Abstract
In this cross-sectional analysis of the UK Biobank, we investigated whether biological sex moderates the association of residential nature exposure with brain volume or cognitive function. We included 11,448 cognitively healthy UK residents aged 37–73 years (98% White; 51% female). Residential nature exposure was estimated as the percentage of land classified as natural environment within 1000 m and 300 m buffers around each participant’s home. Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) outcomes included total grey matter volume, total white matter volume, and average hippocampal volume, all normalized for head size. Cognitive function was assessed with the Trail Making Test (B-A) and the Symbol Digit Substituton Test. In linear regression models, higher residential nature exposure at both buffer sizes was associated with greater grey matter volume (1000 m: β = 629 mm3 per 10% increment in nature exposure; 95% CI: 234 to 1023; p = 0.002; 300 m: β = 642 mm3; 95% CI: 286 to 997; p < 0.001), greater white matter volume (1000 m: 659 mm3; 95% CI: 229 to 1089; p = 0.003; 300 m: β = 527 mm3; 95% CI: 140 to 914; p = 0.008), and more correct matches on the Symbol Digit Substituton Test (1000 m: β = 0.106 matches; 95% CI: 0.057 to 0.154; p < 0.001; 300 m: β = 0.049 matches; 95% CI: 0.006 to 0.092; p = 0.03). In males, compared with females, higher nature exposure was associated with a greater increment in grey matter volume (1000 m; β = 635 mm3; 95% CI: 53 to 1217; p = 0.03; 300 m: β = 634 mm3; 95% CI: 102 to 1167; p = 0.02), and with a greater reduction in Trail-Making Test B-A time (300 m only: β = 0.284 s; 95% CI: 0.016 to 0.551; p = 0.04). These sex differences showed some sensitivity to participant residence changes and to type of nature exposure measure. Residential nature exposure may support brain volume and cognitive function, and some of the potential benefits may vary by sex.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Dementia is a major global healthcare burden1. With no effective pharmaceutical treatment widely available, the World Health Organization urges the development of low-cost, accessible strategies to prevent or delay the onset and progression of cognitive impairment and dementia2.
Nature spaces, like parks and forests, are a community-level feature that may support brain health and reduce the risk of cognitive decline3. Observational evidence suggests that, among adults, residential nature exposure is associated with greater regional grey matter volume4, cortical thickness5,6,7, and structural integrity8, as well as better cognitive performance across multiple domains5,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17.
Despite these promising findings, there are key limitations to our knowledge about the relationship of nature exposure with brain and cognitive health. Most prior studies have focused on greenspace, i.e., outdoor land covered in green vegetation18,19, rather than all types of nature spaces (e.g., land covered by vegetation, dirt, rock, or water)20,21. These studies may therefore have underrepresented nature exposure22, leading to less accurate estimates of nature-health associations. Moreover, few studies have examined potential moderators of the association of nature exposure with brain or cognitive health. Identifying moderators would help inform on the conditions and the individual characteristics under which nature exposure may be beneficial3.
Biological sex is a moderator of interest because there are notable sex differences in dementia risk and in brain and cognitive aging23,24,25,26,27. Females have double the prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) versus males28, show greater brain atrophy than males after onset of mild cognitive impairment or AD24, and experience greater β-amyloid deposition and hippocampal atrophy during the menopausal transition, as compared with age-matched males25. Moreover, various modifiable factors are hypothesized to link nature exposure to brain or cognitive health—such as stress, mental health, physical activity, and air pollution22—and there are sex differences within many of these pathways22,2329,30,31,32,33,34,35. For example, females are more vulnerable to stress-associated cognitive decline or impairment32,36,37, and nature exposure can reduce stress38,39,40, potentially in a sex-dependent manner33,34,35. Females may therefore be more responsive to brain or cognitive benefits of nature that occur through stress reduction. Given these potentially interacting pathways, it is plausible that sex could moderate the association of nature exposure with brain volume or with cognitive function.
Few studies have examined whether biological sex moderates the association of nature exposure with brain volume or with cognitive function, and the results have been equivocal7,10,11,16,41. For example, Besser et al. (2021) did not find sex differences in the association of residential greenspace with brain structure41, while De Keijzer et al. (2018) found that residential greenspace was associated with slower global cognitive decline in females than in males16. Among these studies, most had small samples (N < 300),7,10,11 and/or conducted sex-stratified analyses without formally testing moderation by sex7,10,11,16. Given the potential of nature exposure in promoting brain and cognitive health3, and that nearly 2/3 of dementia cases are among females28, more research—using more comprehensive measures of nature exposure, large samples, and formal tests of moderation—is warranted to clarify if there are sex differences in the association of nature exposure with brain and cognitive health.
To address these gaps, using data from the UK Biobank, we assessed the association of nature exposure with brain volume or with cognitive function, and potential sex differences therein.
