Abstract
Insula activity has often been linked to pain perception, making it a potential target for therapeutic neuromodulation strategies such as neurofeedback. However, it is not known whether insula activity is under cognitive control and, if so, whether this activity is consequently causally related to pain. Here, we conducted a double-blind randomized controlled crossover trial to test the modulation of insula activity and pain thresholds using neurofeedback training. Nineteen healthy subjects underwent neurofeedback training for upmodulation and downmodulation of right insula activity using our magnetoencephalography (MEG)-based brain–machine interface. We observed significant differences in insula activity between the upmodulation and downmodulation training sessions. Furthermore, resting-state insula activity significantly decreased following downmodulation training compared to following upmodulation training. Compared with upmodulation training, downmodulation training was also associated with increased pain thresholds, albeit with no significant interaction effect. These findings show that humans can cognitively modulate insula activity as a potential route to develop therapeutic MEG neurofeedback systems for clinical testing. However, the present findings do not provide direct evidence of a causal link between modulation of insula activity and changes in pain thresholds.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Pain perception is determined not only by external stimuli but also by internal states related to perceptual sensitivity. Various neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that pain perception is regulated by a broad cerebral network1,2, which includes the S1, S2, insular cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex and thalamus3.
Among these regions, the insular cortex serves as a key hub in pain perception, contributing to both nociceptive processing and its cognitive modulation4,5,6,7,8,9,10. Research in animal models has shown that manipulating neural activity in the insula significantly affects pain perception11,12. Multiple human studies have shown that the insular cortex is activated following exposure to noxious stimuli1,7,13,14,15,16,17,18. Additionally, insula activity has been reported to be associated with chronic pain states19, such as those experienced by patients with fibromyalgia20,21 and complex regional pain syndrome22. Notably, direct electrical stimulation of the human anterior insula can increase pain thresholds23, suggesting that engaging this region can causally influence pain perception. However, despite these associations, it remains unclear whether insular cortex activity can be deliberately controlled and, if so, whether such control has a direct causal effect on pain perception. In other words, a definitive link between insula modulation and changes in pain experience has yet to be established, thus highlighting the need for studies that directly test insula-focused neuromodulation in humans.
One promising strategy to modulate region-specific neural activity is neurofeedback24. In a typical neurofeedback system, neural activity is continuously recorded, processed, and displayed in real-time. Participants interact with the feedback, learning to regulate their brain activity toward a target state, thereby facilitating adaptive neuroplasticity25. Previous neurofeedback studies have provided preliminary support for targeting the insula in pain modulation. For example, EEG-based neurofeedback training has shown that changes in insular theta-band oscillatory power correlate with alterations in pain perception4. Similarly, real-time fMRI studies reported that modulating blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signals in the insula can lead to changes in pain processing26,27. These prior findings suggest a strong relationship between insula activity and pain modulation. Nevertheless, they do not conclusively demonstrate a direct causal link, as they primarily show correlations or group differences rather than providing proof that an individual’s deliberate control of insular activity drives pain changes. Key questions remain: (i) To what extent is insula activity under cognitive control such that it is susceptible to neurofeedback? (ii) Are changes in insula activity associated with changes in pain perception?
To address these questions, we proposed the following hypotheses: (i) the mean cortical excitability, or root mean square (RMS) of the cortical currents, of insula activity can be downmodulated through neurofeedback; and (ii) downmodulating the mean cortical excitability of insula activity through neurofeedback can increase the pain threshold. Additionally, as an exploratory analysis, we examined whether changes in mean cortical excitability were associated with changes in oscillatory power.
To test our hypothesis, we developed a magnetoencephalography (MEG)-based neurofeedback approach that enables participants to modulate their right anterior insula activity in real time. We then conducted a double-blind, randomized controlled crossover trial. Our system was specifically designed to extract the RMS of the insular cortical current—a measure of mean cortical excitability—from the MEG data. By using the RMS of estimated insula currents as the feedback signal, we targeted the overall amplitude of insular activity rather than focusing on a single frequency band. This approach differs from previous insula neurofeedback studies, which primarily targeted oscillatory power (e.g., theta rhythm) or BOLD activity.
Results
Subjects and task description
Twenty healthy volunteers were recruited and randomly allocated to two groups; 19 of them (11 males, 8 females; mean age 28.8 years; range 22–58 years; all right-handed) completed this trial. A CONSORT diagram of the subjects throughout the trial process is provided in (Fig. 1).
The subjects engaged in a series of recording and training sessions (Fig. 2a). We chose the right insula region (Fig. 2b) from the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) atlas28 as our region of interest (ROI). First, we recorded the MEG signals of the subjects during a 5-min resting period. Subsequently, we measured their pain threshold and proceeded with neurofeedback training. During the training sessions, the subjects were asked to mentally control the size of a black disk displayed on a screen (Fig. 2b). During the upmodulation training, insula activity was directly proportional to the size of the black disk, which indicated that successful training would result in increased insula activity. On the other hand, during downmodulation training, insula activity was inversely proportional to the size of the black disk, implying that successful training would lead to decreased insula activity. Following training, we measured the subjects’ pain thresholds again and recorded brain activity for another 5-min period while the subjects were in the resting state.
a Each experiment consisted of the following tasks: first, MEG resting-state data were recorded for 5 min. Then, the subject’s pain threshold was measured. Neurofeedback training was performed 2 times for 10 min each. Two types of modulation were used to control the feedback picture, one for each experiment. After the pain threshold was measured, the MEG resting-state data were recorded again for 5 min. b Neurofeedback training. The subjects were instructed to intentionally try to enlarge the size of the black disk on the screen.
