Abstract
The experience of emotion is a form of meaning-making: it reveals one’s relationship to the circumstances. Often, the emphasis is on the emotions explicitly named or subjective feelings conveyed. In this perspective, we argue that psychology should use a broader set of tools to study emotional meaning in language. We put forward three sets of language features that capture: the contextual features or aspects of experience salient at each moment (attention); the conceptual vantage point which from events are viewed (construal); the evaluation of events along relevant dimensions (appraisal). We explain how each of these language features can be used to answer specific questions about emotional meaning-making and how it varies based on situation, person, and culture. Our interdisciplinary approach—grounded in socio-, cognitive, and computational linguistics as well as discursive, cognitive, and emotion psychology—seeks to move the field to a higher-dimensional, dynamical account of emotional meaning.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The experience of emotion is a form of meaning-making. Instances of disappointment, frustration, relief, and other emotion categories are mental events that reflect one’s relationship to the circumstances1. This relationship entails a particular focus or locus of attention, a particular perspective on the event, and a particular set of goals and values against which it is assessed. Disappointment, for an English speaker in North America, might imply that hopes were high, effort was great, or outcomes important. The focus would be on the expectation that was not met. Frustration, carrying similar implications, would focus instead on the thwarted intentions. Experiencing one emotion as opposed to another (or as opposed to none) is meaningful because each indicates a different position with respect to a given context2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9. Emotional meaning is positional in nature. It foregrounds certain concerns and backgrounds the rest.
Experiences of emotion are multidimensional and inherently situated. These experiences are typically described in terms of subjective feelings, yet they also encompass a wide array of contextual features—thoughts and desires; sensations and movements; surroundings and interactions; past and future events—all of which have bearing on the meaning-making process10,11,12,13,14. An instance of disappointment does not happen in a vacuum, but with reference to a constellation of expectations, behaviors, and social repercussions. These features vary in their identity and in their saliency. No two instances of disappointment are identical, not even within the same person15,16,17,18,19,20. ‘Dinner not going to plan’ is different when the meal is a reception for dozens versus an experiment for one; in one case, there are observers and potential threats to reputation, in the other there are not. And both cases are different from the disappointment of ‘passed over for a much-anticipated promotion.’ The meaning of emotion is made in each moment.
This meaning can be articulated in language. Psychology has a long history of using language to study emotional meaning-making (for recent reviews, see21,22,23). Often, the emphasis is on the emotions explicitly named (e.g., “I am disappointed”) or on the primary dimensions of affect (in)directly conveyed24 (e.g., disappointment is an unpleasant experience). These approaches have yielded valuable insights into the words that individuals and cultural groups use for emotions25,26,27,28,29,30,31, and how language relates to subjective experience, well-being and health32,33,34 (for a review, see ref. 35). Yet language is more than a collection of labels or a vehicle for subjective feeling36,37,38. The contextual features that comprise the experience of emotion can also be gleaned from language, expanding the dimensionality of situated meaning-making.
In this perspective, we argue that psychology should use a broader set of tools to study the construction of emotional meaning in language. We put forward three sets of language features that capture: the contextual features or aspects of experience salient at each moment (attention); the conceptual vantage point which from events are viewed (construal); the evaluation of events along relevant dimensions (appraisal). We explain in turn how each of these language features can be used to answer specific questions about emotional meaning-making and how it varies based on situation, person, and culture. These questions are ideally answered using natural language—the words, sentences, and stories organically generated by people as they speak and write, constrained only by the pragmatics of where, with whom, and about what they are communicating. These characteristics make natural language a rich and ubiquitous source of information about psychological processes, without requiring these processes to be introspected or reported upon directly39,40,41. We hypothesize that the patterns of attention, construal, and appraisal revealed by language will correspond with momentary fluctuations in emotion as well as with differences in how experiences of emotion play out over time for individuals and groups (Figs. 1 and 2). Our approach to emotional meaning-making is constructionist and profoundly interdisciplinary. We conclude by considering its implications and next steps.
