traffic29,51, as a potential behavioral response to a threat. If both these trends are also the case in the PC, we might see higher rates of assortative mixing due to only observing synchrony interactions during a period of heightened interactions. Future work on the disruptions to contact structure due to seasonal dynamics or anthropogenic factors could reveal important findings for understanding population-level infection risk over an annual period.

Our results also highlight that adult females and mother-calf pairs have a lower infection risk than adult males due to their smaller synchrony degree. Synchronized breathing is important for social bonding, particularly in bottlenose dolphin populations where male alliances are common, such as in Shark Bay15. In contrast, female associations are considered weaker on average and vary distinctively across individuals between being almost solitary to having moderate strength relationships with other females14,16,52,53. We still know very little about the sex specific behaviors of the dolphins that visit the PC. However, our results show that the differences in synchrony degree among males and females are similar compared to Shark Bay, suggesting that these sex specific behaviors may be maintained across populations and different species of the genus Tursiops. Our past work has also demonstrated that social behavior puts male bottlenose dolphins at high infection risk for pathogens of a variety of transmission modes, including sexual and skin to skin transmission54,55. Male-biased infection rates due to behavioral differences between males and females are a phenomenon seen in many non-human animal species56,57,58, and our work thus supports these findings.

We observed a difference in infection risk between adults and juveniles, suggesting that synchrony degree may be highest in the juvenile period. The relationships formed in the juvenile period are likely critical for survival, forming future social bonds, and reproductive success

for both males and females59,60,61. Previous work on juvenile associations in Shark Bay has shown that juveniles socialize significantly more than adult females in the same population, and compared to adults from other bottlenose dolphin populations17,62. The higher synchrony degree seen in juveniles compared to adults in the PC reflects these findings; synchronized breathing may be most important as bonds begin to form and less important in the maintenance of stable bonds. The adult/juvenile effect in SB is not as large, which is likely driven by the high adult male synchrony degree observed in this population, which has one of the most complex male alliances in the non-human animal world. Male alliance structure and social bonds have not been investigated in the PC dolphins, but our results suggest that while synchronized breathing is still important for social bonding between adult males, it might be even more important in the juvenile period for these individuals.

We found that disease consequences were worse overall when infections originated in juveniles compared to adults, and in adult males compared to adult females. This suggests that while certain demographic groups are at greater risk of infection because of their high synchrony degree, the entire population is also more vulnerable to infection when infections are initiated in these groups. These results demonstrate the importance of considering how different demographic groups might be more susceptible to contracting novel pathogens, as there could be varying impacts at the population level. The emergence of DMV in the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin populations is thought to be from interactions with pilot whales, which are morbillivirus reservoirs22. Other highly pathogenic viruses, including avian influenza, have also been recently discovered in marine mammals, including bottlenose dolphins, likely from interactions with sea birds63,64. Interactions with humans might also affect the emergence and spread of infectious diseases in dolphins. A recent study found evidence for human pathogens present in bottlenose dolphins that have high human contact due to a provisioning program in Shark Bay, Australia65, suggesting opportunity for a direct pathogen transmission from humans to animals. Our work thus highlights that understanding variation in interspecies (both human and nonhuman) interactions across demographic groups could provide a better understanding of overall population infection risk for current and emerging infectious diseases in wildlife.