Neurofeedback training effectively modulates real-time insula activity
The total training duration was 1200 s, corresponding to 6000 time points. To compare insula activity between the two types of training, the data were standardized across the entire training process using the magnitude and variance of insula activity during the initial 60 s (baseline) for each subject, with values deviating by more than three standard deviations treated as artifacts and replaced with the median. Time-series data were smoothed using a moving average filter with a window of 100 data points (corresponding to 20 s). This smoothing was performed to better illustrate the trends of neurofeedback-induced modulation of neural activity. Fig. 3a shows the time course of average standardized insula activity, while Fig. 3b illustrates a significant difference in insula activity between the two training conditions. Numerical source data for Fig. 3 are provided in Supplementary Data 1. Specifically, the standardized insula activity during downmodulation training was significantly lower than that during upmodulation training (t = 2.168, p = 0.044 < 0.05; paired t test, n = 19). These results suggested that neurofeedback training successfully modulated insula activity levels.
a Temporal dynamics of mean standardized insula activity during upregulation (blue) and downregulation (red) conditions. The solid lines represent the mean activity across subjects, and the shaded areas indicate the standard error of the mean (SEM). All curves in this figure have been smoothed using a 100-point (20-s) moving average to clearly illustrate the neurofeedback modulation trends. The vertical dashed line at 600 s marks a brief pause between two training sessions. b The boxplot compares the mean insula activity during the entire training process for each subject under the upmodulation and downmodulation conditions. Insula activity during upmodulation training was significantly greater than that during downmodulation training (blue for upmodulation training; red for downmodulation training). Two-tailed paired Student’s t test, *p < 0.05. In the boxplots, centerlines represent medians, the box limits represent the lower and upper quartiles, the whiskers represent 1.5 times the interquartile range, and the circles represent outliers.
Resting-state insula activity is effectively modulated following neurofeedback training
To further confirm that neurofeedback successfully modulated insula activity levels, we extracted RMS value of estimated right insula currents from the resting-state brain activity before and after training and compared the changes after each training session and differences between the two training types (Fig. 4). Numerical source data for Fig. 4 are provided in Supplementary Data 1. We found that resting-state insula activity significantly decreased after downmodulation training (t = 3.349, p = 0.004 < 0.025; paired Student’s t test, Bonferroni correction; n = 19). The decrease in resting-state insula activity was significantly greater in the downmodulation experiment than in the upmodulation experiment (t = 2.191, p = 0.042 < 0.05; paired Student’s t test, n = 19). This shows that neurofeedback modulation has a persistent effect on insula activity.
a Mean resting-state insula activity before (pre) and after (post) the upmodulation (blue) and downmodulation (red) training are shown. Paired t-tests were conducted to compare pre and post activity levels within each training condition. b Differences in insula activity (post - pre) for the upmodulation (blue) and downmodulation (red) conditions are shown. A paired t-test was conducted to compare these differences between the two conditions. All the data were analyzed by two-tailed paired Student’s t tests with Bonferroni correction. Asterisks (*) denote statistically significant differences, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. In the boxplots, dots represent individual data points, centrelines represent medians, box limits represent lower and upper quartiles, whiskers represent 1.5 times the interquartile range, and circles represent outliers.
Whole-brain activity and insula specificity
To ensure that our feedback was indeed specific to insular activity despite the inherent challenges of reconstructing signals from this deep cortical structure, we performed a whole-brain analysis of source activity during both upmodulation and downmodulation training. Specifically, we estimated cortical current distributions across the entire brain for a 300-s resting period, computed the RMS of these currents, and then applied a 10-mm Gaussian smoothing kernel to the cortical surface. We subsequently evaluated the RMS differences (posttraining minus pretraining) between the two feedback conditions by conducting paired t tests on each vertex (downmodulation vs. upmodulation). As shown in (Fig. 5), the RMS difference decreased in the right insular cortex after downmodulation compared with upmodulation. The region with the most significant t value was the right insular cortex, the target of the feedback, although the differences were not statistically significant after correction for multiple comparisons (false discovery rate (FDR), p > 0.05). The result suggests that our magnetoencephalography (MEG) neurofeedback altered the targeted region, the right insula, most effectively.
Effect of neurofeedback training on pain thresholds
We also conducted a statistical analysis of pain thresholds pre- and posttraining (Fig. 6). Numerical source data for Fig. 6 are provided in Supplementary Data 1. There was no significant change after upmodulation training (t = −1.816, p = 0.086 > 0.025; n = 19; paired Student’s t test, Bonferroni correction). The pain threshold significantly increased after the downmodulation training (t = −3.103, p = 0.006 < 0.025; paired Student’s t test, Bonferroni correction; n = 19). However, the changes in pain thresholds after the two types of training were not significantly different (t = −0.474, p = 0.641 > 0.05; n = 19; paired Student’s t test).
a Pain thresholds before (pre) and after (post) the upmodulation (blue) and downmodulation (red) training are shown. Paired t-tests were conducted to compare pre and Pain thresholds within each training condition. b Differences in Pain thresholds (post - pre) for the upmodulation (blue) and downmodulation (red) conditions are shown. A paired t-test was conducted to compare these differences between the two conditions. All the data were analyzed by two-tailed paired Student’s t tests with Bonferroni correction. Asterisks (*) denote statistically significant differences, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. In the boxplots, dots represent individual data points, centerlines represent medians, box limits represent lower and upper quartiles, whiskers represent 1.5 times the interquartile range, and circles represent outliers.
