Abstract
Goal-directed behavior requires resolving both consciously and subconsciously induced response conflicts. Neuronal gain control, which enhances processing efficacy, is crucial for conflict resolution and can be increased through pharmacological or brain stimulation interventions, though it faces inherent physical limits. This study examined the effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (atDCS) and methylphenidate (MPH) on conflict processing. Healthy adults (nā=ā105) performed a flanker task, with electroencephalography (EEG) used to assess alpha and theta band activity (ABA, TBA). Results showed that combining atDCS with MPH enhanced cognitive control and reduced response conflicts more effectively than atDCS alone, particularly when both conflict types co-occurred. Both atDCS and atDCS + MPH exhibited similar task-induced ABA and TBA modulations in the (pre)supplementary motor area, indicating heightened gain control. Overlapping neuroanatomical effects in mid-superior frontal areas suggest that atDCS and MPH share a common neuronal mechanism of gain control, especially in high-conflict/-demand situations.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Resolving response conflicts is influenced by the brainās ability to effectively distinguish relevant input from intrinsic noise (including irrelevant stimulus features). This key component of goal-directed behavior and cognitive control particularly relies on the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), which indicates the processing systemās sensitivity to important inputs versus neural noise1,2. Fine-tuning the SNR, namely sharpening the neuronal input/output function (the probability that varying strengths of presynaptic input will trigger a postsynaptic response), describes neuronal gain control mechanisms1,3. Enhanced neural gain control is linked to improved response control and overall cognitive, motor, and sensory performance4,5,6. Here, we investigated the effect of modulating gain control onto cognitive control processes, specifically the selection of appropriate actions by prioritizing relevant information over consciously and subliminally perceived cued distractions.
Research indicates that gain control mechanism can be modulated by various pharmacological and non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques1,7,8,9, both of which may exhibit ceiling effects as there is a limit to how much gain control can be increased. Combining both approaches in a proof-of-concept study will provide crucial insights into their interactive effects. In the current study, we therefore combined a NIBS method and a psychostimulant, of which both have been shown to modulate the efficacy of gain control mechanisms. Specifically, we used transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), which through a low electric current applied to the scalp induces focal changes in cortical excitability (i.e., increase or decrease dependent on electrode polarity), which last about one hour10,11. Cortical excitability adjusts neuronal gain control9,12, i.e., anodal tDCS (atDCS) induced amplification of spontaneous neuronal firing, and cortical excitability has been associated with modulation of gain control efficacy12,13. We applied atDCS to the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) a region critical for response conflict resolution, and action inhibition14,15,16. This region was targeted, as it was shown to reflect response conflict effects specific to the experimental task at hand6. Furthermore, the catecholaminergic system underlies effective neural information processing, in that appropriate levels of dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine (NE) are central to conflict processing and resolution17,18,19. Thus, we used methylphenidate (MPH), a psychostimulant that selectively blocks DA/NE reuptake, consequently increasing postsynaptic DA and NE levels20,21,22. Previously, MPH-induced modulations of cognitive control have been attributed to neuronal gain mechanisms and linked to fronto-striatal loops6,23. Importantly, the rIFG has been highlighted as a key site for reflecting the modulatory effects of MPH on cognitive control6, making it, together with its well-established role in cognitive control, a suitable stimulation target.
In the current literature, there is a notable lack of research combining tDCS with pharmacological interventions in the context of cognitive control. Few studies have explored how these two modulations might interact to influence neural plasticity and reorganization24,25,26,27,28,29, underlining the need for further investigations in this area. While both tDCS and MPH have been studied individually, their combined effects on behavioral accuracy and brain oscillations, particularly during cognitive tasks inducing conscious and subconscious conflicts, have not been investigated. However, both consciously and subconsciously processed information affect controlled behavior30,31,32,33. To investigate such response conflicts in the current study, we used a combination of a spatial flanker task with preceding subliminal primes6,34,35 (full details in the methods section). Gain control facilitates task-relevant processing and modulations via atDCS appear to facilitate the resolution of stimulus-response conflicts36. In contrast, MPH-induced increases in gain control were shown to benefit the resolution of subconsciously and consciously induced conflicts differentially6.
As a measure of neuronal mechanisms underlying task performance modulations, we focused on both theta and alpha band activity (TBA and ABA). TBA coordinates and integrates neuronal activity in complex networks, reflecting conflict monitoring and response conflicts37,38,39. Whereas, ABA has been implicated in interceding suppression of task-irrelevant information by regulating access to behaviorally relevant information40,41. Both ABA and TBA are associated with neuronal gain control mechanisms by facilitating selective noise inhibition and enhancing perceptual and response selection accuracies42,43,44,45. This dual focus on cognitive and neuronal gain control relevant TBA and ABA will likely provide a comprehensive understanding of the neuronal dynamics underlying gain control modulations via atDCS and MPH.
In this study, we set out to investigate the effects of gain control modulations via atDCS and atDCS + MPH on consciously and subconsciously triggered response conflicts. Presuming that atDCS over the rIFG and MPH administration enhances the SNR in cortical areas relevant to response conflict processing, we hypothesize that (1) consciously and subconsciously induced response conflicts will engage distinct functional neuroanatomical networks (2) behavioral effects will be accompanied by comparable ABA and TBA modulations (reflecting gain control modulations) in mid-superior and inferior-frontal areas6,29,39 (3) the combined effects of atDCS and MPH will surpass the individual effects of each technique29 (4) combining atDCS with MPH will result in additive effects (rather than doubling) of both stimulation methods29, given that the extent to which gain control may be increased underlies a physical limit. The goal was to investigate neuronal gain control as a common neuronal mechanism underlying atDCS and MPH effects. Our neurophysiological analyses were aimed at investigating the neural mechanisms driving behaviorally relevant effects, providing insight into the cortical dynamics that underpin cognitive and behavioral outcomes obtained in this study. Accordingly, functional-neuroanatomical networks underlying consciously and subconsciously induced response conflicts modulated with atDCS and MPH (separately and combined) were examined.
Results
Included participants
After initial data inspection, nā=ā12 participants were excluded from all subsequent analyses (behavioral and EEG data). Participants were excluded based on the following criteria: (1) incomplete data due to technical issues (e.g., equipment malfunctions), (2) failure to comply with task instructions (e.g., responding inconsistently or inaccurately beyond acceptable thresholds), and (3) inability to complete study both study appointments (e.g., withdrawal or scheduling conflicts). Additional details are provided in the supplement (section āAdditional details on exclusion decisions and the composition of the final sampleā).
This resulted in a final sample of nā=ā93 (mean age 25.11ā±ā0.315 SEM; 44 females) participants included in all subsequent analyses. Specifically, there were nā=ā45 (mean age 25.20ā±ā0.428 SEM; 21 females) participants in the placebo group and nā=ā48 (mean age 25.02ā±ā0.465 SEM; 23 females) in the MPH group.