Disease models have been used in the protection of vulnerable, threatened populations of ecosystem sentinels worldwide, and our work contributes to such research. Our models use empirical data to predict demographic groups that are most at risk during epidemic outbreaks. It is also important to note that the large numbers of juveniles found dead during the 2013 outbreak (and in some other morbillivirus outbreaks globally12,22) were also likely exacerbated by their lack of immunity to DMV compared adults who may have been infected in a past outbreak44. As epidemics are typically extinguished when the virus has run out of susceptible individuals to infect, the eventual replenishment of these susceptible individuals (i.e., births) will cause epidemic recurrences with high mortalities66. Fully naïve population without established immunity would thus see a more similar burden across age classes (e.g., Guiana dolphins, Sotalia guianensis, in Brazil67). Future models could thus consider these rates of immunity to better understand demographic risk and to predict the periodicity of DMV outbreaks in populations. These predictions can be used to understand downstream impacts on population dynamics and recovery, as well as for developing potential targeted control strategies for reducing disease burden33. The control of disease in dolphins remains a challenge that requires effective and long-term solutions. Candidate vaccines against morbillivirus exist for DMV68, but the vaccination of free-living, obligate marine wildlife, such as dolphins, is fraught with logistical and ethical challenges. Therefore, we recommend focusing on reducing factors that are known to increase disease burden in marine species11. Overall, our work highlights how the variation in social behavior among different demographic groups of dolphins is crucial in influencing disease transmission. By integrating these behavioral differences into disease models, we can improve our predictions and develop more effective mitigation strategies. This holistic approach is essential not only for dolphins but for wildlife globally, as it underscores the importance of considering variable social structures in efforts to protect and preserve vulnerable species.

Materials and methods

Data collection

To evaluate contact dynamics in bottlenose dolphins, we used behavioral data collected from boat-based observational surveys and focal animal sampling49. When dolphins were sighted, we first conducted a survey consisting of a 5-min scan sample to determine group composition (individual identities determined via photo identification (ID) of unique dorsal fins69), life history information, and predominant group behavioral activity (see Mann, 199925 for behavioral activity definitions). To evaluate synchronized breathing, we then conducted focal follows on an individual dolphin or a mother and her dependent calf (considered a single focal animal due to largely overlapping behavior) observed in the initial survey for a predetermined amount of time between 15 min and 3 h. Focal follows are 1-min interval point samples where we record the behavioral activity of the focal animal. The number of behavioral events (e.g., synchronized breathing) was collected continuously throughout the follow for the focal individual, as well as the ID of their contacts. We used survey and focal follow-up data from two studies on wild bottlenose dolphins: The Potomac-Chesapeake Dolphin Project (Tursiops erebennus) and The Shark Bay Dolphin Research Project (Tursiops aduncus).

Our primary populations of interest are those studied by the Potomac-Chesapeake Dolphin Project (PC), a long-term study initiated in 2015 containing detailed data on individual life histories and behavior of bottlenose dolphins that inhabit the waters where the plurality of the mortalities occurred during the 2013 DMV epidemic (Fig. S1)38. These dolphins are thought to be a part of at least three different populations of Atlantic bottlenose dolphins. For the purpose of this analysis, we assume that all the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin populations in the study area have similar contact trends across demography. We collected data over an 8-year time period between April and October 2015–2022 in Maryland and Virginia waters from the lower tidal Potomac River (southeast of the Governor Harry W. Nice Memorial Bridge, N 38.361595, W 76.997144) to the middle Chesapeake Bay (specifically Ingram Bay, N 37.790233, W 76.997144). We collected 99 focal follows on 97 PC dolphins between June and September 2015–2022 with an average follow time of 25 min.

For testing generalizability across bottlenose dolphin populations, we also used data from the Shark Bay Dolphin Research Project (SB), a population that has not yet been documented to have experienced a DMV epidemic. This long-term study was initiated in 1982 and contains more than 35 years of detailed data on individual life histories, including sex and age70. We used 610 focal follows on 132 SB dolphins conducted over an 8-year time period between April and December 2010–2017, with an average follow time of 119 min.