During the upmodulation training period, there were no significant changes in pain thresholds. The absence of significant changes does not necessarily suggest ineffectiveness. This could stem from the inherent propensity of the training, regardless of its type, to elevate pain thresholds (i.e., a nonspecific effect of time, manifested as habituation in pain thresholds), as detailed in the supplementary information’s repeated-measures ANOVA (Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Fig. 1). This inherent effect could counterbalance or mask the specific outcomes associated with upmodulation training.
Insula spectral power density shows no significant alteration following neurofeedback training
We conducted a spectral analysis of the resting-state data to compare the changes in right insula spectral power after two types of training (Fig. 7). No significant differences were found. Numerical source data for Fig. 7 are provided in Supplementary Data 1.
We observed no significant difference (n = 19, two-tailed paired Student’s t test, Bonferroni correction) in the spectral power changes (posttraining minus pretraining) after training. In the boxplots, dots represent individual data points, centerlines represent medians, box limits represent lower and upper quartiles, whiskers represent 1.5 times the interquartile range, and circles represent outliers.
Discussion
Our double-blind randomized controlled trial demonstrates that MEG-based neurofeedback, employing a cognitive modulation paradigm guided by the RMS of estimated insula currents, effectively reduces insula cortical activity. Notably, this modulation occurred not only during active neurofeedback but was also evident in resting-state measurements before and after training, providing robust evidence that the neurofeedback intervention induced sustained changes in cortical excitability of the insula. Although downmodulation of insula activity was accompanied by a significant increase in pain thresholds, the specificity of this effect compared with the upmodulation control condition was not confirmed. These findings suggest that insula activity is amenable to modulation via MEG-based neurofeedback and highlight its potential as a therapeutic target for pain management through repeated neurofeedback training. However, further evidence is needed. Due to the lack of a significant interaction effect between training type and pain threshold changes, these findings must be interpreted cautiously and do not establish a direct causal relationship between insula modulation and altered pain perception. We acknowledge the limitations of our study, particularly the relatively small sample size and the borderline significance of the results, which may affect the reliability and generalizability of our findings. Future studies with larger sample sizes are needed to validate these observations and further investigate the underlying effects.
Most current neurofeedback research employs EEG and fMRI29. EEG provides millisecond-level temporal resolution. However, most EEG-based neurofeedback (EEG-NFB) protocols do not modulate specific indicators of neural activity strength. Instead, they focus on regulating spontaneous brain rhythms, which are primarily defined by their oscillatory frequencies and may not directly correspond to the physiological activity of interest30. Due to the limited spatial resolution of EEGs, it is challenging to modulate brain activity in specific regions. Unlike EEG-NFB, fMRI-NFB offers millimeter-level spatial resolution and continues to guide participants in successfully modulating brain activity, as indicated by the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal31. However, the lower temporal resolution of fMRI data poses a challenge to the real-time nature of neurofeedback. Researchers have attempted to combine these modalities by using EEG-fMRI integration to balance their strengths but still face difficulties such as electromagnetic interactions between modalities that introduce artifacts affecting the signal-to-noise ratio, thereby reducing data quality compared to acquiring modalities separately and the challenge of effectively integrating the two modalities32,33. In our study, we used the RMS of estimated insula currents with a MEG-based brain-computer interface, achieving both high temporal resolution and relatively high spatial resolution and enabling the modulation of the mean cortical excitability of insula activity. Our study provides an alternative direction for the successful modulation of insula activity to improve neurofeedback methods.
The diverse functions of the insula include assessing the emotional aspects of pain, modulating cognitive processes related to pain anticipation and attention, regulating physiological responses (e.g., heart rate and blood pressure), perceiving internal body states, and adjusting pain thresholds on the basis of the sensory input context34,35,36,37,38,39. The successful modulation of insular activity in our research may impact pain perception across these dimensions. Further explorations of how neurofeedback may modulate pain thresholds could be performed in future studies by focusing on specific functions of the insula, thereby increasing our understanding of and ability to manage pain.
In previous studies, brain spectral power adjustments have predominantly been used to modulate insula activity40. While some research has indicated potential alterations in pain discrimination4, robust double-blind controlled studies demonstrating that changes in insula brain spectral power within specific frequency bands can modify pain perception or thresholds are lacking. In our study, we did not observe significant changes in spectral power within these specific bands that correlated with alterations in insula activity or changes in pain thresholds, suggesting that the overall neural activity in the insula might be more relevant to pain and could be a more appropriate target for neurofeedback.
Several studies have employed brain stimulation techniques such as TMS to modulate brain activity in deep regions like the insula. However, there is ongoing debate regarding whether these methods can effectively reach such deep areas due to limitations in the depth of stimulation. This controversy highlights the challenges inherent in non-invasive brain stimulation techniques. Nasr et al.41 discussed the challenges of traditional non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, particularly their limited precision and potential side effects when targeting deep brain regions. They emphasize the need for more advanced approaches that can overcome these limitations. Combination of neurofeedback with closed-loop approaches42, especially those that can reliably target deep brain areas such as ultrasound may offer a promising avenue for more effective and precise interventions. Neurofeedback combining with these advanced methods could potentially overcome the limitations of traditional brain stimulation techniques by providing real-time feedback and more accurate targeting.