Behavioral data
Task effects
The typical task effects of prime compatibility and flanker congruency (i.e., higher accuracies in congruent/compatible than in incongruent/incompatible trials) were replicated6,34. There was an interaction of prime compatibility x flanker congruency (F(1,89)ā=ā57.930; pā<ā.001; Ī·2pā=ā0.394). Post hoc t-tests revealed significant differences for all possible contrasts (all pāā¤ā.001). In line with previous results, the prime compatibility effect (i.e. compatible ā incompatible) was larger in incongruent flankers (10.263%ā±ā1.167) than in congruent flankers (6.722%ā±ā0.771) (t(1,75)ā=āā7.390; pā<ā001). Additionally, the flanker congruency effect (i.e. congruent ā incongruent) was larger in incompatible primes (5.264%ā±ā0.553) than in compatible primes (1.754%ā±ā10.234) [t(1,92)ā=āā7.346; pā<ā0.001 (one-sided p)].
Pharmacological group and tDCS stimulation effects
The repeated measures ANOVA for accuracy revealed an interaction of tDCS x order of stimulation x prime compatibility x flanker congruency x pharmacological group (F(1,89)ā=ā4.132; pā=ā.045; Ī·2pā=ā0.044). Next, the interaction effect was separately examined with post-hoc ANOVAs for real vs. sham tDCS appointments.
The post-hoc ANOVA for the active tDCS, but not for the sham tDCS appointment, revealed an interaction of order of stimulation x prime compatibility x flanker congruency x pharmacological group (F(1,89)ā=ā9.535; pā=ā.003; Ī·2pā=ā0.097). To resolve this interaction, independent samples t-tests were conducted to compare the magnitudes of flanker and prime effects between the pharmacological groups when they received active tDCS. Of note, using those difference effects focusses the post-hoc analyses to the most relevant behavioral performance measures while simultaneously shortening subsequent analyses due to employing contrasts. These comparisons were made separately for participants who received active tDCS during their first appointment and those who received active tDCS during their second appointment. The results show that for participants who received active tDCS on the second appointment, the flanker congruency effect (i.e., congruent ā incongruent trials) in case of incompatible primes was larger in the placebo group (7.737%ā±ā1.734) than in the MPH group (3.728%ā±ā1.001) [t(43)ā=ā1.979; pā=ā.027 (one-sided p)]. Moreover, the prime compatibility effect (i.e. compatible ā incompatible trials) in case of incongruent flankers was also larger in the placebo group (9.165%ā±ā2.052) than in the MPH group (4.741%ā±ā1.372) [t(43)ā=ā1.775; pā=ā0.041 (one-sided p)]. For participants who received active tDCS during their first appointment, no statistically significant differences in the contrasts of interest (flanker and prime effects) were observed between the pharmacological groups [all p > 0.051 (one-sided p)].
Taken together, significant differences between the pharmacological groups emerged exclusively when active tDCS was applied during the second appointment. This effect was observed only under high response conflict conditions ā specifically, in scenarios where the respective other conflict was also present (i.e. when the flanker effect involved an incompatible prime, and the prime effect involved an incongruent flanker). In other words, when active tDCS was applied during the second appointment, the MPH group showed smaller flanker and prime effects compared to the placebo group. FigureĀ 1 illustrates the most relevant behavioral accuracy results. The significance and magnitude of the overall learning/practice effects are reported in the supplementary material (section āAdditional details on behavioral data: Learning/practice effectsā).
Shown are the prime effects (in percent) for when the flankers were congruent (top row, left plot) and incongruent (top row, right plot), as well as the flanker effect (in percent) for when the primes were congruent (bottom row, left plot) and incongruent (bottom row, right plot). Green boxplots illustrate data for the placebo group and yellow boxplots for the MPH group. The āxā and the horizontal line inside the boxplots indicate the mean and median, respectively. Asterisks indicate significant differences at pā<ā0.05 and error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals. The raincloud plots illustrate the data distribution (above boxplot) and data points (below boxplot) of nā=ā93 participants.
Response times (RTs)
The repeated measures ANOVA for RTs showed a main effect of the pharmacological group (F(1,89)ā=ā4.914; pā=ā0.029; Ī·2pā=ā0.052), indicating overall faster response times of the MPH group (395.145āmsā±ā4.852) as compared to the placebo group (410.583āmsā±ā4.995). Importantly, there was an interaction of flanker congruency x pharmacological group (F(1,89)ā=ā4.581; pā=ā0.035; Ī·2pā=ā0.049). Post hoc independent samples t-tests show that overall the flanker effect (congruent minus incongruent trials) was larger (more negative) in the placebo group (ā17.372āmsā±ā1.306) than in the MPH group (ā13.709āmsā±ā0.933) [t(80.675)ā=āā2.282; pā=ā0.013 (one-sided p)]. The relevant results are illustrated in Fig.Ā 2.
Depicted is the flanker effect (congruent minus incongruent trials) for both pharmacological groups (MPH and placebo). Data is illustrated in green for the placebo group and in yellow for the MPH group. Asterisks indicate significant differences at pā<ā0.05 and error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals. The āxā and the horizontal line inside the boxplots indicate the mean and median, respectively. The raincloud plots illustrate the data distribution (above boxplot) and data points (below boxplot) of nā=ā93 participants.
Further main and interaction effects not involving the pharmacological group factor included the following: There was a main effect of the order of stimulation (F(1,89)ā=ā4.557; pā=ā.036; Ī·2pā=ā0.049), showing overall faster response times for those who received active tDCS on the second appointment (-395.430āmsā±ā4.995) as compared to those on the first appointment (-410.297āmsā±ā4.852). Moreover, there was an interaction of tDCS x order of stimulation x flanker congruency (F(1,89)ā=ā5.020; pā=ā0.028; Ī·2pā=ā0.053). The interaction was examined with an independent samples t-test comparing the flanker effect after real and sham tDCS between those who received active tDCS on the first vs. those who received active tDCS on the second appointment. The results indicate smaller flanker congruency effects for those who received active stimulation on the second appointment (-9.978āmsā±ā0.959) as compared to those who received active tDCS on the first appointment (-12.948āmsā±ā0.869) [t(91)ā=ā-2.2300; pā=ā.012 (one-sided p)]. There was no difference in the magnitude of the flanker effects on the sham tDCS appointment.
Neurophysiology
The behavioral differences between both pharmacological groups were only significant when participants received active tDCS stimulation on the second appointment. Based on this, subsequent neurophysiological analyses exclusively investigated differences between the MPH and placebo group when active tDCS was administered on the second appointment.