a Potential attentional foci during an example event (blue circles): The woman notices the warmth from the sun (a), the hunger pangs in her stomach (b), the sounds of the baby in the stroller (c), and the smell of grass and flowers (d). b How the event could be conceptualized by the speaker along six possible dimensions (purple dots): dynamism (entities vs processes; 1), distance (proximal vs distal; 2), boundedness (ongoing vs finite; 3), reality status (real vs hypothetical; 4), speaker agency (subject vs object; 5), and location of emotion (self vs world; 6). c How the event could be holistically evaluated along three possible dimensions (orange dots): valence (i), intensity (ii), and certainty (iii). ‘Park graphic black white city landscape sketch illustration vector’ by Aluna1 is licensed under the iStock standard license.
Language features that capture emotional meaning-making
Attention
An analysis of natural language can be used to examine what people talk about when they talk about emotion—beyond the emotion, per se. Among the rich tapestry of features that comprise emotional experience, not all are equally dominant. Content, as well as style and structure, can say something about which features of experience have been picked out. Speakers may attend to bodily sensations, cognitive processes, or the actions and perspectives of others, and these attentional shifts can be captured by the types of words used21. One description of frustration may anchor on repressing the urge to yell or be cross, while another may anchor on the (in)actions of another. Patterns of word use can also represent the activities and contexts encountered42, or the motifs that structure people’s understanding of events43. Frustration could occur during a game night with friends, a political conversation with family, or a project meeting with coworkers, and might be interpreted along the same lines in each (e.g., ‘no one ever listens to me’). These broad dimensions of meaning can be said to represent attentional themes. Themes are salient and/or recurrent relationships between words in texts. These relationships do not have to be between contiguous words (e.g., collocations); they can be distributions that emerge from a collection of texts, or key words or phrases that reappear throughout a single text. We outline three approaches to identifying attentional themes in language with bearing on emotion.
One way of uncovering the salient dimensions of meaning in natural language is to look for words falling into pre-specified categories or dictionaries. In this approach, researchers define, a priori, the words and dictionaries of interest, and count the percentage of dictionary words that appear in text. Commonly, this is accomplished using the software program Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC)44. There are default LIWC dictionaries that capture content about aspects of experience relevant to emotion. For example, in English-speaking Americans, family-related words (e.g., “mom,” “dad,” “parent”) have been associated with more pleasant experiences, whereas work-related words (e.g., “class,” “work”, “school”) have been associated with less pleasant experiences45. In African and East Asian cultural contexts, emotional events are described using more embodied (e.g., “dizzy”) and/or more social (e.g., “friends”) words than in other cultural contexts, such as the US46,47,48. Use of first-person singular (“I”) versus other types of pronouns varies across languages, too49; it also varies across persons50 and within persons over time51,52, suggesting that it indexes, at each level of analysis, the extent to which the individual self receives the focus of attention (e.g., as an agent or experiencer), or whether this attention is directed elsewhere or more broadly distributed53. The use of standardized dictionaries facilitates comparison of attentional themes across texts, samples, and even languages54. At the same time, dictionary-based methods are limited to sets of words that are fixed top-down and selected to meet theoretical assumptions55.
An alternative approach is to work bottom-up, using discovery-oriented methods of generating attentional themes based on the co-occurrences of words and phrases56. This approach, broadly referred to as topic modeling57,58,59, captures the distributions that naturally occur in spontaneous language use, offering flexible descriptions of the themes that emerge in context. Themes extracted from the everyday speech of English-speaking university students in the US showed, for instance, that talking about entertainment (e.g., “game,” “play,” “watching”) coincided with more pleasant experiences, while talking about assignments (e.g., “test,” “study,” “class”) coincided with less pleasant experiences45. At the between-persons level, an analysis of daily diaries from US English speakers and Belgian Dutch speakers showed that people who reported more differentiated negative emotions also referred to a greater variety of attentional themes, suggesting more experiential diversity in their everyday lives42. Topic modeling avoids imposing expectations of how meaning is made, making it useful for cross-cultural or cross-linguistic research60,61,62. For example, both US English speakers and Belgian Dutch speakers attend to social relationships, communication, and professional/scholastic concerns when describing emotional events, but diverge in their focus on personal achievement (US English speakers) versus collaboration (Belgian Dutch speakers)54. Topic modeling is data-driven, rather than hypothesis-driven; this also means that attentional themes are subject to interpretation and may not generalize. These characteristics make topic modeling most appealing for exploratory or descriptive aims.
