Abstract
Stress can detrimentally affect physical and mental health, especially during childhood. During this critical period, parental bonds can foster resilience or amplify stress. This study explored whether mothers’ everyday stress can act as a source of childhood stress, affecting children’s executive functioning. 76 healthy mother-child dyads participated, with mothers assigned to a stress-inducing or stress-free condition. Children observed their mothers and were subsequently tested for cognitive flexibility and working memory. Subjective stress, heart rate, and cortisol were measured repeatedly in mothers and children, alongside everyday stress perceptions. Linear mixed models showed that children’s acute stress response was associated with impaired cognitive flexibility. Maternal stress, both acute and past-month, was a better predictor of children’s cognitive performance than children’s own stress. Quadratic relationships indicated the highest error rates at very low and high maternal stress. We found no evidence that children’s working memory was impaired by their own or their mothers’ stress. Although expected covariations of acute or prolonged stress between mothers and children were not observed, an interaction between maternal past-month stress and acute stress condition provided insights into adaptive mechanisms in children. These findings underscore the significant impact of maternal stress on children’s executive functioning, illustrating how parental experiences shape children’s everyday outcomes.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Extreme or chronic psychosocial stress can have severe consequences for physical and mental health1. In children, stressors are uniquely impactful and can result in detrimental effects on physical, emotional, and cognitive development2. During this vulnerable time, parental bonds have the potential to protect against or to exacerbate stress experiences3,4. The present study explores this dual role parents can play in shaping their children’s stress resilience.
Resilience refers to positive adaptive patterns under adverse circumstances5 and is alternatingly understood as an a) outcome, b) trait, or c) process6. Here, we address resilience as the measurable outcome when facing a stressful situation6. Stress can be understood from three perspectives: a) environmental, focusing on the stressors; b) psychological, focusing on subjective stress perceptions and emotional responses; and c) biological, focusing on the reactivity of physiological systems involved in the stress response7.
Most research on stress resilience examines outcomes following singular traumatic events8. This study, however, hones in on a frequent and highly relevant phenomenon: everyday stress resulting from social interactions. We examined how children’s psychophysiological stress response and, as an ultimate outcome, their executive functioning are affected by observing their mothers undergoing an experimental stressor. By investigating how children respond to their mothers’ exposure to stress, we aim to understand how caregiver relationships—typically a source of resilience4,5—might instead become a risk factor.
Early experiences with attachment figures shape children’s ability to interpret social cues and regulate emotions9,10,11. Empathy and physiological synchrony can facilitate this process, enhancing maternal responsivity to the child’s needs and strengthening the mother-child bond12,13,14. While an empathic attachment figure ideally enhances a child’s stress resilience9,11, strong empathic tendencies in children may lead to negative outcomes when a child consistently shares their mother’s negative emotions or stress.
Drawing from adult studies, we know that stress can be shared empathically between individuals. Such “empathic stress” responses, mirroring the physiological pattern of first-hand stress, include the release of cortisol, increased heart rate, and decreased high frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV) (see Engert et al. for a review15). The extent of empathic stress is influenced by empathic ability as well as physical and emotional closeness16,17. Families, with their close bonds and shared living spaces, provide an essential context to investigate empathic stress and its potential maladaptive consequences.
The notion that stress can be transmitted between family members has previously been investigated in the field of family stress research18,19. For instance, affect spillover was described from parents’ marital conflict or work stress onto their children20,21. Beyond self-reported or behavioral indicators of stress transmission, an increasing body of studies has started to assess physiological covariation between mothers and their children in the context of stressful situations22,23,24. Of particular interest are two naturalistic studies; one with six-year-olds, the other with adolescents, which investigated cortisol levels in mother-child dyads in their everyday lives25,26. Both revealed significant cortisol covariation in mother-child dyads across the day, particularly during periods of negative affect or stress, and with increased time spent together. Beyond the influence of common stressors, this time-bound covariation suggests the existence of an immediate pathway of stress transmission from one individual to the other.
Accordingly, many studies have examined maternal attunement with their children’s stress24,27,28. Yet, despite the important implications for childhood outcomes, fewer studies have focused on the reverse—how children physiologically resonate with their stressed mothers. Meaningful examples are two studies with mothers and their 12-14-month-old toddlers, which found that acute stress-induced sympathetic activation in mothers was transferred to their children, particularly when physical touch was involved29,30.
In a recent study, we extended these findings to 8-12-year-olds, revealing a causal link between psychosocial stress in mothers and children’s empathic stress responses31. After watching their mothers undergo a standardized psychosocial stress paradigm (Trier Social Stress Test32), children were more likely to release cortisol than their peers in a stress-free control group watching their mothers during relaxed reading. They also showed higher subjective stress, state empathy, and HF-HRV responses, with the latter depending on elevated trait cognitive empathy scores31. In the current study, the same sample of mother-child dyads was investigated with respect to the children’s cognitive performance following observation of their mothers performing the stress or stress-free control tasks.
Since frequent stress can alter the basal activity of the stress system and its responsiveness2,3, we explored how prolonged stress perceptions in mothers and children might interact with children’s acute stress responses. Particularly in childhood, repeated exposure to stressors can lead to adaptations in the stress system, resulting in hypersensitive or attenuated neuroendocrine and psychological responses to acute stressors33,34.
Like the stress system, cognitive development progresses rapidly during childhood, as children advance through school and face increasing demands to focus, shift attention, and complete complex tasks35,36,37. Early impairment or disrupted development of core executive functions, such as cognitive flexibility and working memory, can have enduring consequences3,38.
In a meta-analysis, Shields and colleagues39 found that in adults, acute stress negatively affects cognitive flexibility and working memory, which are responsible for switching between different tasks or adapting to changing situational requirements40, and for temporary storage and manipulation of information41, respectively. Research on the effects of psychosocial stress on these executive functions in children is scarce. Exceptions include a study by Seehagen and colleagues42, which demonstrated that acute stress diminished cognitive flexibility in children of 15 months of age. Quesada and colleagues43 found no changes in working memory performance following a psychosocial stressor in 8- to 10-year-old children. The present study investigated both cognitive flexibility and working memory of 8- to 12-year-old children after observing their mothers being exposed to either a laboratory stressor or a stress-free control task.
Studies on chronic stress have shown how early life adversity relates to impaired cognitive flexibility44,45,46 and working memory47,48,49 stretching into adolescence and adulthood. As mechanisms connecting stress to impaired executive functioning, Marko and Riečanský50 identified increased sympathetic arousal for impaired cognitive flexibility and cognitive interference for working memory impairment in adults. Additionally, the prefrontal cortex (PFC), a brain region crucial for executive functioning, is highly sensitive to the neuroendocrine environment51 and has been shown to exhibit disadvantageous structural changes after prolonged psychosocial stress52.
Of note, the very cognitive functions that are sensitive to stress exposure simultaneously form an integral resource of stress resilience53. These findings underscore how impactful childhood stress is for the developing brain and long-term resilient outcomes. However, the mentioned studies on chronic stress focused on circumstances as severe as maltreatment, neglectful caregiving, or poverty. Research on the impact of everyday stressors on children’s executive functioning is still needed.