In the interval of 0ā1000āms relative to target onset, separate cluster-based permutation tests (CBPTs) were conducted for alpha and theta band activity (ABA and TBA respectively), and MPH and placebo groups. The results revealed significant negative differences in both ABA and TBA between compatible and incompatible primes trials when the flankers were incongruent, as well as between congruent and incongruent flanker trials when the prime was incompatible. Negative power differences indicate that alpha and theta band power were higher in incompatible prime trials compared to compatible ones, and in incongruent flanker trials compared to congruent ones, potentially highlighting greater difficulty, interference, or need for cognitive control effort in these conditions. Further details are provided in the supplement (section āAdditional neurophysiological results for participants who received sham tDCS on the first appointmentā). The significant electrode locations of the cluster-based permutation tests are visualized in Fig.Ā 3, accompanied by the time-frequency representation (TFRs) of the power differences in the aforementioned contrasts.
a Alpha band time-frequency (TF) analysis and cluster-based permutation testing (CBPT) results. The TF plots display average power differences (0-1000āms post-target arrow onset) for the prime effect (compatible MINUS incompatible trials) when flankers were incongruent, as well as for the flanker effect (congruent MINUS incongruent trials) when the prime was incompatible. Topographic plots highlight significant electrodes identified in the CBPTs, marked with red āxās to indicate negative differences. These plots show average power in the significant timeframe (100 to ~600āms) separately for both MPH and placebo groups, with conflict effects noted in corresponding titles. b Theta band time-frequency (TF) analysis and cluster-based permutation testing (CBPT) results. The TF plots display average power differences (0ā1000āms post-target arrow onset) for the prime effect (compatible MINUS incompatible trials) when flankers were incongruent, as well as for the flanker effect (congruent MINUS incongruent trials) when the prime was incompatible. Topographic plots highlight significant electrodes identified in the CBPTs, marked with red āxās to indicate negative differences. These plots show average power in the significant timeframe (250 to ~750āms) separately for both MPH and placebo groups, with conflict effects noted in corresponding titles.
On the source level, the DBSCAN algorithm on the Prime and Flanker effect ratio identified several negative clusters in both alpha and theta bands for both MPH and placebo groups. Negative clusters (i.e., negative power ratios) indicate that ABA and TBA were higher in incompatible prime trials compared to compatible ones, and in incongruent flanker trials compared to congruent ones. The negative ratio reflects the directionality of this difference and suggests that the incompatible prime/incongruent flanker is influencing the processing system more prominently than the compatible prime/congruent flanker under these specific circumstances. An overview of all resulting clusters is provided in Fig.Ā 4.
Discussion
This study investigated alterations in the functional neuroanatomical sources associated with subconsciously and consciously evoked response conflicts when gain control was experimentally increased. Assuming that atDCS and MPH share a common neuronal mechanism (i.e., gain control), we investigated the modulatory effects of atDCS, MPH, and atDCS + MPH via their effects on interference control and corresponding alpha and theta band oscillations (reflecting gain control). Compared to those who only received tDCS, participants who received both stimulations (i.e., atDCS + MPH) on the second appointment showed lower interference conflict effects, especially when subliminal and conscious conflicts were combined. Using an EEG beamforming approach, we identified the functional neuroanatomical correlates of atDCS-induced, and atDCS + MPH-induced modulations in ABA and TBA, respectively. Across both frequency bands, our results show overlapping neuroanatomical sources when atDCS and atDCS + MPH were applied. Consistent with the assumption that tDCS and MPH share a common underlying neural mechanism [most likely gain control, 4], we show that tDCS separately and combined with MPH modulates TBA in cognitive control-relevant mid-superior-frontal areas. Importantly, we extend these findings by showing similar modulatory effects in the ABA, highlighting the widespread influence of these interventions on cognitive control processes.
The behavioral data indicate clear differences between the pharmacological groups on the active atDCS appointment but not on the sham tDCS appointment. Specifically, when participants received atDCS on their second appointment (i.e., in the presence of learning/practice effects), both the flanker congruency effect and prime compatibility effect were significantly larger in the placebo group compared to the MPH group under high interference conflict conditions (i.e., in the presence of the respective other conflict, i.e., incompatible prime or incongruent flanker trials). While we did not have an a priori hypothesis expecting such learning/practice or order effects, this observation aligns with some of our previous MPH stimulation studies that demonstrated cognitive control-enhancing MPH effects46,47, but also hampered behavioral adaptation48, and sometimes even a reduction of MPH effects on simple response inhibition and perception-action integration49,50,51,52 in case of repeated measurements (i.e., after the task had already been performed once without MPH before). Given that the effectiveness of MPH modulation does generally not only depend on the dose, but also on task difficulty and prior learning/practice, it would have been unscientific to have postulated a clear direction of effects a priori. This does however not mean that the obtained effect cannot be interpreted: Given that the participants showed a small, but significant overall improvement in behavioral performance from the first to the second appointment (by 0.9%ā±ā0.3 and 11āmsā±ā25; see supplement for details), it can be assumed that their improvement in accuracy and speed is likely due to learning/practice effects and/or response automatization. Given that we found no significant MPH differences in prime and flanker effects on the first appointment, it is unlikely that MPH produced out data pattern by hampering the initial learning of the task to a relevant degree. Instead, it seems that MPH only made a difference, once a relevant degree of learning / training had taken place (and tDCS stimulation was present, but this will be discussed in further details below). Given that the MPH effects were specific to high-conflict/demand situations, it seems plausible to assume that the combined effects of MPH and tDCS are most pronounced in a situation where easy responses have been extensively trained / automatized, which might potentially make it harder to switch into a cognitive control mode when needed (i.e., when a high-conflict trial pops up). Neurobiologically, the modulatory impact of elevated catecholamine levels on gain control processes may have been further strengthened by increasing cortical excitability via atDCS13,26,29, leading to optimal performance. No significant differences were found when atDCS was applied on the first appointment. Our results corroborate previous findings on the effects of MPH [19ā22 etc.] and underscore the significant impact of the intricate interplay between atDCS, catecholaminergic stimulation, and the order of tDCS administration onto behavioral outcomes, particularly under challenging conditions (i.e. when the mental effort needed to perform well is high). In the following, we will separately discuss the functional neuroanatomical sources reflecting the prime and flanker effects when gain was modulated (via atDCS) on the second appointment.
Prime effect
In both frequency bands and pharmacological groups, the prime effect (in incongruent flanker trials) was associated with negative clusters in the (pre)SMA. Agreeing with previous results, TBA in the SMA is elevated when information conflicts and the need for cognitive control is high29,37,53. Moreover, TBA in mid-superior frontal regions is essential for cognitive control processes during response control, selection, and execution29,37,54,55. This is likely because TBA in these regions is particularly well-suited for integrating sensory information with motor commands37.
Notably, ABA is crucial for TBA-associated cognitive control processes and information-gating56, and their interplay is central for goal-directed action control. Increased ABA in the SMA likely reflects active engagement in attentional modulation/weighting and cognitive control56,57,58. The combined atDCS and MPH-induced amplification of the signal-to-noise ratio was sufficient to better suppress the processing of distracting information59, leading to improved/optimal interference and cognitive control. Mid-superior frontal ABA thus likely reflects the suppression of task-irrelevant information40,41. Our results show that after modulation of gain control via atDCS, but not when atDCS and MPH stimulation were combined, ABA additionally increases in an occipito-temporal cluster when the response conflict is high. The occipito-temporal areas have been linked to guiding (visual) attentional selection processes60,61. This pattern suggests that participants in the placebo group who experienced higher behavioral interference demonstrated enhanced visual processing of the prime stimulus and subsequently needed higher suppression of distracting information within occipito-temporal regions implemented by ABA. This likely reflects a gain-induced compensatory mechanism to manage increased cognitive load and mental effort to perform well. Our current results support the role of ABA in the temporal lobe as a critical element in moderating interference through selective (visual) attention and filtering out irrelevant or distracting stimuli40,59, especially when the cognitive load is high.