Another, distinct approach to uncovering attentional foci is to examine the themes or motifs that emerge across a text or set of texts. By motifs, we mean the central ideas or linguistic devices (e.g., phrases, structures) that guide and reflect conceptualization and communication43. Stories, for instance, may progress through a sequence of stages63, or traverse established narrative arcs64, in conveying thoughts and experiences to others. Motifs may be used at various junctures to tie points together, attract the listener’s attention, or reinforce key aspects65,66. For example, coming back to the idea of “there’s no way” to resolve an issue is a way of highlighting perceived futility and emotional fatigue43. Themes and motifs that recur over time, then, highlight habitual patterns of attention and thought, and can be predictive of psychological functioning67. These may not assume consistent forms or occur frequently enough to be studied using the word-counting and topic-modeling approaches described above, however, and so are often better identified using inductive techniques such as thematic, discourse, and narrative analysis68,69,70,71. Pre-established themes or motifs can also be manually annotated36,67. Both methods yield insights into how people navigate longer-form meaning-making (whether in writing or transcribed speech) and can provide a basis for further, automated analyses (e.g., via key words or phrases, or using machine learning algorithms trained on human-coded data72,73).
Construal
Construal refers to the situated way in which a speaker conceptualizes an event and represents that conceptualization in language74,75. If attention is, roughly, the “what,” construal is the “how.” It is, in short, perspective: from what physical or temporal vantage point is the event mentally viewed, what relationships hold between actors and the environment, or how the action unfolds8,76,77,78. A speaker may describe a past event as if it were currently in progress, highlight their own agency versus what was done to them by others, or consider what might have been. These phenomena have long been the subject of psychological research on construal level theory79,80,81,82 and are fundamental to a cognitive linguistic approach to language, in which construal is intrinsic to the process of meaning-making83,84. One does not understand oneself in relation to others and the world—just as one does not see—without adopting a point of view. The richness and inherent subjectivity of experience means that there is no single way to represent a situation, and each representation goes beyond the set of observable and non-observable features to make meaning in context85. Because of this, construal is expressed by a wide variety of lexical and grammatical resources. Here, we cover six dimensions of construal, their relevance to emotional meaning-making, and how they may be inferred from natural language.
First, events vary in the degree to which their constituents are framed as entities versus processes. Entities are static, conceptually autonomous ‘things,’ whereas processes are dynamic, unfolding in time. Examining emotional events along this dimension of construal may indicate when people perceive their experience as abstract and rigid versus embodied and flexible. In language, the distinction between entities and processes is conveyed by the word class used for the emotion76. Nouns (e.g., “anger”) and adjectives (e.g., “angry”) designate entities and their properties; verbs (e.g., “to anger”) and adverbs (e.g., “angrily”) designate processes and their properties. Consider the love in “his love for her is great” compared to the love in “he loves her greatly.” The difference goes beyond mere poetics86. Nouns are associated with categorical and essentialist thinking in US English87,88, while verbs are associated with a sense of bodily involvement and greater sensorimotor activation in Netherlandic Dutch and Metropolitan French89,90. Moreover, languages differ on which word classes are used to describe emotions and emotional experiences. Speakers of Hindustani spontaneously generate more verbs (e.g., “pyar karnar” [“to love”]) and fewer nouns (e.g., “pyar” [“love”]) than Dutch speakers91. Russian speakers prefer verbal constructions (e.g., “perezhivat” [“to suffer things through”]), whereas English prefers an adjectival construction (e.g., “upset”)92,93,94,95. The use of word classes is just now beginning to be systematically investigated across and within individuals96, suggesting implications for emotion regulation, mindfulness, and more97,98. For example, the use of verbs for emotion may be associated with beliefs about emotion malleability99, whereas the use of nouns may track with beliefs about immutability100,101.