In summary, empathic stress responses among adults have been demonstrated repeatedly15, initial evidence for covariation between maternal and child stress was found31, and numerous studies have established a link between stress and impaired executive functioning39. We took the next step and sought empirical evidence linking maternal stress, both acute and prolonged, to children’s cognitive outcomes. Considering the central role of the mother-child bond for child development and resilient outcomes4,5,10, alongside the prevalence of chronic stress in families54,55, understanding how empathic stress influences child-specific outcomes becomes crucial for identifying resilience and risk factors.
Extending Blasberg et al. 31, this study builds on confirmed empathic stress in a German sample of healthy mother-child dyads. We hypothesized that children’s acute psychophysiological stress response would impair their cognitive flexibility and working memory. Next to children’s stress response, we expected the resonance with their mothers’ subjective stress, autonomic reactivity, and cortisol release to predict slower reaction times and reduced accuracy (H1). Second, we explored whether prolonged experiences of everyday stress in children and mothers would influence the effect of the acute experimental condition, differentially affecting children’s cognitive performance (H2). As seen for acute stress31, our third aim was to provide empirical evidence that children’s trait empathy (H3a) and relationship closeness (H3b) with their mothers would shape the extent to which they share their mother’s past-month stress experience.
Methods
Participants
The study sample was recruited within the context of a larger study31. Participation was promoted online and with posters, targeting schools and after-school care in Leipzig (Germany) and surrounding areas. Mothers underwent a telephone screening before inclusion. Age criteria were set at 31 to 45 years for mothers and 8 to 12 years for children to minimize hormonal influences on cortisol levels56. Mothers after menopause onset and children after menarche onset were excluded. The lower age limit in children ensured they could read and write, show stress responses to a cognitively processed event57, and show higher-order components of empathy58. Further exclusion criteria for mothers were pregnancy, daily cigarette use, regular recreational drug use, or inability to abstain from alcohol for one week. Dyads with a BMI below 18.5 or above 30, lack of German fluency, dyslexia, HPA axis-interfering medications, previous psychosocial stress test participation, or other physical and mental health factors that influence the stress response were excluded.
Upon completion of the recruitment phase, 76 children (n = 37 girls, n = 39 boys) and their mothers were eligible to participate. Children were 8 to 12 years old (M = 9.95, SD = 1.42), and mothers were 31 to 45 years old (M = 40.3, SD = 3.33). Dyads were pseudo-randomly assigned to an experimental (n = 39) or control group (n = 37), controlling for self-reported age and sex. No data on race or ethnicity were collected. The study received approval from the Ethics Committee of Leipzig University (number: 084/18-ek). Written informed consent was sought from mothers on behalf of themselves and their children. Mothers were financially compensated and children received a gift for participation.
Procedure
The study protocol was registered in the context of the primary study by Blasberg and colleagues31 (https://osf.io/f3wke). Testing took place at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences in Leipzig from June 2018 to March 2020. Testing procedures are depicted in Fig. 1. Each testing session took approximately 2.5 hours and was scheduled between 3 pm and 7 pm to align with cortisol diurnal patterns59.
The testing session started with a resting period. Children were subsequently familiarized with the cognitive tasks. Simultaneous sampling of all stress markers in mothers and children began 20 minutes before the stress induction (Trier Social Stress Test; TSST; Kirschbaum et al. 32) or control paradigm. Children then underwent cognitive testing. Finally, mothers and children completed a set of questionnaires. Subjective stress, heart rate, and cortisol were assessed at eight time points each. An electrocardiogram (ECG) was recorded from 20 minutes prior until 45 minutes after the TSST or control task.
Upon arrival, mothers and children were separated, briefed about the experiment, and provided with a snack and juice to equalize blood glucose levels. They were allowed only water for the remainder of the session. After a short resting period to allow participants to settle in, mothers signed informed consent and took a drug test. In parallel, children were familiarized with the cognitive tasks they performed later in the experiment. To reduce potential stress caused by the laboratory setting, time was allotted for resting before beginning the experiment. Children were then accompanied to an observation room which was separated from the testing room by a one-way mirror. Mothers were informed that their children could see and hear the procedure, and instructed for their upcoming task. Depending on the experimental group, mothers either underwent the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST32) or a stress-free control task. Upon completion of the mothers’ respective tasks, children’s cognitive flexibility and working memory were assessed in two computerized tasks, the category-switching60 and the n-back task61. The tasks were administered in randomized order. Before reunion, mothers and children completed a set of questionnaires.
To assess acute stress reactions simultaneously in mothers and children, eight measurements of each subjective stress, heart rate, and salivary cortisol were collected in regular intervals from −20 min until +70 minutes relative to TSST or control task onset. Continuous electrocardiogram (ECG) recordings were collected but not analyzed due to the high number of missing data points31.
Stress induction and control tasks
Mothers in the experimental and control groups participated in different tasks of the same duration, providing the children with equivalent exposure to their mothers across groups. In the experimental group, acute psychosocial stress in mothers was induced through the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), a standardized laboratory stressor that reliably provokes physiological stress responses32,62. The TSST comprises a five-minute anticipation phase (task preparation) and two stressful tasks: a five-minute mock job talk and a five-minute mental arithmetic session. Both are audio- and video-taped and evaluated by a committee of two alleged behavioral analysts, who are trained to act non-responsively.
Mothers in the control group completed a non-stressful reading task, involving a five-minute anticipation phase (silent magazine reading) and a ten-minute task in which they read aloud from a nature book. Unlike the stress group, mothers were alone during the control task. Mothers of both groups were aware that their children were observing.
Cognitive tasks
To complete the computerized tasks, children sat approximately 50 to 60 cm away from a computer screen with a resolution of 1680 × 1050 pixels and a refresh rate of 60 Hz. Both tasks were implemented using Presentation® (2018).
Category-switching task
Cognitive flexibility was assessed using a category-switching task60. In three blocks of 64 trials each, children categorized words based on presented categories (“animate/inanimate” or “bigger/smaller than a soccer ball”) within 3000 ms. Categories varied randomly, requiring the children to constantly update their mental representation of the task. Accordingly, trial types included repetition and switch trials. Additionally, the Cue Target Interval (CTI) between category and word was either short (200 ms) or long (1000 ms). For an accurate comparison of mean reaction times in switch and repetition trials, the first trial of each block and trials following an error were excluded. Personalized Tukey far out outliers63 were calculated to exclude reaction times of three standard deviations above or below individual means.