Methods
Participants
The UK Biobank (http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk) is a prospective cohort with genetic, lifestyle, and health data on over 502,000 UK residents42. From 2006 to 2010, individuals aged 37–73 years and registered with the National Health Service (NHS) were recruited by mail to one of 22 assessment centres throughout England, Wales, and Scotland for a baseline assessment, at which time nature exposure data were documented. From 2014 to 2015, all 340,000 participants with an email address were invited by email to complete an online, self-administered neuropsychological battery; ~120,000 participants completed all online neuropsychological tests. From 2014 onward, ~ 50,000 participants completed structural brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans at one of three imaging centres.
Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and consents
Ethics approval for the UK Biobank study was provided by the NHS National Research Ethics Service (11/NW/0382). All UK Biobank participants gave written informed consent prior to participation. All research was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. The current analyses were conducted on data extracted October 7, 2021 as part of UK Biobank Application 69,022.
Eligibility criteria
We included participants with complete data for nature exposure (collected at baseline, 2006–2010), structural brain MRI (completed 2014–2020), neuropsychological testing (completed 2014–2015), and covariates (collected at baseline, 2006–2010). We excluded participants with a diagnosis of dementia, Parkinson’s disease, or stroke.
Measures
Nature exposure
Residential nature exposure was estimated as the percentage of land cover classified as “natural environment”—as opposed to “built environment”—in 1000-m and 300-m buffers around participants’ home locations. Land cover data were obtained from the UK Centre for Ecology and Hydrology (CEH) 2007 Land Cover Map (LCM)43. Given the variable findings and uncertainty regarding buffer sizes relevant to nature-health associations22,44, we used both the 1000-m and 300-m buffers to examine the broader neighbourhood and the immediate residential area, respectively, as potentially relevant geographic contexts44,45. We chose the land cover-based “natural environment” measure for our primary analysis, rather than the land use-based “greenspace” measure, because it includes a broader set of natural environment types beyond just greenspace (e.g., water and rock areas). Sensitivity analyses with the alternative nature exposure measure (“greenspace”) are described below.
Structural brain MRI
The UK Biobank MRI acquisition protocol46 and processing pipeline47 have been described previously and are documented on the UK Biobank website48. Structural brain MRI scans were acquired on a Siemens Skyra 3 T scanner, with a standard 32-channel RF receive head coil46. Brain volumes were generated from T1-weighted structural images by the image-processing pipeline developed by and run on behalf of the UK Biobank47. Our study outcomes included total grey matter volume, total white matter volume, and hippocampal volume (averaged across both hemispheres). Each brain volume variable was normalized for head size. Total grey and white matter volumes were normalised for head size by the UK Biobank using a T1-based estimate of the external surface of the skull47. To normalise hippocampal volume, which was provided as a raw value, we multiplied the raw volume by the T1-based head size scaling factor provided by the UK Biobank (data field 25000).
Cognitive function
UK Biobank participants completed an online neuropsychological testing battery from 2014 to 2015, detailed on the UK Biobank website49. Among these tests, we examined the Trail Making Test (TMT) versions A and B and the Symbol Digit Substituton Test because they are sensitive to age-related decline and changes in cognitive status50,51,52, and associated with daily function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment53,54. Additionally, a systematic review of acute experimental studies showed that executive functions were benefitted by exposure to natural environments, compared with built environments55.
In TMT A, participants were presented a series of circles labelled with numbers, and instructed to connect them in ascending order. In TMT B, participants were presented a series of circles labelled with numbers or letters, and instructed to connect them in ascending order, alternating between numbers and letters56. We indexed the difference in time to complete versions B and A (i.e., TMT B-A) as a measure of set-shifting, with a smaller time difference indicating better performance51.
The Symbol Digit Substituton Test measures complex processing speed57. Participants were presented a sequence of randomly arranged symbols, and instructed to match each symbol to a number according to a code provided. The total number of correct matches made in one minute was the score, with a higher number indicating better complex processing speed57.
Moderator
Biological sex was recorded as male or female, acquired from the NHS central registry at recruitment.
Covariates
We identified ten covariates that have previously been associated with brain and cognitive health58,59,60. Age at baseline was derived from date of birth and truncated to whole years. BMI was calculated from height and weight measured in-person. Participants self-reported their educational attainment, average annual total household income, time spent outdoors, smoking status, and disability status. Physical activity was self-reported using the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) short form61. We estimated neighbourhood-level socioeconomic status with the Index of Multiple Deprivation62. Home area population density was determined by the UK Biobank from participant postal codes at the time of recruitment matched to 2001 census data.