The second and third dimensions of construal are closely related to each other. Namely, events can be conceptualized as temporally proximal versus distal and as actively unfolding versus complete. Each of these dimensions says something about events’ psychological immediacy or embeddedness102,103,104,105 and so may have bearing on emotional processing. Linguistically, these dimensions are indexed by the tense and aspect of verbs used to describe experiences of emotion. Verb tense locates the event before (i.e., past tense; e.g., “I loved”), concurrent with (i.e., present tense; e.g., “I love”), or after (i.e., future tense; e.g., “I will love”) the time of speaking. The use of past versus present verb tense, among other language features (e.g., first-person singular pronouns)—known as ‘linguistic distancing’—is associated with healthy emotion regulation strategies and better self-reported well-being in US English speakers52,73,106,107. Verb aspect, in turn, describes the event in terms of its parts and bounding108,109: as ongoing (i.e., imperfective aspect; e.g., “I love[d]”) or as finite (i.e., perfective aspect; e.g., “I have/had loved”). Imperfective aspect is associated with first- (versus third-) person perspective in US English and German110,111, resulting in heightened subjective awareness and emotional intensity112,113,114. Future work can examine the relative contributions of tense and aspect to emotional meaning-making and their adaptiveness in context. For instance, verb aspect may help to explain some apparent discrepancies in the therapeutic utility of linguistic distancing in US English (e.g., 107, vs115.) because it signals whether prior events are being construed as ongoing versus completed, regardless of tense (see also evidence from Chilean Spanish116).
Fourth, the emotional events represented by speakers can have multiple possible ‘reality’ statuses. They can be real or known, as when describing present or past experiences. They can also be hypothetical, unknown, or inconsistent with reality, as when describing wishes, desires, or imaginary situations. One form of this latter ‘reality’ status is counterfactuals, which reconstruct past events by revisiting what might or could have been117,118. Counterfactual thinking involves mentally simulating possible explanations119, facilitating conceptual integration120 and goal-directed behaviors121. For these reasons, it has been linked in US English speakers to successful emotion regulation and life satisfaction122,123. In language, counterfactual thinking can be identified by the use of verbs in the subjunctive mood (e.g., “if I were to see you, I would be happier”; a mood that is more common in other languages [e.g., Spanish, French] than in English) or by other modal or conditional constructions (e.g., “I would be happier if I saw you,” “I would be happier if I could see you”). A full examination of situated and (mal)adaptive meaning-making, we hypothesize, will involve looking at the types of counterfactual statements used and the contexts in which they occur. For example, temporal distance (as indexed by verb tense or aspect) may moderate the relationship between counterfactual thinking and rumination123. Counterfactuals may be used to explain contemporary or retrospective emotional events, providing insight into the complexity of mental representation5 and how the self is perceived over time and in relation to social others36,124.
A fifth dimension of construal with relevance to emotional meaning-making is the extent to which speakers see themselves as agentic or the locus of control as internal. Does someone say, “I fear dogs” or do they say, “dogs frighten me”? Evidence from US English and German shows that each phrasing carries a different attentional focus and perception of causality125,126, with implications for the volition and responsibility of the participants involved124,127. Speakers’ positioning of themselves and others on this dimension is driven by regulatory and rhetorical goals70,106, yet may be adaptive or maladaptive given the circumstances. For example, although constructions that convey diminished agency may be used by speakers of US English to preserve a sense of self-efficacy after the loss of a partner128, they are also associated with greater grief-related disturbances36 (see also refs. 129,130). Over time, these momentary conceptualizations may come to reflect more stable individual or cultural differences in self-construal131 or identity132. There are many features of language that convey agency (for a review, see ref. 36; see also ref. 71). One feature is the grammatical relation or semantic role played by the speaker: in the examples above, “I” is the subject or agent, whereas “me” is the [direct] object, patient, or recipient133,134. Another feature is the voice of the dominant verb. In passive voice constructions (e.g., “I was frightened by the dogs”), the grammatical object (“I”) is more salient due to its initial position, biasing readers of US English to see it as the primary actor135,136. Accounting for syntactic structure, then, can provide critical insight into how speakers understand and communicate their role in an emotional event4,37,78,93,137,138.