For the PC, we used a set of rules for assigning age and sex classes to each focal dolphin and their contacts using sighting records, behavioral data, and photographic data, with high or low confidence (more information in Supplementary Note 1 and Table S1). From these rules, we determined the following demographic groups: juvenile (n = 15), adult female and mother-calf pairs (n = 41), and adult male (n = 43). Given our sex assignment rules and juvenile behavior, juveniles are considered one demographic group instead of two (female juveniles and male juveniles). We also considered adult females with dependent calves (mother-calf pairs) as one unit for disease transmission, given the close contact between mother and calf. Further, due to our small sample size for PC adult females without a dependent calf, we combined mother-calf pairs and adult females into one demographic group to allow for a better balance of sample sizes across groups. For SB, we use the same demographic group definitions as PC to allow for reliable comparison (juvenile n = 169; adult female and mother calf pair = 416; adult male = 25). Given high-quality genetic data and high-confidence birth observations for SB70, only focals with known demographic assignments were used, which resulted in 90% of contacts having known demographic assignments. Given that the SB population is demographically better documented than PC, we also used SB data to support our demographic-specific PC results. In Supplementary Note 2, we used SB data to demonstrate that our demographic group definitions, which are constrained by the PC data limitations (i.e., juveniles are not sex differentiated; adult females with and without calves are grouped), are indeed appropriate (Fig. S13). In Supplementary Note 3, we showed that our results are robust to the uncertainty in PC demographic assignments.

Statement on ethics

This study was strictly observational, and we have complied with all relevant ethical regulations for animal use. All PC and SB data were collected under GU Permit #s IACUC-13-069, 07-041, 10-023; 2016-1235. PC data were collected under NMFS permit nos. 19403 and 23782. SB data were collected under permits from the West Australian Department of Biodiversity, Conservation and Attractions: SF-009876, SF-010347, SF-008076, SF009311, SF007457, FO25000102-6.

Estimating transmission contact relevant to respiratory infection

Since DMV is a respiratory-transmitted pathogen, we focused on synchronized breathing interactions as our measure of contact. Past work in the field of network epidemiology demonstrates that average degree (the number of unique individuals an average focal individual has contact with), and degree heterogeneity (the variation among the population in the number of contacts per individual) are highly predictive of epidemic outcomes21,71 while second-order network structure (including clustering and modularity) are far less predictive21,72. Additionally, we know that increased assortativity by trait (e.g., age or sex), or a tendency to have interactions with individuals of the same trait, can increase infection burden if the number of contacts is also structured by that trait50. Thus, to estimate disease outcomes among the PC bottlenose dolphins, we estimated synchrony degree, \({k}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\) the number of unique individuals a focal animal of demographic group, \(d\), has synchronous breathing interactions with over an average DMV infectious period \(\mu\). We also estimated how the contacts of a focal individual are distributed among the demographic groups \(M\).

Estimating demographic-specific synchrony degree

For contact to be a measure of the total number of individuals that an infected animal can transmit to while they are infected, it must be considered on the timescale of the infectious period of the pathogen. Since our focal follow-up data only measures synchronized breathing for short periods of time (average 25 min for PC and 119 min for SB), it is not representative of total contact over a relevant infectious period for DMV (5–10 days38). To measure relevant synchrony degree, we must therefore extrapolate our measurements for longer time periods. It is challenging to extrapolate degrees beyond periods of observation, as there is fidelity in social bonding, and interactions with the same individual are generally repeated. Based on past work, a power law relationship exists between degree and the number of interactions (while degree measures synchronous breathing events with unique individuals, interaction counts measure all synchronous breathing events) that spans behaviors and can be found across taxa (Fig. S14)73. So, we fit the following power law relationship to observed data on the number of synchronous breathing interactions that occur in t days \(\left({n}_{d}\left(t\right)\right)\), and synchrony degree per individual over t days \(\left({k}_{d}\left(t\right)\right)\) for each demographic group\(,d\):

$${k}_{d}\left(t\right)=\alpha {n}_{d}{(t)}^{-\lambda }$$
(1)

We identified the following parameters (Figs. S15 and S16): \(\alpha =0.587\) and \(\lambda =-0.5209\).