Although our research was focused primarily on acute pain induced by thermal stimulation, the insula is known to play a role in both the modulation of pain perception and the development of chronic pain43. Neurofeedback treatments that modulate insula activities are therefore a plausible clinical option for patients with chronic pain. Two key steps are required to achieve this goal. First, it is necessary to show that insula activity is susceptible to neurofeedback control, and the findings of the current study suggest that this is the case. The second step is to show that training can have an impact on pain perception. For most neuromodulatory strategies, including neurofeedback, regular, repeated training sessions are required. The increased pain threshold we observed after a single session of neurofeedback is in keeping with this prediction, but for robust demonstration against the control condition, multiple sessions are likely required.
In conclusion, we demonstrated that MEG-based neurofeedback can be used to modulate insula activity, with a potential impact on pain perception. Our approach using estimated cortical currents in the insular cortex offers a distinct perspective on how to modulate brain activity through neurofeedback. Future studies should investigate the long-term effects of repeated neurofeedback training on pain perception and evaluate the efficacy of this approach in clinical populations with chronic pain.
Methods
Subjects
We recruited subjects through online solicitation. A total of 20 healthy volunteers, including 11 males and 9 females, were initially recruited for the study. One female subject withdrew partway through the study, resulting in 19 subjects (11 males and 8 females) completing the trial. We collected information solely on the biological sex of the subjects and did not gather data regarding their cultural gender. Efforts were made to ensure a balanced representation of sex among the participants. The mean age of the subjects was 28.8 years, with an age range of 22–58 years. All the subjects were right-handed, as determined by self-reports.
The study adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki, was performed per protocols approved by the Ethics Committee of Osaka University Clinical Trial Center (No. 18472-2) and was registered in the UMIN Clinical Trials Registry (UMIN000037063). We obtained written informed consent from all subjects after informing them of the purpose and possible consequences of this study. All ethical regulations relevant to human research participants were followed. Subjects received a compensation of 5000 JPY for each experimental session they attended, which was independent of their experimental performance.
Randomization and blinding
Before starting the study, we prepared a randomization list using block randomization with block sizes of 2 and 4. The list was kept confidential by the experiment operator. This list equally divided the twenty subjects we intended to recruit into two groups. The blocked randomization list was incorporated into the experiment initiation program, which was subsequently encrypted. During the setup, the operator input the subject’s ID to launch the neurofeedback program. Through this process, the subjects and the experiment operator were blinded to the training allocation. The data preprocessing and analysis procedures were conducted using participants’ individual IDs, without incorporating any group assignment information.
Pain thresholds
To assess the pain threshold, contact thermal stimuli were applied using the Medoc Pathways Pain & Sensory Evaluation System (Medoc Ltd., Ramat Yishai, Israel). A thermode was gently applied to the ventral side of the first web space on the subject’s left hand. We employed the limit method, wherein the thermode temperature was elevated at a steady rate of 1.5 °C/s from a starting temperature of 32 °C. The subjects were instructed to press a button when they perceived pain. This measurement was conducted 15 times. The initial 3 attempts were treated as trials and disregarded. The subject’s pain threshold was determined as the average temperature when the subject pressed the button.
MEG recordings
MEG signals were measured using a 160-channel whole-head MEG system equipped with coaxial gradiometers (MEGvision NEO, Yokogawa Electric Corporation, Kanazawa, Japan) housed in a magnetically shielded room. A projection screen was used to present visual stimuli (Presentation, Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA) from a liquid crystal projector (LVP-HC6800, Mitsubishi Electric, Tokyo, Japan). The data were captured in a frequency band ranging from 0.1 to 500 Hz and sampled at a rate of 2000 Hz.
Every subject assumed a supine posture within a magnetically protected chamber. Prior to initiating the MEG measurement, 5 fiducial coils were affixed to the subject’s face. The coil positions were ascertained both prior to and following each MEG session to establish the relative orientation and location of the MEG detectors to the subject’s cranial structure. The permissible maximum deviation in coil position from the beginning to the end of the recording was set to 5 mm. To coregister the MEG data with individual magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans (T1-weighted; Signa HDxt Excite 3.0 T, GE Healthcare UK Ltd., Buckinghamshire, UK), we captured the three-dimensional facial topology and mapped 50 points across each subject’s scalp using FastSCAN Cobra (Polhemus, Colchester, Vermont, USA). The 3D facial surface data were superimposed on the anatomical facial surface data extracted from the MR images.
During the resting-state recording phase, subjects were instructed to remain awake throughout the MEG scan, keep their gaze fixed on the central fixation point on the screen, and abstain from engaging in any taxing cognitive processes. Under these conditions, continuous MEG data were collected for a span surpassing 300 s for each resting-state session.
During the neurofeedback training phase, the subjects were directed to maintain their focus on the central fixation point on the screen. The subjects were tasked with exerting effort to enlarge the black disk displayed in the center of the screen. The total duration of the neurofeedback training was 1200 s, split into two sessions of 600 s each. Between sessions, we inquired about the subjects’ feelings and strategies. No additional breaks were provided during the training, except for this interval.