Flanker effect
Similar to the prime effect, the flanker effect (in incompatible prime trials) was associated with negative clusters spanning mid-superior-frontal areas62, across both frequency bands and pharmacological groups. When atDCS and MPH were combined, an additional cluster of ABA was detected in the left inferior frontal gyrus (left IFG). This aligns with a recent study29, suggesting that the additive effect of both modulations on neuronal gain control likely pushed the mid-superior frontal regions beyond their optimal functioning level, leading to the compensatory recruitment of higher processing capacities (i.e., the left IFG). Activity in the left IFG highlights its essential function in detecting and resolving conflicts at both the response and motor response levels63,64 and response control65.
In contrast to the prime effect, the flanker effect and associated interference conflict appear to engage a more distributed functional neuroanatomical network, particularly in the theta band. When modulating gain control with atDCS the flanker effect was additionally associated with TBA clusters spanning the right anterior and medial-temporal lobes. Medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures support memory functions and have been implicated in interference control66,67. Increased TBA in the most challenging trials likely indicates the involvement of the MTL in proactive interference control and successfully resolving the possibly detrimental effects of subliminal distractors66. Furthermore, the flanker effect following both atDCS and atDCS + MPH was associated with TBA in the right and left temporal pole (TP) respectively. When interference resolution is required, the right TP (i.e. a high-level visual cortical area) has been associated with object concept categorization based on their visual features68,69. Generally, the TP has been ascribed a role in receiving and integrating sensory modalities including visual inputs and participation in the ventral visual stream70. Ultimately, our results support the notion that in effortful situations the interaction of (mid-superior) frontal areas with MTL structures becomes more important71.
Shared conflict effects
Overall, in high-conflict scenarios, both response conflicts were reflected in gain-induced ABA and TBA modulations in similar, yet critically distinct, neuroanatomical regions. Consistent with our hypothesis, subconsciously and consciously evoked conflicts (in the presence of the respective other conflict) were associated with activity modulations in the (pre)SMA in both frequency bands, likely reflecting gain control29,72. Mid-superior frontal cognitive control-related brain activity29,37,54), likely reflects goal shielding processes essential for stabilizing goal representations amidst distracting input73,74. Here, the presence of both conflicts likely potentiated the conflict effects given a certain threshold was surpassed34. When modulating gain control with atDCS, ABA and TBA were increased in conflicting, as compared to non-conflicting trials37,54,62, irrespective of catecholaminergic stimulation. This indicates enhanced gain control, where neuronal gain is adjusted to prioritize relevant information and effortfully suppress distractions. These gain control increases were reflected in negative clusters for both response conflicts, pharmacological groups, and frequency bands.
Limitations
The observed results support the assumption that tDCS not solely affects the targeted cortical region, but also indirectly influences remote areas through interconnected cortical networks75,76. Given the evident cortical network-level impact of tDCS, this study further highlights the importance of interpreting tDCS effects within the context of broader neural interactions. A potential limitation of our study might have been that we did not assess whether participants could discern whether they were in the placebo or MPH group, nor which session involved active versus sham tDCS. It will further remain somewhat unclear whether the observed order effects are due to task learning, automatization, or both.
Conclusions
In summary, we dissociated the modulatory effect of gain control stimulated with atDCS and atDCS + MPH on subliminally and consciously induced response conflicts. Combining catecholaminergic enhancement with atDCS resulted in greater gain-induced cognitive control and lower response conflicts for both subconscious and consciously induced conflicts compared to atDCS alone, especially when both conflict types were present. Following active atDCS (irrespective of MPH stimulation), both response conflicts were associated with larger conflict-induced ABA and TBA modulations (reflecting gain control) spanning mid-superior-frontal areas. Activity modulations in the SMA likely indicate the need to correct cued response tendencies by generating internal (correct) responses that overwrite stimulus-triggered movements. Gain control modulation (via atDCS and atDCS +MPH), induced similar neuronal gain task requirement-specific allocations of ABA and TBA. The overlapping modulatory pattern in the same cortical network subserving response conflict resolution substantiates the theoretical assumption that atDCS and MPH exert their effects through a shared underlying mechanism, specifically neuronal gain control29,72.
Methods
Participants
An a priori estimation of the required sample size using G*Power software77,78 assuming a within-inbetween interaction in a 4āĆā8 ANOVA design (for details on the factors, please refer to the section on behavioral statistics) with αā=ā5%, 1-βā=ā95%, a correlation of 5, the assumption of non-sphericity and an effect size of 2% (i.e., \({\eta }_{p}^{2}=.02\), or fā=ā0.14285) yielded a required total sample size of nā=ā100. Of note, this sample size is comparable to another publication of ours investigating the combined effects of tDCS and (a higher dose of) MPH on response inhibition, where the smallest significant pharmacological effect explained around 4% of variance (i.e., twice as much as assumed for the a priori sample size estimation)29. Nā=ā105 healthy adults (mean age 25.06ā±ā0.288 SEM; 54 females) voluntarily participated in this study. All participants were 20-31 years old, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, had no current/reported history of psychiatric, neurologic, or developmental disorders, and reported no CNS-affecting medication use. Participants provided written informed consent and received course credits or financial compensation. The study was approved by the ethics committee of TU Dresden (EK 420092015) and conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. All ethical regulations relevant to human research participants were followed.
Experimental design and methylphenidate administration
The study employed a mixed design with anodal tDCS (real vs. sham) and experimental conditions as within-subject factors, and pharmacological group (placebo vs. MPH) and order of stimulation (active tDCS on the first vs. second appointment) as between-subject factors. Pseudorandomization ensured equal numbers in each subgroup, defined by pharmacological group and order of stimulation. Group assignment was otherwise random and double-blind.
Participants attended two experimental sessions, spaced seven days apart. Depending on their randomly assigned pharmacological group, participants received either their respective MPH dose (0.50āmg per kg body weight) or a placebo (lactose) on both study appointments in a double-blind manner. Participants were instructed to abstain from caffeine and smoking for a minimum of two hours before testing. The experimental task on both appointments started roughly 120āmin after drug administration, as this is when MPH plasma levels peak (MPH plasma levels peak around 1ā3āh, with maximum drug concentration at about 2āh after oral administration79,80). A visual summary is provided in Fig.Ā 5.