Then, to empirically estimate the degree of an individual for a DMV infectious period \(\mu\), \({k}_{d}(\mu )\), we must first estimate the number of synchrony interactions that individuals would have over \(\mu\) days: \({n}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\). To estimate \({n}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\) we first decomposed the social processes required to be:

$${n}_{d}(\mu )={\mu G}_{d}\,{S}_{d}{r}_{d}$$
(2)

where \({G}_{d}\) was the probability that individuals of demographic group \(d\) are observed in a group, \({S}_{d}\) was the probability that individuals of demographic group \(d\) breathe synchronously while in a group, \({r}_{d}\) was the typical rate of synchronized breathing for demographic group \(d\), and \(\mu\) was the infectious period of DMV. Next, using empirical data from each research site (PC and SB), we used a bootstrapping approach to estimate a distribution for \({G}_{d}\,\)(based on survey data), and fit a series of generalized linear mixed models to estimate distributions for \({S}_{d}\) (based on focal data) (Figs S17 and S18), and \({r}_{d}\)(based on focal data) (Figs. S19 and S20). We then estimated \({n}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\) by sampling from these fitted distributions for an infectious period, \(\mu \epsilon \left[{\mathrm{5,10}}\right]\) days for n = 1000 individuals of each group \(d\).

Using these estimates for \({n}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\) and the power law fit from Eq. 1, we inferred \({k}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\) and tested for statistically significant differences using a one-way ANOVA and Tukey’s test. Finally, we fit each \({k}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\) mean and standard deviation to a negative binomial distribution as an estimate of the demographic-specific degree distribution \(p\left({k}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\right)\), as this represents both the average degree and degree heterogeneity for each group \(d\). Full details on 2.2.1 methods can be found in Supplementary Methods 1.

Validation

To demonstrate that our estimate of degree by demographic group over an infectious period \({k}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\) is supported by data, we compared our \({k}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\) to our observations of degree during an average focal follow period. Colman et al. (2021)73 show that the (power law) relationship between degree and the number of interactions is determined by the nature of social behaviors (affiliative versus antagonistic) and is relatively consistent. Given that our study focuses on a particular (affiliative) behavior, we assumed that the relationship between number of interactions and degree is stable for a given demographic group, and that our estimated degree for an infectious period should reflect similar patterns to the observed degree (PC n = 99 follows, SB n = 610 follows) over an average focal follow period (for further details, see Supplementary Methods 1.4). To control for the length of the follow and variation across years and individuals, we ran a generalized linear mixed model with observed degree as the response variable and a categorical predictor variable for demographic groups with the length of the follow-up as a fixed effect, and random effects for year and dolphin identity.

Estimating demographic mixing in synchronous breathing

To evaluate the degree of mixing among demographic groups, we measured demographic assortativity (the degree to which individuals interact with individuals of their own demographic group) by creating a mixing matrix based on focal follow data (PC: n = 99, SB: n = 610). Each element \({m}_{{gh}}\) in \(M\) represented the proportion of contacts for a focal individual of demographic group \(g\) that are of demographic group \(h\), across all follows. We standardized each \({m}_{{gh}}\) term to control for differences in the number of individuals within each demographic group and their estimated \({{k}}_{d}(\mu )\), and then used a bootstrapping approach to estimate confidence intervals for each term. Finally, we generated a matrix \(E\) to represent our expectation for demographic mixing assuming it occurred at random (for details, see Supplementary Methods 2). A comparison of \(M\) to \(E\) gives us an understanding of which demographic pair is mixing more than expected. We also calculated the assortativity coefficients for \(M\) according to Newman’s definition74. To test if dolphins are more assortative than expected at random, we transformed the assortativity coefficients into z scores and tested for statistical significance (0.05 significance level).

Network generation

Informed by our estimates of demographic-specific synchrony contact structure, we constructed contact network models that represent synchronous breathing contacts unique to each study site. A network was characterized by nodes representing individual dolphins (or mother-calf pairs), and edges representing the presence of synchronized breathing contact between a pair of individuals. Based on the demographic-specific synchrony degree distributions \(\left(p\left({k}_{d}\left(\mu \right)\right)\right)\), synchrony demographic mixing matrix \(\left(M\right)\), and demographic distribution of the population (specified in Supplementary Methods 3), we developed a network generating algorithm to create synthetic networks representing the contact structure of bottlenose dolphins. Networks of any size can be created with our algorithm, but we specified networks of 2000 nodes (individuals) in order to reflect reasonable estimates of the number of individuals sighted in the PC and SB45. The synthetic networks were random in all other properties besides those specified (degree distribution by demographic group and mixing patterns between and among groups). This feature is critical as it allows us to study the implications of the specified structure in a systematic way. We generated 25 networks to capture variability in contact dynamics.