Online cortical current estimation by variational Bayesian multimodal encephalography
For online estimation of the cortical currents in right insular cortex from MEG data, we used Variational Bayesian Multimodal EncephaloGraphy (VBMEG)44,45 (ATR Neural Information Analysis Laboratories, Kyoto, Japan) in MATLAB (version R2013). First, cortical segmentation was performed on T1-weighted MR images via FreeSurfer software (Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA), generating a subject-specific cortical surface model. On this cortical surface, 4004 current dipoles were predefined, each positioned at a fixed location covering the entire cortex and oriented perpendicular to the local cortical surface. VBMEG was then used to estimate the time-varying current amplitude at each dipole by applying an MEG/EEG inverse solution incorporating fMRI-informed priors. To localize the right insular cortex, as defined in the AAL atlas, in each participant’s anatomical MRI, the individual MRI was spatially normalized to the MNI152 template space using SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, UK). This process generated a deformation field and an affine transformation matrix that mapped the subject space to MNI space. Following normalization, the inverse transformation was applied to the AAL atlas, which was originally defined in MNI space, to warp it into each participant’s native anatomical space. The right insular cortex label was then projected onto the subject’s cortical surface model, ensuring anatomical alignment with the individual’s brain structure. The transformed region was used as the region of interest for source localization and neurofeedback training within the VBMEG system. The lead field was computed by applying the boundary element method to the coregistered sensor positions and polygon model based on data collected immediately before each 10-min training session. Cortical currents were estimated from the MEG signals recorded from 110 selected sensors (Supplementary Fig. 3.). The hyperparameters m0 and γ0 were set to 100 and 10, respectively. This online cortical current estimation method was chosen because our previous researchs25,46 have confirmed its effectiveness in evaluating motor cortical responses for neurofeedback applications.
Neurofeedback system
Before the neurofeedback training session, MEG signals were first recorded for 300 s without the subject in the scanner. This environmental noise data was used to characterize baseline noise for signal preprocessing and to refine the VBMEG reconstruction filters. The neurofeedback training session began with a 300-s resting-state MEG recording, during which participants remained calm and fixated on the center of a disk presented in front of them. VBMEG was then applied to the resting-state MEG data to estimate the cortical currents at 4004 vertices over the 300-s period (Fig. 2b). Among these, we utilized the cortical currents \({{{{\rm{I}}}}}_{{{{\rm{v}}}}}\left({{{\rm{t}}}}\right)\) estimated at the v-th vertices \(\left(v=1\ldots 225\right)\) of the right insula cortex, as defined by the AAL2 parcellation provided in VBMEG. We define \(T\) as the total number of time points in the 300-second baseline period. For the estimated cortical currents of 300-second duration, we calculated the time average of \({{{{\rm{I}}}}}_{{{{\rm{v}}}}}\left({{{\rm{t}}}}\right)\) (\({\mu }_{{{{\rm{v}}}}}\)) and standard deviation (\({\sigma }_{{{{\rm{v}}}}}\)) for each vertex as follows:
Then, using \({{{{\rm{\mu }}}}}_{{{{\rm{v}}}}}\) and \({{{{\rm{\sigma }}}}}_{{{{\rm{v}}}}}\), we obtained the standardized current \(\hat{{{{{\rm{I}}}}}_{{{{\rm{v}}}}}}\left({{{\rm{t}}}}\right)\) for each v-th vertex at time t as follows:
The standardized current \(\hat{{I}_{v}}\left(t\right)\) of 300 s was segmented into 300 intervals of 1000 ms without overlaps, within which the standardized currents were time averaged to compute the mean values for the k-th interval, \(\bar{{I}_{v}}\left({{{\rm{k}}}}\right)\). We define \({{{\rm{V}}}}\) as the number of vertices in the right insular cortex. Then, we computed the RMS value of all vertices within the right insula cortex for the k-th time interval as follows:
The baseline insula activity and its standard deviation across 300 intervals were calculated as follows:
Similarly, we averaged the real-time estimated currents of the v-th vertex in the right insular cortex using a 200-ms time window and subsequently standardized the data by using \({\mu }_{v}\) and \({\sigma }_{v}\) to compute the real-time insula activity.
The real-time RMS value for insula activity was calculated as follows:
During neurofeedback training, the feedback signal was computed differently for upmodulation and downmodulation sessions on the basis of real-time insula activity.
Upmodulation Feedback:
For upmodulation, the aim of feedback was to increase insula activity. The feedback value was calculated as follows:
Downmodulation Feedback:
For downmodulation, the aim of feedback was to decrease insula activity. The feedback value was calculated as follows:
Clipping and Rounding:
The feedback values out_vals were rounded to the nearest integer and clipped to the range [0, 255]:
Notably, we did not remove artifacts to select the noisy time segments for the calculation for neurofeedback training. All the MEG signals obtained in the resting-state and neurofeedback sessions were used for the calculations above.
For both the upmodulation and the downmodulation conditions, the subjects were instructed to intentionally try to increase the size of the black disk on the screen. The neurofeedback strategies employed by participants and their perceived success rates in modulation were documented at the end of the experiment through unstructured interviews.
We selected the RMS of the insular source signals as the neurofeedback metric (instead of, for example, a frequency-domain power measure spanning 1–100 Hz) because the RMS provides a direct and efficient index of overall insular cortical activity in real time. Mathematically, the RMS of a broadband signal is proportional to the total power of that signal across the defined frequency range, meaning that the time-domain RMS effectively captures the aggregate neural power without requiring an explicit spectral decomposition. This approach takes advantage of the high temporal resolution of MEG by allowing immediate computation from the most recent data, without relying on sliding time windows or frequency transforms, thereby ensuring minimal latency and computational overhead. In practice, the RMS-based feedback is also straightforward to interpret: an increase in the RMS value corresponds directly to an increase in the insula’s signal amplitude (and thus overall neural activation), making it an intuitive measure of cortical excitability for both experimenters and participants. However, we acknowledge that using the RMS as the feedback signal lacks frequency specificity. Because the RMS collapses activity across all frequencies, it cannot distinguish which particular oscillatory bands (e.g., theta, alpha, beta, gamma) contribute to changes in insula activity. In other words, any frequency-specific neural dynamics within the insula are obscured when using a broadband amplitude metric. This trade-off was deemed acceptable for our purposes, given that our goal was to modulate the general level of insular activity rather than target a specific EEG or MEG frequency band. We opted not to use a frequency-domain power calculation for the feedback (such as summing the power spectral density from 1 to 100 Hz in each interval) because it offered no clear advantage over the time-domain RMS while incurring additional complexity. A frequency-domain approach would require computing power spectra for each time segment and integrating across frequencies, operations that could introduce extra latency or noise in the real-time feedback loop. In contrast, the RMS measure yields an equivalent reflection of total insular activity power in a simpler and more time-responsive manner, albeit at the expense of detailed spectral information.