N-back task
To assess working memory, the computerized n-back task by Kirchner61 was used. Children were presented with letters (B, C, D, F, G, J, K, L, M, N, O, P, R, S, T, V, W, Y, Z) for 500 ms in random order. They had to decide whether the current letter equaled the last (1-back) or second last (2-back) letter and had 1500 ms to indicate their decision by pressing the right or left arrow keys. The first block consisted of 65 1-back trials, the second block of 65 2-back trials. Working memory was challenged by holding the series of letters in mind and operationalized in terms of mean reaction time and accuracy per block. Incorrect or missed trials were excluded from the reaction time mean calculations, as were reaction times more than three standard deviations away from the individual mean reaction time (personalized Tukey far outs). As an accuracy index, we calculated d-prime (d’) as a normalized comparison of the hit rate (correct responses vs. misses) and the false alarm rate (false rejection vs. correct rejection)64.
Measurements of acute stress
Subjective stress
Subjective stress was assessed using the single self-report item, “How stressed do you feel at this moment?”. Answers were provided on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (no stress) to 7 (severe stress).
Heart rate
As an indicator of sympathetic activity65, heart rate was recorded using an OMRON RS2 wrist blood pressure monitor (Medizintechnik Handelsgesellschaft mbH, Mannheim, Germany) that mothers and children were equipped with at the beginning of the testing session. Heart rate, reported in beats per minute (bpm), was checked concurrently with subjective stress and cortisol measurements (see Fig. 1).
Cortisol
HPA axis activity was measured in terms of salivary cortisol levels. Saliva was collected using Salivettes (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany) with a cotton swab that participants held in their mouths for two minutes. The Salivettes were stored at −30 °C and later analyzed at the Biochemical Laboratory of the Department of Biological and Clinical Psychology, Trier University, using a time-resolved fluorescence immunoassay with intra- and inter-assay variabilities of less than 10% and 12%66.
Preprocessing
Heart rate and cortisol data were natural-log-transformed to ensure normal distribution. Cortisol data were winsorized to three standard deviations to adjust for potential outliers67,68. To measure stress reactivity, the area under the curve with respect to increase (AUCi69) was calculated. The AUCi has been commonly used in endocrinological research to summarize repeated stress measurements while emphasizing change over time69. Following the methodology from Pruessner et al. 69, the AUCi for each stress marker was calculated by summing the area under the curve of cortisol, subjective stress, and heart rate across the eight measurement time points and subsequently subtracting the area between zero and the baseline measurement.
Questionnaire data
Perceived stress in mothers
As a measure of prolonged stress over the past month, mothers completed the German version of the “Perceived Stress Scale” (PSS70). It comprises ten items, assessing unpredictability, uncontrollability, and overload in their lives over the past month. Items are rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (“never”) to 4 (“very often”). Total scores therefore range between 0-40 with higher scores indicating higher stress levels. In our sample, internal consistency was acceptable (α = 0.76).
Perceived stress in children
Children completed the German version of the “Perceived Stress Scale for Children” (PSS-C71), featuring 13 age-adapted items assessing prolonged stress. To yield more adequate reports in that age group, the questionnaire refers to the past week rather than month. The answer scale ranges from 0 (“never”) to 3 (“a lot”) and is complemented by a graphic representation of quantities. Total scores range between 0-39 with higher scores indicating higher stress levels. The questionnaire showed poor internal consistency in our sample (α = 0.5). This issue likely arose because the questionnaire taps into different areas of children’s lives, such as school performance, friendships, relationships with parents, and emotional states. As the goal was to gauge accumulated everyday stress, using a sum score of these heterogeneous items was preferred over discarding the questionnaire at whole. However, the potential lack of reliability and validity calls for cautious interpretation of PSS-C results.
Trait empathy
As a potential modulator of stress transmission between mothers and children, we evaluated children’s trait empathy using the “Empathy Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents” (EmQue-CA72). Children answered the 14 items on a three-point scale (1: “not true” to 3: “true”). The EmQue-CA provides an overall trait empathy score (α = 0.67) and the three subscales: affective empathy (α = 0.54), cognitive empathy (α = 0.76), and intention to comfort (α = 0.47). Higher scores reflect higher trait empathy on the respective scale. As only cognitive empathy showed acceptable reliability, this subscale alone was considered when testing moderation hypothesis H3a.
Relationship closeness
As an indicator of self-reported closeness between mothers and children, all participants filled out the Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale (IOS73). This single-item graphic scale contains seven Venn diagram-like pairs of circles with varying overlap. Mothers and children selected the circle pair that best represented their relationship with each other. Scores could lie between 1-7 with higher values indicating more relationship closeness.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted using R 4.2.074. The analyses for this study were not preregistered. All analysis scripts and data required to replicate the results are publicly available at https://osf.io/kyrbh/. The sample size was predetermined based on the power analysis reported in Blasberg et al. 31, aiming to detect medium to large effects of empathic stress17. Given the exploratory nature of the study, we deemed the sample size adequate for further analyses of empathic stress effects on cognition, acknowledging that power might be limited for some models. Statistical significance was tested two-sided with an alpha of 0.05 in all analyses. Assumptions for each type of analysis, including normality and homoscedasticity, were tested using visual inspection and considered adequately met unless specified otherwise.
Prior to model testing, children with insufficient task understanding were excluded, resulting in the exclusion of one child for the category switching task and two children for the n-back task. Further, individuals with two or more consecutive missing data points of a stress marker were excluded for the respective analysis. For cases with only one missing value, the average of the two data points surrounding the missing value was imputed. Subjective stress data from two individuals (n = 1 mother and n = 1 child) and heart rate data from one child were missing from one time point. Five individuals missed cortisol data from one time point (n = 2 mothers and n = 3 children), with one mother missing cortisol data from two consecutive time points.
Group differences
Based on our previously published results in Blasberg et al. 31 we knew that the number of mothers and children exhibiting a physiologically significant cortisol release ( > 1.5 nmol/l75) was higher in the stress group (28 mothers; seven children) than in the control group (six mothers; one child). Also, stress group mothers and children reported higher levels of subjective stress, and stress group mothers exhibited higher heart rate reactivity than their control group counterparts. Our stress induction paradigm was therefore successful.
In addition to this manipulation check, we tested whether performance on the cognitive tasks differed between stress and control groups. Repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted for accuracy and reaction time, considering multiple values per participant due to the repeated measures task structures. Accuracy was conceptualized as error rates for the category-switching task60 and as a d’ index for the n-back task61. Reaction time was assessed in milliseconds in both tasks. Two additional ANOVAs were run to test whether the stress manipulation influenced mothers’ and children’s self-report of recently perceived stress.
Linear mixed models
Linear mixed models (LMMs) were employed to examine relationships between children’s and mothers’ stress measures and cognitive performance. Two model-versions were run per hypothesis and task, using either reaction time or accuracy as dependent variables. Fixed effects included children’s age, sex, task characteristics (trial or block types), and experimental group in all models. For each hypothesis, we further added fixed effects of interest. All numeric predictors were scaled prior to analysis to enhance interpretability. Each model incorporated a random intercept for subjects to account for the repeated measures structure of the cognitive tasks.