Illustrated is a visual summary of all methods and analyses applied in the current study. The top part depicts the timeline of an experimental session, showing the sequence and duration of key interventions. The horizontal axis marks time in minutes. Each session began with administering either a dose of MPH or a placebo, depending on the participantsā subgroup. Before starting the experimental task and EEG recording, participants received active/real tDCS on one appointment, and sham tDCS on the other (for details see methods section). Behavioral and neurophysiological data collection started 120āmin post MPH/placebo administration and lasted approximately 15āmin for the task reported in this study. Of note, this was followed by another task (duration ~20āmin) that is not reported here. All details are provided in the methods section. The bottom part summarizes the analyses applied from the sensor to the source level. Abbreviations: tDCS (transcranial direct current stimulation), MPH (methylphenidate).
tDCS stimulation
Before the experiment and EEG recording, participants received 20āmin of either anodal tDCS or sham stimulation targeting the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG), as shown in Fig.Ā 5. Following previous studies29,81,82, the rIFG was identified as the midpoint between electrodes FC4 and F8, corresponding to the midpoint of the anode in our setup. The cathode (reference electrode) was positioned contralaterally between the neck and deltoid muscle. Anodal and sham stimulation were administered via two 5 Ć 5ācm² rubber electrodes (NeuroConn, Ilmenau, Germany) using a NeuroConn DC-Stimulator Plus device (NeuroConn, Ilmenaum Germany). Ten20 conductive paste (0.5āmm thick, customized casing4) ensured optimal conductivity and kept impedances below 5 kĪ©. The current was ramped up and down over 15āsec to minimize possible discomfort, with active stimulation lasting 1200ās (20āmin) at 2āmA. For the sham condition, a 2-mA current was applied for 15ās and then tapered off for 15ās to mimic the initial tingling of active tDCS. Both the tDCS protocols included identical waiting and preparation periods preceding the experiment. While the administration of MPH/placebo was double-blind, the experimenter could not be blinded to the tDCS manipulation, as they had to manually start the active or sham stimulation on the tDCS device. This detail was not disclosed to the participants to minimize potential biases. The electrical potential induced by the tDCS using the specified electrode configuration was calculated using the MATLAB toolbox āCOMETS2ā83 and is depicted in Fig.Ā 6.
The EP induced by anodal tDCS with the described electrode configuration was computed using the āCOMETS2ā toolbox83. The anode (5 Ć 5ācm) was positioned above the rIFG, centered between electrodes FC4 and F8 (red patch in the right image). The reference electrode was positioned contralaterally between the neck and deltoid muscle. The input current was 2āmA, with colors denoting simulated electrical potential. Abbreviations: tDCS (transcranial direct current stimulation), rIFG (right inferior frontal gyrus), mA (milliampere).
Experimental task
The task design and structure are described and depicted in Fig.Ā 7. The experimental task was adapted from Boy et al.35 and is identical to the paradigm utilized in previous studies6,34,47. This task allows for the examination of subconsciously and consciously evoked conflicts in response selection by combining the target stimulus with subliminal prime and flankers, respectively.
Illustrated is an exemplary trial configuration. Each trial began with a fixation cross displayed for 100āms. This was followed by a prime (central arrow) for 30āms and a mask (randomly allocated lines) for 30āms. The target (central arrow) and flankers were presented for 100āms, after which the screen turned black. Primes aligning with the target arrow were labeled compatible, while those in the opposite direction were incompatible. Similarly, flankers matching the target arrowsā direction were classified as congruent, and those pointing in opposite directions as incongruent.
Participants were seated approximately 60ācm from a 24-inch CRT monitor, which displayed stimuli against a black background. Manual responses were collected using the āCtrlā buttons on a standard QWERTZ keyboard. āPresentationā software (Version 18.3 by Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.) managed stimulus presentation, response recording, and EEG synchronization. Each trial began with a central white fixation cross for 100āms, followed by a subliminal prime (a white arrow pointing left or right) displayed centrally for 30āms. Immediately after the prime, a mask stimulus (randomly distributed white lines) was shown for 30āms. This resulted in a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 60āms between the onsets of the subliminal prime and the target arrow. Subsequently, the target (a central white arrow pointing either left or right) and two flankers (white arrows positioned above and below the target arrow) were presented simultaneously for 100āms. Participants were instructed to focus on the central target arrow and ignore the flankers. They were then asked to indicate the direction of the central target arrow by pressing the left āCtrlā button with their left index finger for a left-pointing target arrow, or the right āCtrlā button with their right index finger for a right-pointing target arrow. Each trial ended with the participantās first response or 2000 ms after target onset. If no response was recorded within this time frame, the trial was marked as a āmissā. The response-stimulus interval (RSI) between the participantās first response and the onset of the following trial varied randomly between 1000 and 1200āms. Trials were categorized based on the direction of both the prime and target arrows. When the prime and target arrows pointed in the same direction, the trial was labeled as ācompatibleā. Conversely, if they pointed in opposite directions, the trial was labeled āincompatibleā. Additionally, trials were classified as ācongruentā when the flanker and target arrow pointed in the same direction, and as āincongruentā when they pointed in opposite directions. This classification resulted in four conditions: compatible-congruent, incompatible-congruent, compatible-incongruent, and incompatible-incongruent.
Each participant completed 384 trials, equally distributed across four blocks (96 trials per block). Within each block, all possible combinations of prime compatibility, flanker congruency, and target direction were randomized and presented at equal frequencies. At the first appointment, participants completed a practice run of 16 trials to familiarize themselves with the task immediately after MPH/placebo administration (i.e., before the potential onset of full MPH drug effects). Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and precisely as possible. After each block (96 trials), participants could take a self-timed break (i.e., to rest their eyes), and resume via button press. The experiment took on average approximately 15āmin to complete.
EEG recording and analysis
EEG signals were recorded from 60 Ag/AgCl electrodes in equidistant positions, as in previously published publications54,84. The EEG preprocessing was performed using the Automagic toolbox85 and the EEGLAB toolbox86 in Matlab 2020a (The MathWorks Corp.):
The participantsā EEG signals were recorded from 60 Ag/AgCl electrodes in equidistant positions with a āQuickAmpā amplifier (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) and the āBrainVision Recorderā software (Version 2.2), as in previous publications from our research group54,84. For this, 60 Ag/AgCl electrodes in equidistant positions were used. The ground electrode was positioned at the coordinates Īøā=ā58, Ļā=ā78, and the reference at Fpz (Īøā=ā90, Ļā=ā90). EEG signals were initially recorded at a sampling rate of 500āHz, while electrode impedances were kept below 10 kĪ©. First, EEG data were down-sampled to 256āHz, and flat channels were removed (i.e., channels that showed activity below 5 µV for more than 5āsec). The remaining channels were then re-referenced to an average reference. Subsequently, the PREP preprocessing pipeline87 was applied, which removes line noise (for data recorded in Europe: 50āHz) using a multi-taper algorithm. After removing contaminations by noisy/bad channels (using high and minimum variance criterion), a robust common average reference was applied. EOG artifacts were removed using a subtraction method, i.e., EOG Regression88. Subsequently, the EEGLABs pop_eegfiltnew() pipeline was used to apply a high pass filter (cutoff frequency: 0.5āHz) and low pass filter (cutoff frequency: 40āHz); the filter order was estimated by default. Remaining artifactual source components in the data were detected by applying the Multiple Artifact Rejection Algorithm (MARA89), which automatizes independent component analyses (ICA). For the ICA, the data was temporarily high pass filtered with 1āHz, but this option was not applied to the final pre-processed data. In the final step, removed/missing channels were interpolated using a spherical method.