Resting-state data processing
Resting-state MEG data processing was performed using Brainstorm, which is an open-source application available under the GNU general public license (http://neuroimage.usc.edu/brainstorm), and MATLAB version R2017b. To ensure consistency with the online analysis, we also utilized the selected 110 channels. Cortical surfaces reconstructed from each subject’s T1-weighted anatomical MRI scan using FreeSurfer were imported into Brainstorm. The standard preprocessing pipeline was applied; the data were first visually inspected to remove artifacts, such as eye blinks and muscle movements. Artifacts were removed through visual inspection and by applying the Signal Space Separation method. Cardiac and ocular artifacts were further minimized by detecting and removing signal segments corresponding to heartbeats and eye movements using event-based artifact rejection techniques. Bad channel detection and replacement, and head position correction; then, we applied a notch filter at powerline frequencies and harmonics to attenuate powerline artifacts. A bandpass filter was used to remove low-frequency signals (<0.3 Hz). Source reconstruction was performed using the minimum-norm estimation (MNE) method, We applied the dynamic statistical parametric mapping (dSPM)47, which is derived from the default MNE and standardized via the noise covariance matrix. The resulting dSPM maps represent a set of z scores, expressed in unitless “z” values. Therefore, we used the noise covariance matrix, which was generated from the MEG signals of 5 min of empty-room data, to the source reconstruction in Brainstorm. Then, the individual source was projected into default MNI ICBM152/FreeSurfer 15000 vertices anatomy. The time course of the source activity was extracted from the right insular cortex using the AAL2 template provided in Brainstorm. The RMS value of the mean absolute estimated currents across all vertices within the right insula cortex was extracted to represent resting-state insula activity.
The relative power spectral density (PSD) was estimated from source activity using the Welch method. We computed the average PSD power in the delta (0.3–4), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz) bands.
It should be noted that we used two source reconstruction methods to ensure reproducibility and achieve high-performance neurofeedback. MNE in Brainstorm was applied for offline analysis of MEG signals during the resting state, while VBMEG was used for online neurofeedback experiments. MNE was chosen for offline analysis because it is a commonly used method, facilitating easier reproduction of our results compared to VBMEG. On the other hand, VBMEG can estimate cortical currents with high accuracy in real time, and its efficacy has been validated in our previous studies25,46. therefore, we constructed our neurofeedback system with VBMEG.
Insular activity
The insular activity is quantified by computing the RMS value of estimated currents across all vertices within the right insula cortex for each time segment. This RMS value represents the level of insula activity during that specific interval. The formulas are provided below, with symbols defined previously:
Sample size calculation
We used G*Power 3.1 to calculate the required sample size for a paired t test for within-subject comparisons, specifically for changes in insula activity during training and pre/post changes in insula activity and pain thresholds. The effect size (Cohen’s d48) was set to 0.7 (large effect), with a power of 0.8 and a significance level of 0.05. The results indicated that at least 19 participants were required for the study. Based on this calculation, we initially set the sample size to 20 participants. Although relatively small, this sample size (n = 20) is consistent with previous fMRI and MEG decoded neurofeedback studies, which typically include 10–20 participants25,27,46,49,50,51,52,53,54.
Post hoc effect size calculations indicate that insula activity measures during neurofeedback training and resting-state changes exhibited moderate effect sizes (0.497 and 0.503, respectively). However, the effect size for pain threshold changes was substantially smaller (0.109), suggesting that our initial estimate of 0.7 may have been overly optimistic. To improve the reliability of our findings, we plan to increase the sample size in future studies.
Statistical analysis and reproducibility
Statistical analysis was performed via Python 3.8.5 with the scipy package v.1.5.255. Two-sided statistical tests were performed for all the subjects.
To test Hypothesis 1 (pre- versus posttraining changes in insula activity), we performed a paired t test to compare insula activity measured before and after neurofeedback training. To evaluate Hypothesis 2 (changes in pain thresholds associated with modulation of insula activity), we similarly employed a paired t test on pain threshold data measured before and after training. Exploratory analyses assessing oscillatory power changes across multiple frequency bands were also conducted using paired t tests.
To control for multiple comparisons arising from related statistical tests (e.g., analyses of multiple frequency bands in exploratory testing), we applied Bonferroni correction by adjusting the original significance threshold (α = 0.05) according to the number of comparisons performed (n). Specifically, the adjusted threshold for statistical significance was set as α = 0.05/n for each set of related comparisons.
Reporting Summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
Numerical source data for all graphs in the manuscript can be found in Supplementary Data 1 file. The data for this research can be accessed upon request from the corresponding author (T.Y.). The data are not openly accessible due to privacy and/or consent concerns regarding the research participants.