By adding fixed effects of children’s stress reactivity, measured by the AUCi for a given marker, the influence of overall child stress on their cognitive performance was indicated. To target the effect of children’s acute resonance with their mother’s stress on cognitive performance (H1), an interaction term of child and maternal AUCis of either cortisol, subjective stress, or heart rate was introduced. This resulted in three models per outcome and task. The hypothesized modulating effect of recent everyday stress (H2) was tested by adding a three-way interaction of children’s perceived stress (PSS-C), mothers’ perceived stress (PSS), and experimental group.
As outlined above, high levels of stress can have adverse effects on cognitive performance. However, there is reason to assume that the full spectrum of stress, including low and moderate levels, can relate to cognitive performance in a linear or in a quadratic fashion76. Therefore, we first conducted the analysis using the linear term for a given child or maternal stress marker, adding quadratic terms to each model in a second exploratory step.
All models were fitted using the “lme4” R package77 and p-values were computed using the “lmerTest” package78. Bootstrap procedures with 1000 resamples were applied for robust parameter estimation (“boot” package79,80). Continuous predictors were z-standardized to address multicollinearity, yielding acceptable variance inflation factors (VIF) of < 5 in all models. Given the number of models, careful considerations were made regarding the correction for multiple comparisons. Following recommendations by García-Pérez81, we concluded that our analyses do not call for correction, as we assume no omnibus null hypothesis. Instead, we explore the effects of different stress systems (i.e., subjective psychological stress, autonomically regulated heart rate, and HPA-axis output cortisol) on different outcomes (i.e., reaction time and accuracy), which can be assumed to demonstrate opposing patterns in terms of a speed-accuracy trade-off82.
Moderation analysis
This analysis tested whether the hypothesized relationship between maternal and child prolonged stress was moderated by children’s empathy and closeness to their mothers. Multiple linear regression models were used. In the basic model, maternal prolonged stress (PSS) predicted children’s prolonged stress (PSS-C), controlling for age and sex. For the moderation hypothesis H3a, an interaction term of PSS and children’s cognitive empathy was introduced. For H3b the interaction of PSS and relationship closeness (IOS) was added to the basic model.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Results
Correlations between all predictor and outcome variables are provided in Supplementary Table 1. For more detail on sample characteristics and group differences in stress responses, please see Blasberg et al. 31.
Group differences in cognitive performance and prolonged stress
The repeated-measures ANOVA detected no significant effects of experimental condition (TSST vs. control) on children’s reaction time or accuracy in the category-switching (F(1,73) = 0.28, p = 0.599, d = −0.1, 95% CI [−0.33, 0.12]; F(1,73) = 1.37, p = 0.245, d = 0.24, 95% CI [0.01, 0.47]) or n-back tasks (F(1,72) = 0.03, p = 0.853, d = −0.04, 95% CI [−0.36, 0.28]; F(1,70) = 0.33, p = 0.565, d = 0.08, 95% CI [−0.25, 0.4]). Thus, we did not find evidence that the stress manipulation affected children’s executive functioning on a group level.
ANOVA results revealed no significant difference in maternal and child prolonged stress between the TSST and control groups (PSS: F(1,72) = 0.04, p = 0.851, d = −0.04, 95% CI [−0.5, 0.41]; PSS-C: F(1,70) = 0.72, p = 0.400, d = −0.2, 95% CI [−0.66, 0.26]). Therefore, we have no indication to believe that the stress manipulation compromised the reliability of stress reports. The results of the ANOVA models are summarized in Supplementary Table 2.
Effects of acute stress on cognitive performance
Reaction time
For the category-switching task, analyses showed no significant effects of children’s and mothers’ stress markers, or of their interaction, on reaction time. Test statistics of all models predicting reaction time in the category-switching task are provided in Supplementary Table 3. In contrast, reaction time in the n-back task was significantly predicted by maternal subjective stress in the linear model (t(67) = −2.41, p = 0.017, β = −53.64, 95% CI [−100.15, −11.48]). This effect suggests increased response speed when mothers reported more subjective stress. No additional linear or quadratic stress markers of either children or mothers significantly predicted reaction time in the working memory task as outlined in Supplementary Table 4.
Accuracy
Regarding accuracy, children’s error rates in the category-switching task were related to mothers’ acute stress reactivity, showing more errors in children with stronger maternal cortisol increase (Table 1a). We found no evidence of an interaction between mother and child cortisol reactivity and therefore of an influence of empathic stress resonance on children’s cognitive flexibility. No other markers, including those of children, showed significant linear relationships with children’s performance.
Introducing quadratic terms substantially changed this pattern (Table 1b). Children’s subjective stress predicted optimal accuracy in the cognitive flexibility task around average stress reactivity (Fig. 2a). While children’s heart rates remained an insignificant predictor (Fig. 2b), an effect of their cortisol reactivity on error rates emerged (Fig. 2c). All quadratic maternal stress markers significantly predicted children’s cognitive flexibility, with optimal accuracy ranging between 1 SD and 0.5 SD below average stress reactivity (Fig. 2d–f). As before, we did not find the hypothesized interaction between child and maternal stress markers, leaving us with a lack of evidence for empathic stress resonance effects on cognitive flexibility. Moreover, no effects of child stress, maternal stress, or their interaction were found on accuracy in the n-back task (see Supplementary Table 5).
Effects of the AUCis (Area Under the Curve with respect to increase) of children’s and mothers’ stress markers on children’s error rates in the category-switching task (Mayr & Kliegl, 60) are depicted. Panels a–c represent the linear and quadratic effects of children’s subjective stress, heart rate, and cortisol. Panels d–f represent the corresponding effects of maternal stress markers. The grey shading represents the standard error (SE). Four data points per dyad (n = 75) are shown due to the 2×2 task structure.
Summary acute stress
Taken together, we found evidence that children’s acute stress reactivity, particularly subjective stress and cortisol release, was associated with impaired accuracy but not reaction time in the category-switching task. Notably, all maternal stress markers predicted children’s performance on the same outcome, indicating impairing effects of very low or very high maternal stress. Further, maternal subjective stress related to enhanced reaction time in the n-back task. While the data did not support the hypothesized empathic stress resonance effects on cognition, maternal acute stress emerged as a key predictor of children’s performance.
Effects of prolonged stress on cognitive performance
Reaction time
The fixed effects predicting cognitive flexibility speed are presented in Table 2 and reaction time results for working memory are detailed in Supplementary Table 6. As with acute stress, linear and quadratic analyses did not reveal significant associations between maternal or child prolonged stress and reaction time in the category-switching task. Further, no associations were found with speed in the working memory task.
Accuracy
Results for accuracy in the category-switching task are presented in Table 2. We found that children’s error rate was predicted by maternal past-month stress, which exhibited a quadratic effect with optimal performance around 0.25 SD above average (Fig. 3a). Further, an interaction effect of condition with maternal prolonged stress emerged (Fig. 3b). It shows that lower levels of maternal stress in the past month related to better accuracy in the control condition but worse accuracy in the TSST condition. This pattern reversed with higher maternal prolonged stress. Here, children’s cognitive flexibility was better in the TSST condition than in the control condition. Paralleling results for acute stress, we found no evidence for effects of children’s or mothers’ prolonged stress on accuracy in the n-back task (Supplementary Table 6).