Other descriptive variables
To help characterize our sample, we also included the following descriptive variables, all of which were self-reported by participants through a digital questionnaire: ethnic background, hearing difficulties, diabetes diagnosed by a doctor, and cardiovascular problems diagnosed by a doctor (including heart attack, angina, stroke, and hypertension).
Statistical analysis
We performed all analyses in R (version 4.2.3) using RStudio (version 2022.02.3). Descriptive statistics for demographic and outcome variables were calculated, wherein we tested differences between sexes using independent samples t-tests for continuous variables and chi-squared tests for categorical variables.
We conducted linear regressions to assess the independent associations of residential nature exposure with brain volume or cognitive function. Separate models for each brain and cognitive outcome were developed, adjusting for all covariates. Crude models, adjusted only for age, sex, and ethnic background, were also developed (Supplementary Tables S1 and S2).
We then assessed whether sex moderated the associations of residential nature exposure with brain volume or cognitive function by adding an interaction term of nature exposure*sex to all regression models. For outcomes with significant interactions, we conducted post-hoc, sex-stratified regressions to evaluate the association of residential nature exposure with the outcome within each sex.
We conducted two sensitivity analyses. First, as an alternative nature exposure measure, we used the percentage of land classified as “greenspace”, among 10 land-use categories, in 1000-m and 300-m buffers around participants’ home locations. Land use data were obtained from the 2005 Generalized Land Use Database for England (GLUD) for 2001 Census Output Areas, and were therefore limited to participants residing in England63. We conducted these analyses because reviews of observational studies on nature-health associations indicate that the nature exposure measure can moderate the strength and significance of associations, and thus recommend the use of multiple exposure measures22,44,64. Secondly, to account for possible changes in residence between the time of data collection for nature exposure (baseline; 2006–2010) and the time of the MRI and cognitive assessments (follow-up; 2014–2015), we repeated all analyses while restricting the sample to participants whose residence remained unchanged between the baseline and follow-up assessments. To do this, we excluded participants whose time at current address was less than the time between baseline and follow-up assessments.
All analyses were exploratory; the overall alpha was < 0.05.
Results
Participant characteristics
From the parent sample of 502,422 UK Biobank participants, our final analytic sample included 11,448 individuals (Fig. 1). Our sample had a mean age of 55.0 years (SD = 7.5 years), 51% were female (n = 5,784), and 98% were White.
Participant flow for the current cross-sectional analysis of the UK Biobank. Note that the total number of participants excluded for missing data accounts for overlap, i.e.. participants with missing data across more than one of the four categories of variables (nature exposure, brain structure, cognitive function, or covariates).
Table 1 shows sex-stratified sample characteristics. Males were older, spent more time outdoors, had a higher prevalence of hearing difficulty, diabetes, and cardiovascular problems, and had a lower prevalence of probable major depressive disorder. Females had lower residential natural environment and greenspace percentage (buffered to 1000 m and 300 m), better scores on the Trail-Making Test B-A and the Symbol Digit Substituton Test, greater total grey matter volume (normalized for head size) and average hippocampal volume (normalized for head size), and lower total white matter volume (normalized for head size).
Associations of residential nature exposure with brain volume or cognitive function
At both the 1000 m and 300 m buffers, every 10% increment in residential nature exposure was associated with greater grey matter volume (1000 m: β = 629 mm3; 95% CI: 234 to 1023; p = 0.002; 300 m: β = 642 mm3; 95% CI: 286 to 997; p < 0.001), greater white matter volume (1000 m: 659 mm3; 95% CI: 229 to 1089; p = 0.003; 300 m: β = 527 mm3; 95% CI: 140 to 914; p = 0.008), and more correct matches on the Symbol Digit Substituton Test (1000 m: β = 0.106 matches; 95% CI: 0.057 to 0.154; p < 0.001; 300 m: β = 0.049 matches; 95% CI: 0.006 to 0.092; p = 0.03; Table 2). Residential nature exposure was not associated with average hippocampal volume (1000 m: β = 0.39 mm3 per 10% increment in nature exposure; 95% CI: −5.47 to 6.25; p = 0.90; 300 m: β=−3.76mm3; 95% CI: −9.04 to 0.00; p = 0.16), nor with the TMT B-A (1000 m: β=−0.197 s; 95% CI: −0.395 to 0.001, p = 0.05; 300 m: β=−0.120 s; 95% CI: −0.299 to 0.058, p = 0.19).
Crude models—adjusted only for age, sex, and ethnic background—replicated the results of the fully adjusted models described above (Supplementary Table S1).