Code availability
The code utilized in this research can be obtained by contacting the corresponding author (T.Y.).
References
Garcia-Larrea, L. & Peyron, R. Pain matrices and neuropathic pain matrices: a review. Pain 154, S29–S43 (2013).
Jensen, K. B. et al. Brain activations during pain: a neuroimaging meta-analysis of patients with pain and healthy controls. Pain 157, 1279–1286 (2016).
Apkarian, A. V., Bushnell, M. C., Treede, R. D. & Zubieta, J. K. Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur. J. Pain. 9, 463–484 (2005).
Taesler, P. & Rose, M. The modulation of neural insular activity by a brain computer interface differentially affects pain discrimination. Sci. Rep. 11, 9795 (2021).
Mazzola, L., Isnard, J., Peyron, R., Guenot, M. & Mauguiere, F. Somatotopic organization of pain responses to direct electrical stimulation of the human insular cortex. Pain 146, 99–104 (2009).
Isnard, J., Magnin, M., Jung, J., Mauguiere, F. & Garcia-Larrea, L. Does the insula tell our brain that we are in pain?. Pain 152, 946–951 (2011).
Segerdahl, A. R., Mezue, M., Okell, T. W., Farrar, J. T. & Tracey, I. The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 499–500 (2015).
Seymour, B., Crook, R. J. & Chen, Z. S. Post-injury pain and behaviour: a control theory perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 378–392 (2023).
Barroso, J. et al. Subcortical brain anatomy as a potential biomarker of persistent pain after total knee replacement in osteoarthritis. Pain 164, 2306–2315 (2023).
Zebhauser, P. T., Hohn, V. D. & Ploner, M. Resting-state electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography as biomarkers of chronic pain: a systematic review. Pain 164, 1200–1221 (2023).
Li, H., Gan, Z., Wang, L., Oswald, M. J. & Kuner, R. Prolonged Suppression of Neuropathic Hypersensitivity upon Neurostimulation of the Posterior Insula in Mice. Cells 11, 3303 (2022).
Chen, J. et al. Insula→amygdala and insula→thalamus pathways are involved in comorbid chronic pain and depression-like behavior in mice. J. Neurosci. 44, e2062232024 (2024).
Frot, M., Faillenot, I. & Mauguiere, F. Processing of nociceptive input from posterior to anterior insula in humans. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5486–5499 (2014).
Coghill, R. C. et al. Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human brain. J. Neurosci. 14, 4095–4108 (1994).
Coghill, R. C., Sang, C. N., Maisog, J. M. & Iadarola, M. J. Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 1934–1943 (1999).
Oshiro, Y., Quevedo, A. S., McHaffie, J. G., Kraft, R. A. & Coghill, R. C. Brain mechanisms supporting discrimination of sensory features of pain: a new model. J. Neurosci. 29, 14924–14931 (2009).
Alkire, M. T., White, N. S., Hsieh, R. & Haier, R. J. Dissociable brain activation responses to 5-Hz electrical pain stimulation: a high-field functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Anesthesiology 100, 939–946 (2004).
Baliki, M. N., Geha, P. Y. & Apkarian, A. V. Parsing pain perception between nociceptive representation and magnitude estimation. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 875–887 (2009).
Ferraro, S. et al. Dysregulated anterior insula reactivity as robust functional biomarker for chronic pain-Meta-analytic evidence from neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 43, 998–1010 (2022).
Harte, S. E. et al. Pharmacologic attenuation of cross-modal sensory augmentation within the chronic pain insula. Pain 157, 1933–1945 (2016).
Brown, C. A., El-Deredy, W. & Jones, A. K. When the brain expects pain: common neural responses to pain anticipation are related to clinical pain and distress in fibromyalgia and osteoarthritis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39, 663–672 (2014).
Kim, J. H. et al. Impaired insula functional connectivity associated with persistent pain perception in patients with complex regional pain syndrome. PLoS ONE 12, e0180479 (2017).
Liu, C. C., Moosa, S., Quigg, M. & Elias, W. J. Anterior insula stimulation increases pain threshold in humans: a pilot study. J. Neurosurg. 135, 1487–1492 (2021).
Soekadar, S. R. et al. Future Developments in Brain/Neural–Computer Interface Technology in Policy, Identity, and Neurotechnology: The Neuroethics of Brain-Computer Interfaces (eds. Dubljević, V. & Coin, A.) 65-85 (Springer International Publishing, 2023).
Yanagisawa, T. et al. Induced sensorimotor brain plasticity controls pain in phantom limb patients. Nat. Commun. 7, 13209 (2016).
Emmert, K. et al. Comparison of anterior cingulate vs. insular cortex as targets for real-time fMRI regulation during pain stimulation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 350 (2014).
Zhang, S. et al. Pain control by co-adaptive learning in a brain-machine interface. Curr. Biol. 30, 3935–3944 (2020).
Rolls, E. T., Joliot, M. & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. Implementation of a new parcellation of the orbitofrontal cortex in the automated anatomical labeling atlas. Neuroimage 122, 1–5 (2015).
Weber, L. A., Ethofer, T. & Ehlis, A. C. Predictors of neurofeedback training outcome: a systematic review. Neuroimage Clin. 27, 102301 (2020).
Batail, J. M. et al. EEG neurofeedback research: a fertile ground for psychiatry?. Encephale 45, 245–255 (2019).
Thibault, R. T., MacPherson, A., Lifshitz, M., Roth, R. R. & Raz, A. Neurofeedback with fMRI: a critical systematic review. Neuroimage 172, 786–807 (2018).