After visually inspecting each dataset, the preprocessed data was imported into the FieldTrip toolbox90 for further analysis. The EEG data was segmented in a target-locked manner into the four trial conditions: compatible prime - congruent flanker, incompatible prime - congruent flanker, compatible prime - incongruent flanker, and incompatible prime - incongruent flanker. Only correct trials were considered for the subsequent analyses.
To examine alpha-band (8ā12āHz) and theta-band (4ā7āHz) activity during these conditions, time-frequency (TF) decomposition was performed using Morlet wavelets (with a width parameter of 5). The average power in the alpha and theta frequency bands was computed for each electrode and time point. Only significant and most relevant behavioral effects were further investigated at the neurophysiological level. For this approach, cluster-based permutation tests (CBPTs) were conducted to identify clusters of electrodes with significant differences between congruent and incongruent flanker trials in case of incompatible primes, as well as to identify significant differences between compatible and incompatible prime trials in case of incongruent flankers. The CBPTs were performed separately for each frequency band within 0-1000āms following target arrow stimulus onset using 1000 Monte Carlo randomizations and a cluster-alpha level of p =ā.05 (two-tailed). Analyses were conducted separately for placebo and MPH groups for the active tDCS appointment.
Source estimation and beamforming analysis
The neuroanatomical sources that reflect the prime effect (i.e., compatible minus incompatible) with incongruent flankers and the flanker effect (i.e., the congruent minus incongruent) with incompatible primes in the alpha and theta band were reconstructed following a previously established beamforming procedure39. First, a Dynamic Imaging of Coherent Sources (DICS) beamformer91 was used to identify neuroanatomical sources of substantial differences between conditions of interest in alpha and theta band activity. The source localization results were projected onto an equally spaced 0.5ācm grid created from the forward model template provided by the FieldTrip toolbox, based on the standard MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) space. Power in the alpha and theta bands were extracted for the period following the presentation onset of the target stimulus. For both alpha and theta source power differences, corresponding contrasts were calculated and normalized on the total power of the two conditions as a ratio55:
Next, clusters of both frequency bands were identified by applying the density-based spatial clustering of applications with noise (DBSCAN92) algorithm as employed in MATLAB, comparable to previous studies39,93. The DICS beamforming results were restricted to negative ratios, indicating that alpha and theta power were higher in incompatibleā>ācompatible and incongruentā>ācongruent trials. The negative top 2% of the power distribution in the prime and flanker effect ratio within labeled regions on the automated anatomical labeling atlas94 were submitted to the DBSCAN, restricting the analysis to the voxels with the largest negative differences. An epsilon of 1.5 the edge length of each voxel was used to detect neighboring voxels. The resulting clusters (minimum five voxels) were visually inspected.
Statistics and reproducibility
We provide a power analysis justifying the recruited sample size (see section on participants for details). The behavioral data (i.e., response accuracy and reaction times) was analyzed using repeated measured ANOVAs using SPSS version 29.0.0.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, N.Y., USA). As within-subject factors, the ātDCSā (active tDCS vs. sham), āprime compatibilityā (compatible vs. incompatible), and āflanker congruencyā (congruent vs. incongruent) were used. The between-subject factors were āpharmacological groupā (placebo vs. MPH) and āorder of stimulationā (active tDCS on the first appointment vs. active tDCS on the second appointment). Significant main or interaction effects were examined with post hoc ANOVAs and post-hoc t-tests. Given that post hoc tests were only conducted when a justifying significant interaction had been obtained in the ANOVA, post hoc tests were not corrected for multiple testing. Only correct trials, with response times (RTs) between min. 100āms and max. 1000āms after the target arrow onset, were included in the subsequent analyses. When a participantās behavioral accuracy was below chance level (<50%) in two or more task conditions, or average response times were <100āms/>1000āms, that case was marked as an outlier, and no longer considered for all subsequent analyses of the behavioral and neurophysiological data. Descriptive data are given as mean and standard error of the mean (SEM). We used cluster-based permutation tests for the EEG data analysis (see section on EEG recording and analysis). The tDCS approach was based on prior research and a modeling of the tDCS current. EEG-beamforming analysis was only performed for contrasts for the cluster-based permutation testing revealed robust effects and according to previously published protocols.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in theĀ Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
All data from this study are publicly accessible at the following link: https://osf.io/wexgu/95. For any additional inquiries, please contact Anna Helin Koyun (AnnaHelin.Koyun@ukdd.de).
Code availability
The custom scripts used for EEG data processing are available for download at: https://osf.io/wexgu/.
References
Servan-Schreiber, D., Printz, H. & Cohen, J. D. A network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science 249, 892ā895 (1990).
Yousif, N. et al. Dopamine activation preserves visual motion perception despite noise interference of human V5/MT. J. Neurosci. 36, 9303ā9312 (2016).
Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U. & Sikstrƶm, S. Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 479ā486 (2001).
Adelhƶfer, N., Mückschel, M., Teufert, B., Ziemssen, T. & Beste, C. Anodal tDCS affects neuromodulatory effects of the norepinephrine system on superior frontal theta activity during response inhibition. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1291ā1300 (2019).
Adelhƶfer, N. et al. The system-neurophysiological basis for how methylphenidate modulates perceptualāattentional conflicts during auditory processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 5050ā5061 (2018).
Bensmann, W., Roessner, V., Stock, A. K. & Beste, C. Catecholaminergic modulation of conflict control depends on the source of conflicts. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 901ā909 (2018).
Molaee-Ardekani, B. et al. Effects of transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) on cortical activity: a computational modeling study. Brain Stimul. 6, 25ā39 (2013).
Hay, E. & Segev, I. Dendritic excitability and gain control in recurrent cortical microcircuits. Cereb. Cortex 25, 3561ā3571 (2015).
Li, S. C. & Rieckmann, A. Neuromodulation and aging: implications of aging neuronal gain control on cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 29, 148ā158 (2014).
Kuo, M. F. & Nitsche, M. A. Exploring prefrontal cortex functions in healthy humans by transcranial electrical stimulation. Neurosci. Bull. 31, 198ā206 (2015).
Nitsche, M. A. & Paulus, W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 527, 633ā639 (2000).
Lafon, B., Rahman, A., Bikson, M. & Parra, L. C. Direct current stimulation alters neuronal input/output function. Brain Stimul. 10, 36ā45 (2017).
Yamada, Y. & Sumiyoshi, T. Neurobiological mechanisms of transcranial direct current stimulation for psychiatric disorders; neurophysiological, chemical, and anatomical considerations. Front Hum. Neurosci. 15, 631838 (2021).
Kemmotsu, N., Villalobos, M. E., Gaffrey, M. S., Courchesne, E. & Müller, R. A. Activity and functional connectivity of inferior frontal cortex associated with response conflict. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 335ā342 (2005).
Sheu, Y. S. & Courtney, S. M. A neural mechanism of cognitive control for resolving conflict between abstract task rules. Cortex 85, 13ā24 (2016).
Hampshire, A., Chamberlain, S. R., Monti, M. M., Duncan, J. & Owen, A. M. The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. NeuroImage 50, 1313ā1319 (2010).