Moderation by biological sex on the association of residential nature exposure with brain volume or cognitive function
Biological sex moderated the association of residential nature exposure with total grey matter volume, such that every 10% increment in nature exposure was associated with a greater increment in grey matter volume in males than in females, for both the 1000 m (β = 635 mm3; 95% CI: 53 to 1217; p = 0.03) and 300 m buffers (β = 634 mm3; 95% CI: 102 to 1167; p = 0.02; Table 3). In post-hoc, sex-stratified analyses, the association of residential nature exposure with grey matter volume was significant in males (1000 m: β = 797 mm3 per 10% increment in nature exposure; 95% CI: 234 to 1359; p = 0.006; 300 m: β = 882 mm3; 95% CI: 374 to 1391; p < 0.001), but not in females (1000 m: β = 444 mm3; 95% CI: −108 to 996; p = 0.11; 300 m: 447 mm3; 95% CI: −47 to 941; p = 0.08).
For the 300 m nature exposure buffer—but not for the 1000 m buffer—biological sex also moderated the association of residential nature exposure with TMT B-A (Table 3). Every 10% increment in nature exposure was associated with a 0.284 s greater decrement in time in males than in females (95% CI: 0.016 to 0.551; p = 0.04). In post-hoc, sex-stratified analyses, the association of residential nature exposure with TMT B-A was significant only in males (β=−0.318 s per 10% increment in nature exposure; 95% CI: −0.564 to −0.073; p = 0.01; females: β = 0.064 s; 95% CI: −0.193 to 0.322; p = 0.62).
Biological sex did not moderate the association of residential nature exposure with other brain volume or cognitive function outcomes.
Crude models—adjusted only for age, sex, and ethnic background—replicated the results of the fully adjusted models described above (Supplementary Table S2).
Sensitivity analyses
When we used greenspace—rather than natural environment—as the nature exposure measure, all except two results remained the same as the main analyses (Supplementary Tables S3 and S4). The association of residential nature exposure (buffered to 300 m) with the Digit Symbol Substitution test remained in the same direction, but was no longer significant (β = 0.031 matches; 95% CI: −0.007 to 0.069; p = 0.11). For moderation by sex on the association of residential nature exposure (buffered to 300 m) with TMT B-A, the result also remained in the same direction, but was no longer significant (β = 0.159 s more improvement in time in males than in females per 10% increment in nature exposure; 95% CI: −0.086 to 0.403; p = 0.20).
When we restricted the sample to participants with no changes in residence between exposure (2006–2010) and outcome (2014–2020) assessments, results remained largely unchanged (Supplementary Tables S5 and S6). However, for moderation by sex on the association of residential nature exposure (buffered to 1000 m) with total grey matter volume, the result remained in the same direction, but was no longer significant (β = 641 mm3 more in males than in females per 10% increment in nature exposure; 95% CI: −11 to 1292; p = 0.05).
Discussion
In this cross-sectional analysis of the UK Biobank, we found that residential nature exposure was associated with greater total grey matter volume and greater total white matter volume, and better complex processing speed. Sex moderated the association of residential nature exposure with total grey matter volume and with set-shifting, whereby the associations were stronger among males than among females.
Our findings of favourable associations between residential nature exposure and brain volume concur with prior cross-sectional studies in adults that found favourable associations of residential greenspace with regional grey matter volumes4, regional and global cortical thickness5,6,7, regional structural integrity8, and ventricle burden41. Together with the current study, the evidence to date generally suggests that greater residential nature exposure is associated with better brain structure in adults.
While beyond the scope of the current study, three broad pathways have been proposed by which nature exposure may benefit brain health: (1) reducing stress, restoring attention, and supporting mental health; (2) mitigating harmful stimuli, such as air pollution and noise; and (3) facilitating health-related behaviours or capacities, such as physical activity, social engagement, sleep, and immune function3,22,65,66. These pathways have been investigated as mediators by a small number of observational studies, with unclear results3. Future longitudinal analyses are needed to verify current findings and to elucidate mediating pathways by which nature exposure may promote brain health.
We found an association of residential nature exposure with complex processing speed, but not with set-shifting. Specifically, every 10% increment in residential nature exposure was associated with 0.106 (1000 m buffer) or 0.031 (300 m buffer) more correct matches on the Symbol Digit Substituton Test. However, in the Swedish BioFINDER cohort study, the minimum clinically important difference (MCID) on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test was 3.5 matches for cognitively unimpaired older adults67. Thus, the nature exposure–processing speed association observed in our study may not be clinically meaningful. Our findings are comparable to those of previous nature exposure studies examining similar cognitive outcomes3,9,14,15. A study of 13,594 women aged 50–69 years in the USA showed an association of residential NDVI with a psychomotor speed/attention composite, measured by the Cogstate Brief Battery9. In two studies conducted in Europe (n = 884; Mental Alternation Test)14 and in the USA (n = 1602; Colour Trails Test)15 using tests similar to the Trail-Making Test B, there was a mix of favourable and null associations of cognitive performance with various measures of nature exposure. While methodological differences between studies —including differences in nature exposure measures, outcome measures, sample sizes and demographics, and study design (cross-sectional versus longitudinal)— limit comparability and likely contribute to much of the variation in findings3,22,44, the potential benefit of nature exposure may vary by cognitive domain3,55,68.