In a, main effects of mothers’ PSS (Perceived Stress Scale; Cohen et al. 70) scores on children’s error rates in the category-switching task (Mayr & Kliegl, 60) are depicted. In b, interaction effects of the PSS with the experimental condition, i.e. a stress-free control task vs. the TSST (Trier Social Stress Test, Kirschbaum et al. 32), on children’s error rates are shown. The grey shading represents the standard error (SE). Four data points per dyad (n = 75) are shown due to the 2×2 task structure.
Summary prolonged stress
In summary, the results demonstrate that maternal prolonged stress was related to children’s accuracy in the category-switching task. No evidence was found for effects on children’s reaction time in the category-switching task or on any of the outcomes in the n-back task. Further, the analyses indicate that mothers’ latent stress levels modified the impact of an acute stressor on their children’s cognitive flexibility. While the hypothesized interaction with children’s stress levels (and thus resonance) was not observed, these results resemble the pattern found for acute stress.
Relationship of mother and child stress
Neither the model featuring children’s empathy, nor the model using dyad closeness explained a significant amount of variance in children’s everyday stress perceptions, accounting for only 8.5% and 6% of variance, respectively (F(5, 68) = 1.26, p = 0.29, R² = 0.085, 95% CI [0.043, 0.283]; F(5, 68) = 0.86, p = 0.511, R² = 0.06, 95% CI [0.03, 0.348]). Since no evidence for a relationship between maternal and child prolonged stress was found, H3 was rejected.
Discussion
This study investigated how witnessing their mothers under stress can pose a challenge to children’s own stress resilience by influencing their executive functioning. To this end, children observed their mothers completing either a psychosocial stress or a stress-free control task and subsequently underwent cognitive testing. Subjective stress, heart rate, and cortisol of both mothers and children were collected throughout the session, as well as perceived stress in the past month to gauge prolonged stress load. We analyzed the relationship between these stress markers and children’s cognitive performance, and between children’s and mothers’ prolonged stress. While former studies have shown that children resonate emotionally and physiologically with their stressed mothers29,30,31, and that both acute42 and chronic3,38 stress exposure affect children’s cognition, an explicit link between maternal stress exposure and children’s cognitive performance requires empirical evidence. Here we addressed this gap, contributing to the understanding of how the mother-child bond can represent not only a source of - but also a strain on - children’s stress resilience.
Our results demonstrate that children’s acute stress markers were only sporadically associated with impaired cognitive performance and no effects were found for their self-reported prolonged stress. Interestingly, it was maternal acute and prolonged stress that emerged as the better predictor of children’s executive functioning, especially when considering a quadratic relationship. While we found no evidence for the hypothesized effects of mother-child resonance on children’s cognitive performance, an interaction between maternal past-month stress and acute experimental condition pointed towards adaptive mechanisms in children with frequently stressed mothers. Overall, our results show that children’s executive functions are sensitive to their mothers’ stress experiences.
Acute stress effects on cognitive performance
The present study extends our previous finding of a successful empathic stress induction in children31 by exploring the effects of this second-hand response on executive functioning. Although children observing their mothers undergo the TSST experienced subjective and physiological stress more frequently compared to the control group31, this did not translate into a statistical group difference in cognitive performance. On an individual level, however, we found several stress effects of both children and mothers on executive functioning. Of note, these effects followed a general pattern, such that stress effects on cognitive flexibility were only detectable for accuracy, whereas stress effects on working memory were only found for reaction time. This observation is in line with previous studies83,84.
In the category-switching task, children’s cortisol response was linked to impaired accuracy, aligning with a body of studies underscoring the role of cortisol in detrimental stress effects on executive functioning51,83. Through the exploratory second step of the data analysis, accounting for a possible quadratic relationship, children’s subjective stress reaction emerged as an additional predictive marker of cognitive flexibility, revealing that a mild stress response was linked to improved accuracy. The notion that stress and cognitive performance can relate in a bell-shaped manner has long been discussed in the context of the Yerkes-Dodson law85. Despite the intuitive appeal of the idea that an ideal amount of stress increases performance, this assumption has been criticized for its lack of empirical evidence86. Yet, there is reason to assume that acute stress can be beneficial, as one of the core functions of the human stress response is to mobilize energy2. On a cognitive level, mild stress has also been shown to relate to optimal PFC functioning51 and to narrow down the attentional focus to the task at hand87.
Interestingly, we found similar quadratic associations for all maternal stress markers and a linear impairing effect of maternal cortisol on the same outcome. These effects occurred regardless of children’s stress reactions. In our experiment, children’s stress responses were presumably provoked by their empathic reaction to their mothers’ stress. We showed this to be the case in the original report by Blasberg and colleagues31, where covariance between maternal and child stress depending on children’s empathy was found. While the absence of a statistically significant interaction effect between maternal and child stress in our study challenges the assumption that acute empathic stress resonance is the cause of cognitive flexibility impairment, maternal subjective and physiological stress emerged as key predictors of children’s performance.
This partially aligns with Roos et al.’s88 discovery that self-reported maternal parenting stress was associated with children’s physiological stress reactivity, which, in turn, was not associated with children’s cognitive impairment. Parenting behavior has frequently been discussed as an explanatory pathway by which parental stress exerts effects on children89. If a parent can provide high levels of protection90, positive bonding experiences91, and secure attachment92, their parenting behavior can act as a protective mechanism, fostering resilience in the face of stress and the development of executive functioning. However, as our findings illustrate, if the child is subjected to their parents experiencing stressful situations, the child is at risk of suffering the negative second-hand effects, even if not acutely resonating with parental stress.
Whereas mothers’ and children’s stress markers predominantly showed the hypothesized impairing relationship with cognitive flexibility, the effects of acute stress on working memory demonstrated contradicting patterns. Children’s response speed increased with higher maternal subjective stress, while no evidence for effects of their own stress responses was found. The current understanding of stress effects on working memory speed remains inconclusive, with studies reporting both positive93 and negative84 effects, particularly lacking research in children except for one null finding43. Although energy mobilization and attention narrowing may explain speed enhancement effects of stress, the present data don’t allow for a conclusive interpretation regarding this matter.
In summarizing our findings on acute stress effects on executive functioning, the key question arises: How does maternal subjective and physiological stress impact child cognition if the child does not acutely resonate with the mother?
Prolonged stress effects on cognitive performance
A likely mechanism linking maternal stress and children’s cognition lies in the influence of general stress reactivity patterns. Research investigating the influence of subjective appraisals of stressful situations on the extent of the acute physiological stress response has shown that individuals perceiving stressors as more threatening or overwhelming in general, show increased psychophysiological responses to acute stressors94,95. Thus, if mothers who show stronger stress responses in the TSST are also more stress-reactive in everyday life96, it is plausible that their children are more frequently exposed to a stressed parent and experience more stress overall. However, children’s self-reported prolonged stress was not significantly related to their cognitive performance, contradicting the consensus that prolonged stress negatively affects cognitive functioning1,2. Since this association can be considered established, we have reason to believe that the questionable internal consistency of the PSS-C71 may have compromised the validity of our prolonged stress measure for children. Additionally, none of the children reported high levels of perceived stress on this scale. For these reasons, we cannot conclusively interpret the lack of evidence for an interaction between maternal and children’s prolonged stress.