Botvinick, M. M. Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cogn., Affect., Behav. Neurosci. 7, 356ā366 (2007).
Willemssen, R., Falkenstein, M., Schwarz, M., Müller, T. & Beste, C. Effects of aging, Parkinsonās disease, and dopaminergic medication on response selection and control. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 327ā335 (2011).
Beste, C., Willemssen, R., Saft, C. & Falkenstein, M. Response inhibition subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: Basal ganglia disease effects. Neuropsychologia 48, 366ā373 (2010).
Dipasquale, O. et al. Unravelling the effects of methylphenidate on the dopaminergic and noradrenergic functional circuits. Neuropsychopharmacol 45, 1482ā1489 (2020).
Faraone, S. V. The pharmacology of amphetamine and methylphenidate: Relevance to the neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and other psychiatric comorbidities. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 87, 255ā270 (2018).
Kuczenski, R. & Segal, D. S. Effects of Methylphenidate on Extracellular Dopamine, Serotonin, and Norepinephrine: Comparison with Amphetamine. J. Neurochem. 68, 2032ā2037 (1997).
Beste, C. et al. Dopamine modulates the efficiency of sensory evidence accumulation during perceptual decision making. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 649ā655 (2018).
Kuo, M. F., Paulus, W. & Nitsche, M. A. Boosting focally-induced brain plasticity by Dopamine. Cereb. Cortex 18, 648ā651 (2008).
Monte-Silva, K., Liebetanz, D., Grundey, J., Paulus, W. & Nitsche, M. A. Dosage-dependent non-linear effect of l-dopa on human motor cortex plasticity. J. Physiol. 588, 3415ā3424 (2010).
Nitsche, M. A. et al. Catecholaminergic consolidation of motor cortical neuroplasticity in humans. Cereb. Cortex 14, 1240ā1245 (2004).
Nitsche, M. A. et al. Dopaminergic modulation of long-lasting direct current-induced cortical excitability changes in the human motor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 1651ā1657 (2006).
Fonteneau, C. et al. Frontal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Induces Dopamine Release in the Ventral Striatum in Human. Cereb. Cortex 28, 2636ā2646 (2018).
Koyun A. H., Wendiggensen P., Roessner V., Beste C., Stock A. K. Effects of catecholaminergic and transcranial direct current stimulation on response inhibition. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. Published online May 14, 2024:pyae023. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyae023.
Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135ā168 (2013).
Parkinson, J. & Haggard, P. Subliminal priming of intentional inhibition. Cognition 130, 255ā265 (2014).
Mcbride J., Boy F., Husain M., Sumner P. Automatic motor activation in the executive control of action. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00082.
Schlaghecken, F. & Eimer, M. Masked prime stimuli can bias quot;freequot; choices between response alternatives. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 463ā468 (2004).
Stock, A. K., Friedrich, J. & Beste, C. Subliminally and consciously induced cognitive conflicts interact at several processing levels. Cortex 85, 75ā89 (2016).
Boy, F., Husain, M. & Sumner, P. Unconscious inhibition separates two forms of cognitive control. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 107, 11134ā11139 (2010).
Adelhƶfer, N., Stock, A. K. & Beste, C. Anodal tDCS modulates specific processing codes during conflict monitoring associated with superior and middle frontal cortices. Brain Struct. Funct. 226, 1335ā1351 (2021).
Cavanagh, J. F. & Frank, M. J. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414ā421 (2014).
Cohen, M. X. & Donner, T. H. Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 2752ā2763 (2013).
Wendiggensen, P., Ghin, F., Koyun, A. H., Stock, A. K. & Beste, C. Pretrial Theta band activity affects context-dependent modulation of response inhibition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 605ā617 (2022).
Klimesch, W. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 606ā617 (2012).
Klimesch, W. Evoked alpha and early access to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibition timing hypothesis. Brain Res. 1408, 52ā71 (2011).
Van Diepen, R. M., Foxe, J. J. & Mazaheri, A. The functional role of alpha-band activity in attentional processing: the current zeitgeist and future outlook. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 29, 229ā238 (2019).
Jensen O. & Mazaheri A. Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010;4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186.
Zhou, Y. J., Iemi, L., Schoffelen, J. M., de Lange, F. P. & Haegens, S. Alpha oscillations shape sensory representation and perceptual sensitivity. J. Neurosci. 41, 9581ā9592 (2021).
Pertermann, M., Mückschel, M., Adelhƶfer, N., Ziemssen, T. & Beste, C. On the interrelation of 1/ f neural noise and norepinephrine system activity during motor response inhibition. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 1633ā1643 (2019).
Bluschke, A., Friedrich, J., Schreiter, M. L., Roessner, V. & Beste, C. A comparative study on the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying effects of methylphenidate and neurofeedback on inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 20, 1191ā1203 (2018).
Bensmann, W., Zink, N., Roessner, V., Stock, A. K. & Beste, C. Catecholaminergic effects on inhibitory control depend on the interplay of prior task experience and working memory demands. J. Psychopharmacol. 33, 678ā687 (2019).
Mückschel, M., Eggert, E., Prochnow, A. & Beste, C. Learning experience reverses catecholaminergic effects on adaptive behavior. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 23, 12ā19 (2020).
Mückschel, M., Roessner, V. & Beste, C. Task experience eliminates catecholaminergic effects on inhibitory control ā A randomized, double-blind cross-over neurophysiological study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 35, 89ā99 (2020).
Eggert, E. et al. Perception-action integration is modulated by the catecholaminergic system depending on learning experience. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 24, 592ā600 (2021).
Nikolin, S., Martin, D., Loo, C. K. & Boonstra, T. W. Effects of TDCS dosage on working memory in healthy participants. Brain Stimulation 11, 518ā527 (2018).
Rƶhner F. et al. Modulation of working memory using transcranial electrical stimulation: a direct comparison between TACS and TDCS. Front Neurosci. 2018;12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00761.
Cohen, M. X. A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling. Trends Neurosci. 37, 480ā490 (2014).
Koyun A. H. et al. Interactions of Catecholamines and GABA+ in Cognitive Control: Insights from EEG and 1H-MRS. NeuroImage. Published online April 26, 2024:120619. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2024.120619.
Mückschel, M., Stock, A. K., Dippel, G., Chmielewski, W. & Beste, C. Interacting sources of interference during sensorimotor integration processes. NeuroImage 125, 342ā349 (2016).
Beste, C., Münchau, A. & Frings, C. Towards a systematization of brain oscillatory activity in actions. Commun. Biol. 6, 1ā11 (2023).
Lobier, M., Palva, J. M. & Palva, S. High-alpha band synchronization across frontal, parietal and visual cortex mediates behavioral and neuronal effects of visuospatial attention. NeuroImage 165, 222ā237 (2018).
Sadaghiani, S. & Kleinschmidt, A. Brain Networks and α-Oscillations: Structural and Functional Foundations of Cognitive Control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 805ā817 (2016).
Bonnefond, M. & Jensen, O. The role of gamma and alpha oscillations for blocking out distraction. Commun. Integr. Biol. 6, e22702 (2013).