Modeling disease transmission in bottlenose dolphins

To model the spread of DMV across our generated network models, we performed numerical simulations of a susceptible-infected-recovered percolation model74. For each network, we simulated 100 disease outbreaks where infection started in a random individual,  and classified large-scale epidemics as those where at least 10% of the network was infected. The average R0 for morbilliviruses across marine mammals is 1.934,38,75. We evaluated disease outcomes by calculating the (1) epidemic size (proportion of network infected in an epidemic), and (2) demographic risk (proportion of each demographic group infected in an epidemic) across all simulations, regardless of the demographic group of the originally infected node. We tested for statistically significant differences between age and sex class infection risks using one-tailed t-tests. We showed that the variability of epidemic outcomes is not different within one network, compared to across all 25 generated networks, to demonstrate that we generated enough networks to capture potential contact variability (Fig. S21).

Validation from empirical disease data

To demonstrate that our model results are supported by observed disease data, we obtained mortality data available for bottlenose dolphins that washed ashore or were found floating, defined as “stranded” from 2010 to 2015, from the US National Marine Fisheries Service. The DMV epidemic spanned 21 months (1-Jul-13 to 1-Mar-15). Because not all recorded cases of strandings during this time period (n = 1263 strandings) would be due to DMV, we defined excess mortality by removing typical “background” mortality for each demographic group (such as from natural mortality or human interactions)38,76. The resulting data therefore represented the number of recovered mortalities that occurred due to DMV along the Atlantic coast, which we assumed is proportional to the number of individuals infected during the epidemic (n = 504 strandings). To make these data comparable to our model results, we standardized the number of deaths for each demographic class to be an estimated proportion of recovered epidemic mortality for that age or sex class for Atlantic dolphins (See Supplementary Methods 4 and Fig. S22 for more details on this methodology). We tested for statistically significant differences between age and sex class strandings using a one-tailed t-test. These methods assume that the effort to collect dolphin stranding data did not change from before the outbreak to during the outbreak. While this assumption may not be true, we did not find any evidence to suggest that effort would affect the demographic trends of strandings, so we suggest this assumption is valid for our purposes.

Demographic-specific disease introduction

We were also interested in how epidemic outcomes change based on where in the network the pathogen is introduced. While DMV is not a reverse zoonotic pathogen, there are instances in which wild dolphins have direct contact with humans that could allow for emergence of other respiratory transmitted pathogens in dolphins, such as through human-associated foraging and in swim with dolphin programs32,77,78. There has also been observed variation in how frequently different demographics partake in human interactions. For example, studies have found that juvenile bottlenose dolphins are more likely to come into close contact with humans participating in wild swim with dolphin tourism79. Further, adult male cetaceans in certain populations are more likely to interact with humans by depredating recreational fishing lines80 and food provisioning81. Thus, we assessed the impact of disease introductions in particular demographic groups on disease risk. Because disease introductions are known to affect the likelihood of an epidemic occurring, or epidemic probability, rather than epidemic size, we considered a product of epidemic probability and demographic risk and tested for differences between scenarios using a one-way ANOVA and Tukey’s test.

Structural analysis

To examine which structural feature of the contact network is most predictive of demographic specific risk, we defined two null networks: one in which demographic-specific degree distributions were random and homogeneous across demographic groups but demographic mixing was still empirically informed; and another in which demographic mixing was random, but demographic-specific degree distributions were retained as empirically-informed. We compared disease consequences of our empirically informed networks against these two null models (see Supplementary Methods 5 for details) using a one-way ANOVA and Tukey’s test.