Instead of the expected interaction effect of mother and child prolonged stress, we found that maternal perceived stress in the past month predicted performance in the category-switching task irrespective of children’s PSS-C values. As an influential factor of this relationship, Knauft and colleagues97 highlighted the importance of the interplay between chronic and acute stress. In their study with adults, they found that an acute stressor had negative effects on cognitive flexibility only in the absence of chronic stress. Our results affirm this pattern: Children’s cognitive flexibility in the stress group was only impaired when their mothers did not report prolonged stress. This pattern may indicate the presence of successful coping, with children in high-stress environments being less perturbed by their parent’s acute stress, having adapted to and regulated or repressed their emotional reactions. Especially in younger children, whose internal working models are still developing, emotional detachment from their caregivers can occur as a strategy of self-protection10,98. While most frequently reported with parental neglect or maltreatment99,100, our data suggest that similar processes may be at play in the face of maternal everyday stress. This capacity to adapt can be considered as resilience to adverse environments.
Stress transmission between mothers and children in everyday life
Last, we aimed to examine factors that influence the degree to which prolonged perceptions of stress are shared between mothers and children. While children in our sample showed acute resonance with their mother’s subjective and physiological stress31, evidence of a covariance of prolonged stress within the same dyads could not be found. Therefore, we could not replicate the moderating role of trait empathy and closeness found in our recent31 and other studies16,17,29. Of note, the present study focused on everyday empathic stress transmission as a source of chronic childhood stress and consequent cognitive impairment. However, it became apparent that stress resonance alone cannot account for maternal influences on child cognition. Other theories involve broader stressors shared within families such as poverty and discrimination101,102 or altered parenting behavior due to stress experience89.
Limitations
There are several limitations to this study. First, our sample cannot be considered representative, as only healthy, German, and predominantly highly educated mother-child dyads participated. Across the whole sample, self-reported prolonged stress of mothers and children was low. Especially given the quadratic relationship observed within this limited stress range, future research should explore the circumstances under which stress promotes or compromises cognitive performance. To this end, we suggest extending findings to more diverse samples, broader ranges of chronic stress, and additional attachment constellations like father-child dyads.
Second, the questionnaire administered to assess children’s perceived stress (PSS-C71) turned out to be of poor internal consistency. Although the PSS-C promised high comparability with the PSS and was considered an adequate measure of accumulated everyday stress in the investigated age group, its lack of reliability and validity compromised the quality of the results. Future research may further benefit from including physiological measures of long-term stress in addition to self-report scales.
Third, children were separated from their mothers upon arrival, despite physical proximity being a moderator of stress contagion25,29. While the separation was realized to prevent active emotional co-regulation, it is likely to have reduced empathic stress occurrence. As the hypothesized effects on child cognition are of indirect nature, future studies should maximize the likelihood of empathic stress transmission and recruit larger samples to increase sensitivity for smaller effects.
Last, the stressful nature of the unfamiliar environment and the cognitive tasks themselves may have been more salient for children than observing their mothers undergo the TSST. Although efforts were made to mitigate potential confounding effects, these factors cannot be entirely ruled out.
Conclusions
Close relationships can be a double-edged sword. On the one hand, they provide us with support and protection in the face of stress. On the other hand, witnessing the stress of those close to us can inflict strain on ourselves. The current results show that even moderately high acute and past-month stress in mothers can relate to impaired accuracy of their children in a cognitive flexibility task. Although results for working memory remained inconclusive and no evidence could be provided for the hypothesized connection between maternal and children’s stress reactivity via empathic resonance, the current results make a viable contribution to the field of stress research in the family environment. Showing that very low or above average levels of maternal subjective and physiological stress are associated with impaired cognitive performance in children highlights the importance of the family environment for children’s everyday outcomes. While the stress response can be adaptive, prolonged or severe exposure, even of secondary nature, bears the danger of long-term detrimental effects on body and brain1. Especially for children, in whom stress and its effects can have long-lasting developmental consequences3,103, it is crucial to identify and tackle potential risks to strengthen resilience at an early age. Understanding the consequences of parental stress for children can inform future studies developing targeted strategies to help parents promote positive outcomes for their children.
Data availability
The data used in this study, including cognitive performance metrics, physiological measures, and questionnaire responses, are publicly accessible at https://osf.io/kyrbh/.
Code availability
Analysis scripts are publicly accessible at https://osf.io/kyrbh/.
References
McEwen, B. S. Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: Understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur. J. Pharm. 583, 174–185 (2008).
Chrousos, G. P. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 5, 374–381 (2009).
Thompson, R. A. Stress and child development. Future Child. 24, 41–59 (2014).
Winston, R. & Chicot, R. The importance of early bonding on the long-term mental health and resilience of children. Lond. J. Prim. Care (Abingdon) 8, 12–14 (2016).
Masten, A. S. & Obradović, J. Competence and resilience in development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1094, 13–27 (2006).
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M. & Garmezy, N. Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Dev. Psychopathol. 2, 425–444 (1990).
Cohen, S., Kessler, R. C., & Gordon, L. U. (Eds.). Measuring stress: A guide for health and social scientists. (Oxford University Press, USA, 1997)
Dunkel Schetter, C. D. & Dolbier, C. Resilience in the context of chronic stress and health in adults. Soc. Personal. Psychol. compass 5, 634–652 (2011).
Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1, Attachment. (New York, NY, Basic Books, 1969/1982)
Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss: Vol. 2: Separation, anxiety and anger. (New York, NY, Basic Books, 1973)
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. Attachment style and affect regulation: Implications for coping with stress and mental health. Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Interpersonal processes, 535-557. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470998557 (2003).
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. Patterns of attachment. A psychological study of the Strange Situation. (Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, 1978)
Ebisch, S. J. et al. Mother and child in synchrony: thermal facial imprints of autonomic contagion. Biol. Psychol. 89, 123–129 (2012).
Manini, B. et al. Mom feels what her child feels: Thermal signatures of vicarious autonomic response while watching children in a stressful situation. Front Hum. Neurosci. 7, 299 (2013).
Engert, V., Linz, R. & Grant, J. A. Embodied stress: The physiological resonance of psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 105, 138–146 (2019).
Buchanan, T. W., Bagley, S. L., Stansfield, R. B. & Preston, S. D. The empathic, physiological resonance of stress. Soc. Neurosci. 7, 191–201 (2012).
Engert, V., Plessow, F., Miller, R., Kirschbaum, C. & Singer, T. Cortisol increase in empathic stress is modulated by emotional closeness and observation modality. Psychoneuroendocrinology 45, 192–201 (2014).