Moreau, Q., Candidi, M., Era, V., Tieri, G. & Aglioti, S. M. Midline frontal and occipito-temporal activity during error monitoring in dyadic motor interactions. Cortex 127, 131ā149 (2020).
Ramezanpour, H. & Fallah, M. The role of temporal cortex in the control of attention. Curr. Res. Neurobiol. 3, 100038 (2022).
Bensmann, W., Zink, N., Mückschel, M., Beste, C. & Stock, A. K. Neuronal networks underlying the conjoint modulation of response selection by subliminal and consciously induced cognitive conflicts. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1697ā1709 (2019).
Moss, H. E. et al. Selecting among competing alternatives: selection and retrieval in the left inferior frontal Gyrus. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1723ā1735 (2005).
Zhang, J. X., Feng, C. M., Fox, P. T., Gao, J. H. & Tan, L. H. Is left inferior frontal gyrus a general mechanism for selection? NeuroImage 23, 596ā603 (2004).
Swick, D., Ashley, V. & Turken, A. U. Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci. 9, 102 (2008).
Oztekin, I., Curtis, C. E. & McElree, B. The medial temporal lobe and the left inferior prefrontal cortex jointly support interference resolution in verbal working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1967ā1979 (2009).
Winocur, G., Moscovitch, M. & Bruni, J. Heightened Interference on Implicit, but Not Explicit, Tests of Negative Transfer: Evidence from Patients with Unilateral Temporal Lobe Lesions and Normal Old People. Brain Cogn. 30, 44ā58 (1996).
Herlin, B., Navarro, V. & Dupont, S. The temporal pole: From anatomy to functionāA literature appraisal. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 113, 101925 (2021).
Martin, C. B. et al. Resolving cross-modal semantic interference among object concepts requires medial temporal Lobe Cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 35, 869ā884 (2023).
Cortese, F. et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the left temporal pole restores normal visual evoked potential habituation in interictal migraineurs. J. Headache Pain. 18, 70 (2017).
Simons, J. S. & Spiers, H. J. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 637ā648 (2003).
Rahman, A., Lafon, B., Parra, L. C. & Bikson, M. Direct current stimulation boosts synaptic gain and cooperativity in vitro. J. Physiol. 595, 3535ā3547 (2017).
Beste C. et al. The basal ganglia striosomes affect the modulation of conflicts by subliminal Information - evidence from X-linked dystonia parkinsonism. Cerebral Cortex. 2017;28. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx125.
Goschke, T. & Bolte, A. Emotional modulation of control dilemmas: The role of positive affect, reward, and dopamine in cognitive stability and flexibility. Neuropsychologia 62, 403ā423 (2014).
Jamil, A. & Nitsche, M. A. What effect does tDCS have on the brain? Basic physiology of tDCS. Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep. 4, 331ā340 (2017).
Kuo M. F., PolanĆa R. & Nitsche M. Physiology of Transcranial Direct and Alternating Current Stimulation. In: Brunoni A., Nitsche M., Loo C., eds. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in Neuropsychiatric Disorders: Clinical Principles and Management. Springer International Publishing; 2016:29-46. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-33967-2_3.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G. & Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175ā191 (2007).
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A. & Lang, A. G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res Methods 41, 1149ā1160 (2009).
Challman, T. D. & Lipsky, J. J. Methylphenidate: Its pharmacology and uses. Mayo Clin. Proc. 75, 711ā721 (2000).
Kimko, H. C., Cross, J. T. & Abernethy, D. R. Pharmacokinetics and Clinical Effectiveness of Methylphenidate. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 37, 457ā470 (1999).
Jacobson, L., Javitt, D. C. & Lavidor, M. Activation of inhibition: diminishing impulsive behavior by direct current stimulation over the inferior frontal gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3380ā3387 (2011).
Stramaccia, D. F., Penolazzi, B., AltoĆØ, G. & Galfano, G. TDCS over the right inferior frontal gyrus disrupts control of interference in memory: A retrieval-induced forgetting study. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 144, 114ā130 (2017).
Lee, C., Jung, Y. J., Lee, S. J. & Im, C. H. COMETS2: An advanced MATLAB toolbox for the numerical analysis of electric fields generated by transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Neurosci. Methods 277, 56ā62 (2017).
Koyun, A. H., Stock, A. K. & Beste, C. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the differential effect of reward prospect on response selection and inhibition. Sci. Rep. 13, 10903 (2023).
Pedroni A., Bahreini A. & Langer N. Automagic: Standardized Preprocessing of Big EEG Data. Published online June 18, 2019:460469. https://doi.org/10.1101/460469.
Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9ā21 (2004).
Bigdely-Shamlo, N., Mullen, T., Kothe, C., Su, K. M. & Robbins, K. A. The PREP pipeline: standardized preprocessing for large-scale EEG analysis. Front Neuroinform. 9, 16 (2015).
Parra, L. C., Spence, C. D., Gerson, A. D. & Sajda, P. Recipes for the linear analysis of EEG. NeuroImage 28, 326ā341 (2005).
Winkler, I., Haufe, S. & Tangermann, M. Automatic classification of artifactual ICA-components for artifact removal in EEG signals. Behav. Brain Funct. 7, 30 (2011).
Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E. & Schoffelen, J. M. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011, 156869 (2011).
Gross, J. et al. Dynamic imaging of coherent sources: Studying neural interactions in the human brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 98, 694ā699 (2001).
Ester M., Kriegel H. P., Sander J. & Xu X. A. Density-Based Algorithm for Discovering Clusters in Large Spatial Databases with Noise. 96:226ā231 (1996).
Adelhƶfer, N. & Beste, C. Pre-trial theta band activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex correlates with inhibition-related theta band activity in the right inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage 219, 117052 (2020).
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. et al. Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain. NeuroImage 15, 273ā289 (2002).
Koyun, A. H. & Stock A. K. Neurophysiological Insights into Catecholamine-Dependent tDCS Modulation of Cognitive Control [Data set]. Open Science Frameworf: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/WEXGU.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) SFB 940 project B8 and by a Grant from the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung, BMBF) as part of the German Center for Child and Adolescent Health (DZKJ) under the funding code 01GL2405B.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Anna Helin Koyun: Data collection; Formal analysis; Methodology; Visualization; WritingāOriginal draft. Paul Wendiggensen: Methodology. Veit Roessner: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition. Christian Beste: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Project administration; Supervision. Ann-Kathrin Stock: Conceptualization; Data curation; Funding acquisition; Project administration; Supervision; WritingāOriginal draft.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
Christian Beste is an Editorial Board Member for Communications Biology, but was not involved in the editorial review of, nor the decision to publish this article. All other authors have nothing to disclose.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Biology thanks Miles Wischnewski and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editor: Jasmine Pan.
Additional information
Publisherās note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articleās Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the articleās Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Koyun, A.H., Wendiggensen, P., Roessner, V. et al. Neurophysiological insights into catecholamine-dependent tDCS modulation of cognitive control. Commun Biol 8, 375 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-025-07805-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-025-07805-6