We found a stronger association of residential nature exposure with grey matter volume among males than among females, across both buffer sizes and both nature exposure measures. Residential natural environment, buffered to 300 m, was also associated with better set-shifting among males than among females, although the difference—a 0.284-sec greater improvement in TMT B-A score per 10% increment in residential nature exposure—was much less than the MCID of 12.7 s reported for cognitively unimpaired older adults67. Our findings contrasted somewhat with the minimal literature on sex differences in the association of nature exposure with brain or cognitive health. Two cross-sectional studies in middle-aged7 and/or older adults7,41 found no sex differences in the association of residential greenspace with brain structure7,41, while two of three longitudinal studies found that residential greenspace was associated with slower cognitive decline in females but not in males10,11,16. Notably, only one of these five studies conducted a formal statistical test for moderation41, whereas the remaining studies conducted sex-stratified analyses7,10,11,16. Based on the sex differences suggested in the present study, more studies are needed to assess moderation by sex with formal statistical testing.
Some of our findings, especially our moderation results, were sensitive to buffer size, nature exposure measure, and participant changes in residence. When the exposure measure was greenspace instead of natural environment, two results were no longer significant: the association of nature exposure, buffered to 300 m, with complex processing speed, and moderation by sex on the association of nature exposure, buffered to 300 m, with set-shifting. These results suggest that nature spaces beyond just green vegetation may also contribute to potential cognitive benefits, and reiterate previous findings of favourable associations varying across nature exposure measures and/or qualities of the nature itself22. When we excluded those with changes in residence, moderation by sex on the association of residential nature exposure with total grey matter volume remained robust for the 300 m buffer, but was no longer significant—although remained in the same direction—for the 1000 m buffer. This result emphasizes the importance of longitudinal tracking of residential address in tandem with outcomes when studying residential nature exposure.
Compared to the 1000 m nature exposure buffer, the 300 m buffer showed an additional moderation result for set-shifting and a more robust moderation result for grey matter volume. The 300 m buffer may be more sensitive to sex differences in associations of nature exposure with brain and cognitive health, perhaps simply because individuals may be more likely to interact with or be impacted by nature spaces closer to their residence. Indeed, 300 m is commonly used in health research to reflect the immediate residential area and in policy-making as the maximum recommended distance to public open spaces69,70. Despite this, previous studies have found significant associations of nature exposure with brain and cognitive health across buffer sizes varying between 100 and 2000 m, and further work is required to clarify if any buffer sizes may be most relevant to brain or cognitive outcomes3,44.
There are many biological and behavioural mechanisms by which sex could interact with nature–brain health pathways to elicit sex differences in brain or cognitive outcomes. Examples include sex differences in stress responses31,32,35,37, mental health23, air pollution effects29,30, and physical activity responses71,72,73, as well as differences in the neuroprotective effects74 and aging-related trajectories25,75 of sex hormones. In particular, we note that the age of the UK Biobank sample overlaps with the typical age range of perimenopause and early menopause in females. The negative neuroendocrine changes during the menopausal transition25,75 may attenuate the possible beneficial effects of nature exposure in females, relative to males. Other notable factors exhibiting sex differences in the current study include depression, which was more prevalent in females, and cardiovascular problems (predominantly high blood pressure), which were more prevalent in males. The sex differences in these dementia risk factors58 could modulate either sex’s sensitivity to the potential benefits of nature exposure. These potential pathways for sex differences are an important avenue for future investigation.
This study had several limitations. First, the cross-sectional nature of the analysis precludes causal inference and allows for reverse-causality (e.g., healthier people choosing to live closer to nature)22,76. Second, the binary, land cover-based residential nature exposure measure does not account for the quality of the natural environment, the participant’s actual engagement with the nature, and the participant’s nature exposure during daily activities beyond the residence22. Third, the study sample was cognitively healthy, 98% White/Caucasian, and based in Great Britain, so results may not be generalizable to those with cognitive or neurological impairment (e.g., dementia or stroke), or to other ethnicities or other geographical regions. Fourth, these analyses were exploratory, with no adjustment for type I error inflation from multiple comparions77. Finally, although the APOE ε4 genotype is a critical dementia risk factor58, we were unable to include it as a covariate in our models because we did not have access to the necessary genotyping data.
Future longitudinal analyses examining associations between nature exposure and changes in brain health will help verify our findings and provide stronger evidence for a potentially causal relationship76. Alongside observational research, experimental studies, such as randomized controlled trials, are required to provide causal evidence and to investigate the effects of various types and doses of nature exposure on brain and cognitive health55,78. Future research should also test other potential moderators (e.g., age, menopausal status, ethnicity), as well as potential mediators (e.g., physical activity, stress, mental health, air pollution) of nature exposure on brain health3,22.