Jorge, J., van der Zwaag, W. & Figueiredo, P. EEG-fMRI integration for the study of human brain function. Neuroimage 102, 24–34 (2014).
Philiastides, M. G., Tu, T. & Sajda, P. Inferring macroscale brain dynamics via fusion of simultaneous EEG-fMRI. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 44, 315–334 (2021).
Orenius, T. I. et al. The interaction of emotion and pain in the insula and secondary somatosensory cortex. Neuroscience 349, 185–194 (2017).
Hoskin, R. & Talmi, D. Adaptive coding of pain prediction error in the anterior insula. Eur. J. Pain. 27, 766–778 (2023).
Ullsperger, M., Harsay, H. A., Wessel, J. R. & Ridderinkhof, K. R. Conscious perception of errors and its relation to the anterior insula. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 629–643 (2010).
Cortelli, P., Giannini, G., Favoni, V., Cevoli, S. & Pierangeli, G. Nociception and autonomic nervous system. Neurol. Sci. 34, S41–S46 (2013).
Rahmani, M. & Rahmani, F. Cortex, Insula, and Interoception in Biophysics and Neurophysiology of the Sixth Sense (eds. Rezaei, N. & Saghazadeh, A.) 59-68 (Springer International Publishing, 2019).
Kuehn, E., Mueller, K., Lohmann, G. & Schuetz-Bosbach, S. Interoceptive awareness changes the posterior insula functional connectivity profile. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 1555–1571 (2016).
Roy, R., de la Vega, R., Jensen, M. P. & Miro, J. Neurofeedback for pain management: a systematic review. Front. Neurosci. 14, 671 (2020).
Nasr, K. et al. Breaking the boundaries of interacting with the human brain using adaptive closed-loop stimulation. Prog. Neurobiol. 216, 102311 (2022).
Haslacher, D. et al. In vivo phase-dependent enhancement and suppression of human brain oscillations by transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Neuroimage 275, 120187 (2023).
Lu, C. et al. Insular cortex is critical for the perception, modulation, and chronification of pain. Neurosci. Bull. 32, 191–201 (2016).
Sato, M. A. et al. Hierarchical Bayesian estimation for MEG inverse problem. Neuroimage 23, 806–826 (2004).
Yoshioka, T. et al. Evaluation of hierarchical Bayesian method through retinotopic brain activities reconstruction from fMRI and MEG signals. Neuroimage 42, 1397–1413 (2008).
Yanagisawa, T. et al. BCI training to move a virtual hand reduces phantom limb pain: A randomized crossover trial. Neurology 95, e417–e426 (2020).
Dale, A. M. et al. Dynamic statistical parametric mapping: combining fMRI and MEG for high-resolution imaging of cortical activity. Neuron 26, 55–67 (2000).
Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (routledge, 2013).
Shibata, K., Watanabe, T., Sasaki, Y. & Kawato, M. Perceptual learning incepted by decoded fMRI neurofeedback without stimulus presentation. Science 334, 1413–1415 (2011).
Cortese, A., Amano, K., Koizumi, A., Kawato, M. & Lau, H. Multivoxel neurofeedback selectively modulates confidence without changing perceptual performance. Nat. Commun. 7, 13669 (2016).
Emmert, K. et al. Meta-analysis of real-time fMRI neurofeedback studies using individual participant data: How is brain regulation mediated?. Neuroimage 124, 806–812 (2016).
Koizumi, A. et al. Fear reduction without fear through reinforcement of neural activity that bypasses conscious exposure. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0006 (2016).
Nicholson, A. A. et al. The neurobiology of emotion regulation in posttraumatic stress disorder: Amygdala downregulation via real-time fMRI neurofeedback. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 541–560 (2017).
Sherwood, M. S. et al. Self-directed down-regulation of auditory cortex activity mediated by real-time fMRI neurofeedback augments attentional processes, resting cerebral perfusion, and auditory activation. Neuroimage 195, 475–489 (2019).
Virtanen, P. et al. SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat. Methods 17, 261–272 (2020).
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (JP19dm0307008). Additional support came from Japan Science and Technology Agency, the Core Research for Evolutional Science and Technology (JPMJCR18A5), Exploratory Research for Advanced Technology (JPMJER1801), the Moonshot R&D–MILLENNIA Program (JPMJMS2012), and the AIP Acceleration Research (JPMJCR24U2). We also received Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JP20H05705) and the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (19dm0207070h0001 and JP24wm0625517). This project is also supported by the National Institute for Health and Care Research (NIHR) Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre, the Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC, EP/W03509X/1), the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS, 22H04998), the Wellcome Trust (214251/Z/18/Z), and the National Institute for Health and Care Research (NIHR, 203316), all to B.S. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the funding bodies.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Y.W.: methodology, investigation, data analysis, data interpretation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review & editing, literature review. R.F.: software, data analysis, writing—review & editing, methodology. B.S.: conceptualization, writing—review & editing. H.Y.: data analysis, writing—review & editing. H.K.: writing—review & editing. T.Y.: conceptualization, methodology, software, supervision, data analysis, writing—review & editing.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Biology thanks Surjo R. Soekadar and the other, anonymous, reviewer for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: Shenbing Kuang and Joao Valente. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Wang, Y., Fukuma, R., Seymour, B. et al. Neurofeedback modulation of insula activity via MEG-based brain-machine interface: a double-blind randomized controlled crossover trial. Commun Biol 8, 770 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-025-08176-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-025-08176-8