The sixth and final dimension of construal advanced in this perspective is where the emotion is located in relation to the speaker. Emotions can be experienced as a property of the self (e.g., “I’m annoyed”) and as a property of the world (e.g., “this is annoying”)139. These construals, like those of agency and control, may have implications for behavior and coping, and there may be individual and cultural differences in the tendency for these construals that lead to or interact with putative effects36,140. For instance, ‘owning’ one’s annoyance by locating it in the self, rather than in an interaction partner, may facilitate healthy communication strategies and reduce implications of blame in cultural contexts that value individual autonomy and self-assertion141. In cultural contexts where social functioning is foregrounded, such as when ‘honor’ should be maintained, emotions are located in the world142. The extent to which the experiencer versus the ‘stimulus’ is foregrounded corresponds with a continuum of syntactic structures143,144,145, with the poles of this continuum mapping onto the examples above. These syntactic structures could be the focus of future research into how speakers understand themselves as experiencers of emotion, and how this may vary systematically across cultures.
Appraisal
The term “appraisal” is used by theories of emotion2,3,9 and language4,146 to refer to dimensional evaluations that are thought to be central to emotional meaning-making, and indeed to meaning-making in general147. In psychological research, appraisals include dimensions of affect such as (un)pleasantness (i.e., valence) and intensity, and situational evaluations such as certainty and goal congruence148. They can also refer to higher-order assessments such as threat or loss9. In linguistic research, appraisals are the dispositions that speakers routinely (if not inherently) adopt toward the content of their own message and its context (i.e., the [emotional] event). This disposition—a stance or attitude149—is an evaluation of ongoing speech and circumstances that also serves an intersubjective function by signaling to the listener what to be regarded as important, obvious, unreliable, etc.150. For example, a speaker might indicate that they are more or less enthusiastic about what is currently happening, or that they are not sure about what they are saying. With both psychological and linguistic meanings of “appraisal” in mind, our recommendations focus on the dimensions most feasibly assessed through natural language: valence, intensity, and certainty. As reviewed below, there is much work on estimating valence from natural language and its variation across situations, individuals, and cultures. Considerably less work has examined intensity and certainty. While lexicometric (word-based) approaches can be used to estimate each of these appraisals, they also show up in lower-level (e.g., punctuation) and higher-level (e.g., phrasing) features of language. These represent underutilized resources for psychological science. The general approaches we outline for estimating intensity and certainty can also by applied toward other lower- (e.g., goal congruence) and higher-order (e.g., threat) appraisals in text.
Valence is a primary dimension of evaluation in many theories of appraisal and affect151,152,153,154. One common way of estimating valence in language is using dictionary-based methods such as LIWC that count the percentage of pleasantly- and unpleasantly-valenced words (for similar dictionaries, see155,156,157), or collections of words that have been assigned continuous valence ratings or weights158,159,160,161,162. Valence is also conveyed through punctuation (e.g., exclamation points)163, abbreviations (LMFO) emoji ☺, and emoticons :)—indeed, these features are leveraged by contemporary sentiment analysis algorithms to classify the attitude expressed in texts as positive or negative159,164. In US English and Belgian Dutch, use of valenced language can track with momentary self-reports165,166,167,168 (but see45,169,170). In English, valenced language has also been associated across persons with personality dimensions such as neuroticism (i.e., emotional [in]stability)171,172,173,174 and with mental and physical health outcomes such as happiness175, longevity176, and depression177,178,179. These relationships may not be universal, however, as there are notable cultural differences in the use of valenced language, for example between western (e.g., US) and non-western (e.g., India) English users60,180, or between White and Black English speakers in the United States181.