Among the strengths of our study, this was one of very few studies assessing—and one of even fewer studies statistically testing for—sex differences in the association of nature exposure with brain and cognitive health. Also, to our knowledge, we used by far the largest sample size among the few studies assessing the association of nature exposure with brain structure3,6,41.
Conclusions
Greater residential nature exposure may be associated with better brain and cognitive health in adults, and males may benefit more than females. These cross-sectional results add to the emerging observational literature on the potential utility of nature spaces as a community-level dementia prevention strategy. Future longitudinal studies are needed to verify and extend these findings.
Data availability
We analyzed data extracted from the UK Biobank on October 7, 2021 as part of UK Biobank Application 69022. Access to UK Biobank data is not publicly available but can be accessed by applying to the Research Analysis Platform described at the following link: https://dnanexus.gitbook.io/uk-biobank-rap. Code for all analyses can be viewed at: github.com/noseDr/UK-Biobank-Nature-Exposure-and-Brain-Health/tree/main.
References
Wimo, A. et al. The worldwide costs of dementia in 2019. Alzheimer’s Dement. 19, 2865–2873 (2023).
World Health Organization. Global Action Plan on the Public Health Response to Dementia 2017–2025. (2017).
Besser, L. Outdoor green space exposure and brain health measures related to alzheimer’s disease: a rapid review. BMJ Open. 11, 43456 (2021).
Falcón, C. et al. Brain correlates of urban environmental exposures in cognitively unimpaired individuals at increased risk for alzheimer’s disease: A study on barcelona’s population. Alzheimers Dement. (Amst) 13, (2021).
Dzhambov, A. M., Bahchevanov, K. M., Chompalov, K. A. & Atanassova, P. A. A feasibility study on the association between residential greenness and neurocognitive function in middle-aged bulgarians. Arh Hig Rada Toksikol. 70, 173–185 (2019).
Min, K. D. et al. New assessment for residential greenness and the association with cortical thickness in cognitively healthy adults. Sci. Total Environ. 778, 146129 (2021).
Crous-Bou, M. et al. Impact of urban environmental exposures on cognitive performance and brain structure of healthy individuals at risk for alzheimer’s dementia. Environ Int 138 (2020).
Kühn, S. et al. In search of features that constitute an enriched environment in humans: Associations between geographical properties and brain structure. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–8 (2017).
Jimenez, M. P. et al. Residential green space and cognitive function in a large cohort of Middle-Aged women. JAMA Netw. Open. 5, E229306 (2022).
Cherrie, M. P. C., Shortt, N. K., Thompson, C. W., Deary, I. J. & Pearce, J. R. Association between the activity space exposure to parks in childhood and adolescence and cognitive aging in later life. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2019. 16, 632 (2019).
Cherrie, M. P. C. et al. Green space and cognitive ageing: A retrospective life course analysis in the lothian birth cohort 1936. Soc. Sci. Med. 196, 56–65 (2018).
Zhu, A. et al. Association between residential greenness and cognitive function: analysis of the chinese longitudinal healthy longevity survey. BMJ Nutr. Prev. Health. 2, 72–79 (2019).
Clarke, P. J. et al. Cognitive function in the community setting: the neighbourhood as a source of ‘cognitive reserve’? J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 66, 730–736 (2012).
Prohaska, T. R. et al. Walking and the preservation of cognitive function in older populations. Gerontologist 49 Suppl 1, (2009).
Zijlema, W. L. et al. The relationship between natural outdoor environments and cognitive functioning and its mediators. Environ. Res. 155, 268–275 (2017).
de Keijzer, C. et al. Residential surrounding greenness and cognitive decline: A 10-Year Follow-up of the whitehall II cohort. Environ. Health Perspect. 126, (2018).
Hystad, P., Payette, Y., Noisel, N. & Boileau, C. Green space associations with mental health and cognitive function: results from the quebec cartagene cohort. Environ. Epidemiol. 3 (2019).
Taylor, L. & Hochuli, D. F. Defining greenspace: multiple uses across multiple disciplines. Landsc. Urban Plan. 158, 25–38 (2017).
World Health Organization. Urban Green Space Interventions and Health: A review of impact and effectiveness. (2017).
Hartig, T., Mitchell, R., De Vries, S. & Frumkin, H. Nature and health. Annu. Rev. Public. Health. 35, 207–228 (2014).
Bratman, G. N., Hamilton, J. P. & Daily, G. C. The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1249, 118–136 (2012).
Markevych, I. et al. Exploring pathways linking greenspace to health: theoretical and methodological guidance. Environ. Res. 158, 301–317 (2017).