Boss, P. Family stress: Perception and context. In Sussman, M. & Steinmetz, S. (Eds.), Handbook on marriage and the family (pp. 695-723). (New York, Plenum Press, 1987)
Larson, R. W., & Almeida, D. M. Emotional transmission in the daily lives of families: A new paradigm for studying family process. J. Marriage Family, 5-20. https://doi.org/10.2307/353879. (1999)
Cummings, E. M. & Davies, P. T. Effects of marital conflict on children: Recent advances and emerging themes in process‐oriented research. J. child Psychol. psychiatry 43, 31–63 (2002).
Matjasko, J. L., & Feldman, A. F. Emotional transmission between parents and adolescents: The importance of work characteristics and relationship quality. In Schneider, B. & Waite, L. J. (Eds.), Being together, working apart: Dual-career families and the work-life balance (pp. 138–158). (Cambridge University Press, 2005)
Feldman, R. Parent–infant synchrony: Biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Curr. directions psychological Sci. 16, 340–345 (2007).
Field, T. M. Infant gaze aversion and heart rate during face-to-face interactions. Infant Behav. Dev. 4, 307–315 (1981).
Sethre-Hofstad, L., Stansbury, K. & Rice, M. A. Attunement of maternal and child adrenocortical response to child challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology 27, 731–747 (2002).
Papp, L. M., Pendry, P. & Adam, E. K. Mother-adolescent physiological synchrony in naturalistic settings: Within-family cortisol associations and moderators. J. Fam. Psychol. 23, 882–894 (2009).
Pratt, M. et al. Mother-child adrenocortical synchrony; Moderation by dyadic relational behavior. Horm. Behav. 89, 167–175 (2017).
Atkinson, L. et al. Maternal sensitivity and infant and mother adrenocortical function across challenges. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 2943–2951 (2013).
Ruttle, P. L., Serbin, L. A., Stack, D. M., Schwartzman, A. E. & Shirtcliff, E. A. Adrenocortical attunement in mother-child dyads: Importance of situational and behavioral characteristics. Biol. Psychol. 88, 104–111 (2011).
Waters, S. F., West, T. V., Karnilowicz, H. R. & Mendes, W. B. Affect contagion between mothers and infants: Examining valence and touch. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 146, 1043–1051 (2017).
Waters, S. F., West, T. V. & Mendes, W. B. Stress contagion: Physiological covariation between mothers and infants. Psychol. Sci. 25, 934–942 (2014).
Blasberg, J. U., Jost, J., Kanske, P., & Engert, V. Empathic stress in the mother-child dyad: Boys show greater cortisol and emotional empathic stress reactivity than girls. J. Exp. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001430 (2023).
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M. & Hellhammer, D. H. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’—A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–81 (1993).
Charmandari, E., Tsigos, C. & Chrousos, G. Endocrinology of the stress response. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 67, 259–284 (2005).
Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J. & Shirtcliff, E. A. The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1562–1592 (2011).
Buttelmann, F., & Karbach, J. Development and plasticity of cognitive flexibility in early and middle childhood [Mini Review]. Front. Psychol. 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01040 (2017).
Finch, J. E. Do schools promote executive functions? Differential working memory growth across school-year and summer months. AERA Open, 5, 2332858419848443. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858419848443 (2019).
Nelson, T. D. et al. Executive control goes to school: Implications of preschool executive performance for observed elementary classroom learning engagement. Dev. Psychol. 53, 836–844 (2017).
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R. & Heim, C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 434–445 (2009).
Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A. & Yonelinas, A. P. The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neurosci. Biobehav Rev. 68, 651–668 (2016).
Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168 (2013).
Baddeley, A. Working Memory. Science 255, 556–559 (1992).
Seehagen, S., Schneider, S., Rudolph, J., Ernst, S. & Zmyj, N. Stress impairs cognitive flexibility in infants. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 12882–12886 (2015).
Quesada, A. A., Wiemers, U. S., Schoofs, D. & Wolf, O. T. Psychosocial stress exposure impairs memory retrieval in children. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 125–136 (2012).
Harms, M. B., Shannon Bowen, K. E., Hanson, J. L. & Pollak, S. D. Instrumental learning and cognitive flexibility processes are impaired in children exposed to early life stress. Developmental Sci. 21, e12596 (2018).
Kalia, V. & Knauft, K. Emotion regulation strategies modulate the effect of adverse childhood experiences on perceived chronic stress with implications for cognitive flexibility. PLOS ONE 15, e0235412 (2020).
Kalia, V., Knauft, K. & Hayatbini, N. Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) associated with reduced cognitive flexibility in both college and community samples. PLOS ONE 16, e0260822 (2021).
Doan, S. N. & Evans, G. W. Maternal responsiveness moderates the relationship between allostatic load and working memory. Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 873–880 (2011).
Evans, G. W. & Fuller-Rowell, T. E. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and young adult working memory: The protective role of self-regulatory capacity. Developmental Sci. 16, 688–696 (2013).
Evans, G. W. & Schamberg, M. A. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 6545–6549 (2009).
Marko, M. & Riečanský, I. Sympathetic arousal, but not disturbed executive functioning, mediates the impairment of cognitive flexibility under stress. Cognition 174, 94–102 (2018).
Arnsten, A. F. T. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 410–422 (2009).
Liston, C. et al. Stress-induced alterations in prefrontal cortical dendritic morphology predict selective impairments in perceptual attentional set-shifting. J. Neurosci. 26, 7870–7874 (2006).
Obradović, J. Physiological responsivity and executive functioning: Implications for adaptation and resilience in early childhood. Child Dev. Perspect. 10, 65–70 (2016).
American Psychological Association. Stress in America™ 2020: A National Mental Health Crisis (Stress in America, Issue. American Psychological Association. https://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2020/sia-mental-health-crisis.pdf (2020).
Nochaiwong, S. et al. Global prevalence of mental health issues among the general population during the coronavirus disease-2019 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci. Rep. 11, 10173 (2021).
Kajantie, E. & Phillips, D. I. The effects of sex and hormonal status on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 151–178 (2006).
Buske-Kirschbaum, A. et al. Attenuated free cortisol response to psychosocial stress in children with atopic dermatitis. Psychosom. Med 59, 419–426 (1997).
Tousignant, B., Eugène, F. & Jackson, P. L. A developmental perspective on the neural bases of human empathy. Infant Behav. Dev. 48, 5–12 (2017).
Fries, E., Dettenborn, L. & Kirschbaum, C. The cortisol awakening response (CAR): Facts and future directions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 72, 67–73 (2009).
Mayr, U. & Kliegl, R. Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 26, 1124–1140 (2000).
Kirchner, W. K. Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 352–358 (1958).
Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. Ten years of research with the Trier Social Stress Test—Revisited. In Harmon-Jones, E. & Winkielman, P. (Eds.), Social neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior (pp. 56-83). (New York, NY, The Guilford Press, 2007).