The intensity applied to a message or an event is a way of positioning that message or event relative to others of its kind. To be “not very disappointed” situates the current emotional experience as weaker than other past or hypothetical disappointments, beyond the simple label. For this reason, functional theories of language4,146 see intensity as a marker of qualified appraisal. In psychological studies, emotion is most often assessed by asking people to rate the intensity of their experience on a set of provided terms. This information can also be recovered from natural language by accounting for adverbs (e.g., “very”) or other modifying phrases (e.g., “kind of”), as they are used either throughout entire texts or in combination with emotion labels. Some emotion labels may be perceived as more intense than others (e.g., “furious” versus “annoyed”182), and this distinction could also be captured by dictionary-based methods. Orthographic features such as punctuation (!) and writing in ALL CAPS, corollaries of prosodic features (e.g., intonation, stress) in text, additionally impact perceived emotional intensity159,183. The advantage of assessing intensity in natural language is that its presence represents an expression on the part of the speaker, rather than a requirement of a rating scale (for discussion, see184,185).
Certainty, as a dimension of (linguistic) appraisal, refers to the confidence or conviction with which a speaker adopts a particular stance or attitude. Consider the distinction between “I’m clearly disappointed” and “I guess I’m disappointed,” or between “I’m always disappointed” and “I might be disappointed.” Looking at the emotion word alone fails to capture the full meaning of these statements and might even assume something that is not true (e.g., that the speaker is disappointed). Appraisals of high or low certainty are therefore foundational to emotional meaning-making. Like appraisals of intensity, these can be captured using dictionary-based methods; for example, both “clearly” and “always” are considered ‘certain’ language by LIWC, whereas “guess” and “might” are ‘tentative.’ Question marks and repeating letters to deliberately mimic spoken inflections (e.g., “weeeeell”) can likewise influence the degree of emotional certainty perceived in texts186. Appraisals can also manifest across longer stretches of text or speech, in ways that cannot be accounted for by word choice or orthography alone. Compare “I might feel bad about it” (i.e., I feel bad but do not want to say it directly) and “I might have felt bad about it” (i.e., I did not feel bad but could have if circumstances were different). Not only does the received valence flip (bad vs. not bad), but the type of thinking also shifts (factual vs. counterfactual). For this reason, appraisals of certainty (and other dimensions) are often manually annotated across phrases or sentences187,188. Recent work in linguistics demonstrates methods of increasing the transparency, reliability, and replicability of this high-value but high-effort task189,190,191.
Outlook
To fully understand the construction of emotional meaning, psychological language analysis must move beyond the simple act of labeling or the communication of feeling. There is so much more to learn about how speakers represent their experience in each moment and the role that language plays. Speakers—and scientists—have more than emotion words and sentiment at their disposal, so why do we not look at the larger repertoire in use? The continuous advancement of text-analytic techniques and natural language processing (NLP) capabilities makes this ever easier. In this perspective, we have focused on attention, construal, and appraisal as core processes of meaning-making in action. We illustrated the documented or potential relevance of each process for the science of emotion and then put forward sets of language features that have been shown, across other areas of psychology and linguistics, to capture it (summarized in Table 1). As we discuss next, these processes and features can help reframe our understanding of what emotions are, as well as our understanding of what language does. This reframing is consistent with a constructionist perspective on emotion and language and offers an interdisciplinary path forward for the study of emotion in context. We conclude by highlighting additional avenues for exploration and possibilities for the future study of emotion.