Mielke, M. M. et al. Consideration of sex and gender in alzheimer’s disease and related disorders from a global perspective. Alzheimers Dement. 18, 2707–2724 (2022).
Mielke, M. M., Vemuri, P. & Rocca, W. A. Clinical epidemiology of alzheimer’s disease: assessing sex and gender differences. Clin. Epidemiol. 6, 37 (2014).
Mosconi, L. et al. Increased alzheimer’s risk during the menopause transition: A 3-year longitudinal brain imaging study. PLoS One 13 (2018).
Mosconi, L. et al. Sex differences in alzheimer risk. Neurology 89, 1382–1390 (2017).
Than, S. et al. Interactions between age, sex, menopause, and brain structure at midlife: A UK biobank study. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 106, 410–420 (2021).
Nichols, E. et al. Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet Public. Health. 7, e105 (2022).
Cho, J. et al. Long-term ambient air pollution exposures and brain imaging markers in korean adults: the environmental pollution-induced neurological effects (epinef) study. Environ. Health Perspect. 128, 1–11 (2020).
Kim, H. et al. Gender difference in the effects of outdoor air pollution on cognitive function among elderly in korea. Front. Public. Health. 7, 474333 (2019).
Bale, T. L. & Epperson, C. N. Sex differences and stress across the lifespan. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1413 (2015).
Seeman, T. E., McEwen, B. S., Singer, B. H., Albert, M. S. & Rowe, J. W. Increase in urinary cortisol excretion and memory declines: macarthur studies of successful aging. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 82, 2458–2465 (1997).
Roe, J. J. et al. Green space and stress: evidence from cortisol measures in deprived urban communities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health. 10, 4086 (2013).
Astell-Burt, T., Mitchell, R. & Hartig, T. The association between green space and mental health varies across the lifecourse. A longitudinal study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health (1978). 68, 578–583 (2014).
Jiang, B., Chang, C. Y. & Sullivan, W. C. A dose of nature: tree cover, stress reduction, and gender differences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 132, 26–36 (2014).
Yan, Y., Dominguez, S., Fisher, D. W. & Dong, H. Sex differences in chronic stress responses and alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Stress. 8, 120 (2018).
Otte, C. et al. A meta-analysis of cortisol response to challenge in human aging: importance of gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 80–91 (2005).
Yao, W., Zhang, X. & Gong, Q. The effect of exposure to the natural environment on stress reduction: A meta-analysis. Urban Urban Green. 57, 126932 (2021).
Ideno, Y. et al. Blood pressure-lowering effect of Shinrin-yoku (Forest bathing): a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Complement. Altern. Med 17, (2017).
Egorov, A. I. et al. Vegetated land cover near residence is associated with reduced allostatic load and improved biomarkers of neuroendocrine, metabolic and immune functions. Environ. Res. 158, 508–521 (2017).
Besser, L. M. et al. Associations between neighborhood greenspace and brain imaging measures in non-demented older adults: the cardiovascular health study. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 56, 1575 (2021).
Sudlow, C. et al. UK biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. PLoS Med. 12, 1001779 (2015).
Morton, D. et al. Countryside Survey: Final Report for LCM2007 – the New UK Land Cover Map. NERC/Centre for Ecology & Hydrology. Countryside Survey Technical Report No 11/07 (CEH Project Number: C03259). vol. 11 (2011).
Labib, S. M., Lindley, S. & Huck, J. J. Spatial dimensions of the influence of urban green-blue spaces on human health: A systematic review. Environ. Res. 180, 108869 (2020).
Kwan, M. P. The uncertain geographic context problem. Ann. Assoc. Am. Geogr. 102, 958–968 (2012).
Miller, K. L. et al. Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1523–1536 (2016).
Alfaro-Almagro, F. et al. Image processing and quality control for the first 10,000 brain imaging datasets from UK biobank. Neuroimage 166, 400–424 (2018).
Smith, S., Alfaro-Almagro, F. & Miller, K. UK Biobank Brain Imaging Documentation. (2022).
UK Biobank. Category 116: cognitive function online. UK Biobank Data Showcase https:https://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/label.cgi?id=116
Hoyer, W. J., Stawski, R. S., Wasylyshyn, C. & Verhaeghen, P. Adult age and digit symbol substitution performance: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging. 19, 211–214 (2004).
Yuspeh, R., Drane, D., Huthwaite, J. & Klingler, L. Derived trail making test indices: normative observations. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 15, 687–688 (2000).
Gualtieri, C. T. & Johnson, L. G. Neurocognitive testing supports a broader concept of mild cognitive impairment. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 20, 359–366 (2005).
Marshall, G. A. et al. Executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in MCI and AD. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 300 (2011).
Clemmensen, F. K. et al. The role of physical and cognitive function in performance of activities of daily living in patients with mild-to-moderate alzheimer’s disease – a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr. 20, 1–9 (2020).