Tukey, J. W. Exploratory data analysis. (Boston, MA, Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 1977)
Haatveit, B. C. et al. The validity of d prime as a working memory index: Results from the “Bergen n-back task. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 32, 871–880 (2010).
Grassi, G. et al. Heart rate as marker of sympathetic activity. J. Hypertens. 16, 1635–1639 (1998).
Dressendörfer, R. A., Kirschbaum, C., Rohde, W., Stahl, F. & Strasburger, C. J. Synthesis of a cortisol-biotin conjugate and evaluation as a tracer in an immunoassay for salivary cortisol measurement. J. Steroid Biochem Mol. Biol. 43, 683–692 (1992).
Adam, E. K. & Kumari, M. Assessing salivary cortisol in large-scale, epidemiological research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1423–1436 (2009).
Reifman, A., & Keyton, K. Winsorize. In Salkind, N. J. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of research design (pp. 1636-1638). (Thousand Oaks, CA, Sage, 2010)
Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G. & Hellhammer, D. H. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 916–931 (2003).
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T. & Mermelstein, R. A Global Measure of Perceived Stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385 (1983).
White, B. P. The Perceived Stress Scale for Children: A Pilot Study in a Sample of 153 Children. Int. J. Pediatrics Child Health 2, 45–52 (2014).
Overgaauw, S., Rieffe, C., Broekhof, E., Crone, E. A. & Güroğlu, B. Assessing Empathy across Childhood and Adolescence: Validation of the Empathy Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents (EmQue-CA). Front. Psychol. 8, 870 (2017).
Aron, A., Aron, E. N. & Smollan, D. Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 63, 596–612 (1992).
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. In R Foundation for Statistical Computing https://www.R-project.org (2022).
Miller, R., Plessow, F., Kirschbaum, C. & Stalder, T. Classification criteria for distinguishing cortisol responders from nonresponders to psychosocial stress: Evaluation of salivary cortisol pulse detection in panel designs. Psychosom. Med 75, 832–840 (2013).
Muse, L. A., Harris, S. G. & Feild, H. S. Has the inverted-U theory of stress and job performance had a fair test? Hum. Perform. 16, 349–364 (2003).
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B. & Christensen, R. H. B. lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26 (2017).
Canty, A., Ripley, B. D. boot: Bootstrap R (S-Plus) Functions. R package version 1.3-28.1. (2022)
Davison, A. C., Hinkley, D. V. Bootstrap Methods and Their Applications. (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge). https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511802843 (1997).
García-Pérez, M. A. Use and misuse of corrections for multiple testing. Methods Psychol. 8, 100120 (2023).
Heitz, R. P. The speed-accuracy tradeoff: history, physiology, methodology, and behavior. Front. Neurosci. 8, 150 (2014).
Plessow, F., Kiesel, A. & Kirschbaum, C. The stressed prefrontal cortex and goal-directed behaviour: Acute psychosocial stress impairs the flexible implementation of task goals. Exp. Brain Res 216, 397–408 (2012).
Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J. F., Luo, J. & Fernández, G. Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 25–32 (2009).
Yerkes, R. M. & Dodson, J. D. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Comp. Neurol. Psychol. 18, 459–482 (1908).
Corbett, M. From law to folklore: work stress and the Yerkes-Dodson Law. J. Manag. Psychol. 30, 741–752 (2015).
Chajut, E. & Algom, D. Selective attention improves under stress: Implications for theories of social cognition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 85, 231–248 (2003).
Roos, L. E. et al. Acute stress impairs children’s sustained attention with increased vulnerability for children of mothers reporting higher parenting stress. Developmental Psychobiol. 62, 532–543 (2020).
Masarik, A. S. & Conger, R. D. Stress and child development: A review of the Family Stress Model. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 13, 85–90 (2017).
Orekhova, E. Personality and Parental Bonding in Stress Reactivity and Chronic Stress (Doctoral dissertation, University of Roehampton). https://pure.roehampton.ac.uk/portal/files/411253/orekhova_elizabet.pdf (2014).
de Cock, E. S. A. et al. Longitudinal Associations Between Parental Bonding, Parenting Stress, and Executive Functioning in Toddlerhood. J. Child Fam. Stud. 26, 1723–1733 (2017).
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M. & Matte-Gagné, C. Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: A closer look at the caregiving environment. Developmental Sci. 15, 12–24 (2012).
Lin, L., Leung, A. W. S., Wu, J. & Zhang, L. Individual differences under acute stress: Higher cortisol responders performs better on N-back task in young men. Int. J. Psychophysiol.: Off. J. Int. Organ. Psychophysiol. 150, 20–28 (2020).
Gaab, J., Rohleder, N., Nater, U. M. & Ehlert, U. Psychological determinants of the cortisol stress response: the role of anticipatory cognitive appraisal. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 599–610 (2005).
Harvey, A., Nathens, A. B., Bandiera, G. & LeBlanc, V. R. Threat and challenge: cognitive appraisal and stress responses in simulated trauma resuscitations. Med. Educ. 44, 587–594 (2010).
Henze, G. I. et al. Testing the ecological validity of the Trier Social Stress Test: Association with real-life exam stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 75, 52–55 (2017).
Knauft, K., Waldron, A., Mathur, M. & Kalia, V. Perceived chronic stress influences the effect of acute stress on cognitive flexibility. Sci. Rep. 11, 23629 (2021).
Bretherton, I. The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth. Developmental Psychol. 28, 759–775 (1992).
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. Psychotherapy for Borderline Personality Disorder. (Oxford University Press). https://doi.org/10.1093/med:psych/9780198527664.001.0001 (2004).
Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E. & Seeman, T. E. Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bull. 128, 330–366 (2002).
Evans, G. W. & English, K. The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Dev. 73, 1238–1248 (2002).
Troxel, W. M., Matthews, K. A., Bromberger, J. T. & Sutton-Tyrrell, K. Chronic stress burden, discrimination, and subclinical carotid artery disease in African American and Caucasian women. Health Psychol. : Off. J. Div. Health Psychol., Am. Psychological Assoc. 22, 300–309 (2003).
Shonkoff, J. P. & Garner, A. S. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 129, e232–e246 (2012).
Acknowledgements
This study was funded by a project grant to Veronika Engert (EN 859/3-1) and Philipp Kanske (KA 4412/5-1) from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. We thank Klara Steinberg for her valuable input and the Social Stress and Family Health Research Group at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, particularly Henrik Grunert, Elisabeth Murzik and Silvia Tydecks, for their support with running the study.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
V.E. and P.K. secured the funding and designed the experiment. V.E. planned the study and was responsible for study execution. J.U.B. supported data preparation. J.U.B. and J.R. supported data analysis. E.L. and I.G.L. analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. V.E. supported the drafting of the manuscript. All authors critically revised the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications psychology thanks Madeline Harms and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editor: Jennifer Bellingtier. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Lashani, E., Larsen, I.G., Kanske, P. et al. Witnessing their mother’s acute and prolonged stress affects executive functioning in children. Commun Psychol 2, 98 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-024-00150-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-024-00150-0