An approach to emotional meaning-making that accounts for the interrelated processes of attention, construal, and appraisal, and how these manifest in language, can reveal how the beliefs and interpretations that constitute emotional experience are assembled. Emotion and language are both systems of meaning. They transduce heterogeneous and continuous features into established formats, chunking and binning the raw experience of individuals according to categories and symbols agreed upon by a group. They are communicative in the sense that they are used to convey meaning to others192,193, but also in the sense that they are used to structure reality for oneself194,195,196. Practically, this means that the details matter. Variation in specific aspects—noting your bodily sensations rather than your social milieu, conveying the items on your to-do list as boring vs. yourself as bored, clarifying that you are not completely sure how you feel—is revealing because each option carries a different implication for experience and behavior. This approach is constructionist at its core because it holds that meaning (of any kind) is built from a complex and dynamic set of features that plays out against personal and sociocultural backgrounds124,197,198,199,200,201,202,203. Neither language nor emotion is a read-out of static and received meanings204,205,206,207. Instead, meanings are actively molded and negotiated in real time, as people accomplish situation-specific and interactional goals. It is at the level of these specific (interpersonal) events that meaning must be studied. Only at this level can we observe what utterances and emotions achieve208 for the people who construct them209.
Considering emotion and language in this light has the potential to contribute to theoretical frameworks for emotion and address key knowledge gaps. A central goal of many prominent emotion theories is the development of emotion typologies, according to which specific categories should be reliably distinguished on the basis of their concomitant behaviors, situations, appraisals, and more148,210,211,212,213,214. These typologies abstract away from the expansive variation and situated nuance of lived experiences by classifying them via labels25,26. Recent updates to typological theories describe a larger number of emotions with fuzzy rather than discrete boundaries, underscoring the high dimensionality of the semantic space of emotion215. Our approach is consistent with this emphasis on complexity yet departs from its inherent focus on categories. Instead of centering the emotion words people use (or categories that might otherwise be inferred from natural language), we propose an examination, via language, of the processes that support emotional meaning and experience. Rather than abstracting away from the details of lived experiences, attention, construal, and appraisal unfold over time and are deeply rooted in context. These characteristics expand our capacity for capturing the situational, personal, and cultural shaping of emotion and meaning that is too often neglected in emotion science11,216,217.
Achieving this goal entails the integration of concepts and methods from socio-, cognitive, and computational linguistics with discursive, cognitive, and emotion psychology. The empirical approach we have laid out in this perspective does this by targeting specific features of language and testing their relation to psychological mechanisms that give rise to emotion. In most cases, these language features can be identified automatically in text using existing software programs (e.g., LIWC) or NLP scripts in widely-used programming languages (e.g., Python, R). Manual annotation may still prove necessary for features corresponding with attentional motifs and appraisal dimensions, although automated tools are rapidly becoming available64,218. More generally, the kind of bottom-up, feature-based research that we have proposed is not the only way to approach psychological language analysis. It is also possible to work top-down, by first identifying a set of psychological features in data (i.e., through manual annotation) and then characterizing the consistent language features. This approach has a long history and is gaining traction through recent advances in explainable AI, where feature inputs are assessed for their contributions to otherwise opaque models (e.g., transformer-based neural networks like BERT219,220), and in generative AI, which enables computers to do qualitative labeling instead of humans221,222. Computer annotation of emotion, for example, is mostly of similar quality to human annotation223,224, with the major caveat that models are primarily trained on US English texts, and so give annotations consistent with an average American225,226. As such, both bottom-up and top-down approaches to psychological language analysis can contribute to explanations of process.