Stevenson, M. P., Schilhab, T. & Bentsen, P. Attention Restoration Theory II: a systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health - Part B: Crit. Rev. 21, 227–268 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1080/10937404.2018.1505571
UK Biobank. UK Biobank Trail Mak. Test. 1–4 (2015).
UK Biobank. UK Biobank Symbol Digit. Substit. 1–3 (2022).
Livingston, G. et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet 390, 2673–2734 (2017).
Lai, K. Y., Kumari, S., Webster, C., Gallacher, J. E. J. & Sarkar, C. Neighbourhood residential density, urbanicity and incident dementia and alzheimer’s disease: A 12-year prospective cohort study from the UK biobank. Environ Res 226, (2023).
Zhang, Y. et al. Identifying modifiable factors and their joint effect on dementia risk in the UK Biobank. Nat. Hum. Behav. 7, 1185–1195 (2023).
IPAQ-Group. Guidelines for Data Processing and Analysis of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) – Short and Long Forms. (2005).
UK Biobank. Resource 6810: Sources and Cleaning of IMD Scores. (2024). https://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/ukb/refer.cgi?id=6810
Department for Communities and Local Government. Generalised Land Use Database Statistics for England 2005 (Enhanced Basemap). (2007).
Jarvis, I., Koehoorn, M., Gergel, S. E. & van den Bosch, M. Different types of urban natural environments influence various dimensions of self-reported health. Environ. Res. 186, 109614 (2020).
Dzhambov, A. M., Browning, M. H. E. M., Markevych, I., Hartig, T. & Lercher, P. Analytical approaches to testing pathways linking greenspace to health: A scoping review of the empirical literature. Environ. Res. 186, 109613 (2020).
Kruize, H. et al. Exploring mechanisms underlying the relationship between the natural outdoor environment and health and well-being – Results from the PHENOTYPE project. Environ Int 134, (2020).
Borland, E. et al. Clinically relevant changes for cognitive outcomes in preclinical and prodromal cognitive stages: implications for clinical alzheimer trials. Neurology 99, e1142 (2022).
Frumkin, H. et al. Nature contact and human health: A research agenda. Environ. Health Perspect. 125, 075001 (2017).
Van Den Annerstedt, M. et al. Development of an urban green space indicator and the public health rationale. Scand. J. Public. Health. 44, 159–167 (2016).
Smith, G. et al. Characterisation of the natural environment: quantitative indicators across europe. Int. J. Health Geogr. 16, 16 (2017).
Barha, C. K. et al. Sex-specific relationship between long-term maintenance of physical activity and cognition in the health ABC study: potential role of hippocampal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume. Journals Gerontol. - Ser. Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 75, 764–770 (2020).
Brown, B. M. et al. Non-Modifiable factors as moderators of the relationship between physical activity and brain volume: A Cross-Sectional UK biobank study. J. Alzheimer’s Disease. 88, 1091–1101 (2022).
Varma, V. R., Chuang, Y. F., Harris, G. C., Tan, E. J. & Carlson, M. C. Low-intensity daily walking activity is associated with hippocampal volume in older adults. Hippocampus 25, 605–615 (2015).
Barha, C. K., Davis, J. C., Falck, R. S., Nagamatsu, L. S. & Liu-Ambrose, T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 46, 71–85 (2017).
Brinton, R. D., Yao, J., Yin, F., Mack, W. J. & Cadenas, E. Perimenopause as a neurological transition state. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 11, 393–405 (2015).
Thiese, M. S. Observational and interventional study design types; an overview. Biochem. Med. (Zagreb). 24, 199–210 (2014).
Streiner, D. L. Best (but oft-forgotten) practices: the multiple problems of multiplicity-whether and how to correct for many statistical tests. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 102, 721–728 (2015).
Noseworthy, M. et al. The effects of outdoor versus indoor exercise on psychological health, physical health, and physical activity behaviour: A systematic review of longitudinal trials. Int J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 20, (2023).
Acknowledgements
We conducted our analyses on data from the UK Biobank (Application 69022). We are grateful to the UK Biobank study participants and research team for their time and effort in generating this database.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conception: M.N., R.S.F., T.L.A. Data acquisition: L.A.G. Data analysis: R.S.F., M.N. Interpretation of results: M.N., R.S.F., T.L.A, L.A.G., T.C.H. Manuscript writing: M.N., R.S.F., T.L.A. Manuscript revisions: M.N., R.S.F., T.L.A, L.A.G., T.C.H.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Noseworthy, M., Falck, R.S., Galea, L.A.M. et al. Investigating biological sex as a moderator of the association of nature exposure with brain health: a cross-sectional UK biobank analysis. Sci Rep 15, 21063 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-05047-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-05047-4