There are many potential expansions on the present recommendations, as well. A complete picture of emotion and language as systems of meaning requires situating them in context. This context can be physical, such as when emotion and language vary as a function of location227,228,229,230. This context can be social, based on who is communicating with whom, and their individual and relational characteristics—how old, extraverted, or close are they56,231,232,233; what power dynamics govern their interaction234,235,236,237, etc. Context can also be cultural, described in terms of the social and psychological patterns that organize shared reality238,239,240. The context of emotion and language becomes more multifaceted still when we examine them across different styles or registers187,241, in conversation70,242,243, or with reference to paralinguistic cues or channels of communication such as vocal intonation, facial movements, or gestures244,245,246,247. Truly dialogic and embedded research on how emotional meaning is made in language opens new horizons for future work, leading toward answers for basic and applied questions alike. If emotional meaning is assembled through continuous engagement with the physical, social, and linguistic environment, then shedding light on the assembly process can reveal patterns that confer vulnerability or resilience to disorders67,179,216,248 and may even enable us to intervene on (or bolster) these patterns. In this way, the present approach could ultimately inform clinical and translational aims to promote health and well-being.
References
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N. & Gross, J. J. The experience of emotion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 373–403 (2007).
Ellsworth, P. C. & Scherer, K. R. Appraisal processes in emotion. in Handbook of Affective Sciences 572–595 (2003).
Frijda, N. H. The Emotions (Cambridge University Press, 1986).
Halliday, M. A. K. An Introduction to Functional Grammar (Edward Arnold, 1994).
Hoemann, K., Gendron, M. & Barrett, L. F. Mixed emotions in the predictive brain. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 15, 51–57 (2017).
Josephs, I. E. The problem of emotions from the perspective of psychological semantics. Cult. Psychol. 1, 279–288 (1995).
Katz, J. M. Discrepancy, arousal and labeling: Towards a psychosocial theory of emotion. Sociol. Inq. 50, 147–156 (1980).
Langacker, R. W. Construal. in Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics Vol. 39 (eds Da̜browska, E. & Divjak, D.) 120–142 (Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co KG, 2015).
Lazarus, R. S. Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. Am. Psychol. 46, 819–834 (1991).
Barrett, L. F. How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain (Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 2017).
Barrett, L. F. Context reconsidered: complex signal ensembles, relational meaning, and population thinking in psychological science. Am. Psychol. 77, 894–920 (2022).
Skerry, A. E. & Saxe, R. Neural representations of emotion are organized around abstract event features. Curr. Biol. 25, 1945–1954 (2015).
Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., Simmons, W. K. & Barsalou, L. W. Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia 49, 1105–1127 (2011).
Winkielman, P., Coulson, S. & Niedenthal, P. Dynamic grounding of emotion concepts. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 373, 20170127 (2018).
Cunningham, W. A., Dunfield, K. A. & Stillman, P. E. Emotional states from affective dynamics. Emot. Rev. 5, 344–355 (2013).
Hoemann, K. Beyond linguistic relativity, emotion concepts illustrate how meaning is contextually and individually variable. Top. Cogn. Sci.12659. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12659 (2023).
Hoemann, K. et al. Context-aware experience sampling reveals the scale of variation in affective experience. Sci. Rep. 10, 12459 (2020).
Kuppens, P., Van Mechelen, I., Smits, D. J. & De Boeck, P. The appraisal basis of anger: specificity, necessity and sufficiency of components. Emotion 3, 254–269 (2003).
McVeigh, K., Kleckner, I. R., Quigley, K. S. & Satpute, A. B. Fear-related psychophysiological patterns are situation and individual dependent: a Bayesian model comparison approach. Emotion 24, 506–521 (2024).
Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F. & Barsalou, L. W. Variety in emotional life: within-category typicality of emotional experiences is associated with neural activity in large-scale brain networks. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 62–71 (2015).
Boyd, R. L. & Schwartz, H. A. Natural language analysis and the psychology of verbal behavior: the past, present, and future states of the field. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 40, 21–41 (2021).
Jackson, J. C. et al. From text to thought: How analyzing language can advance psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17, 805–826 (2022).
Lindquist, K. A. Language and emotion: introduction to the special issue. Affec. Sci. 2, 91–98 (2021).
Feuerriegel, S. et al. Using natural language processing to analyse text data in behavioural science. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 4, 96–111 (2025).
Cowen, A. S. & Keltner, D. Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 114, E7900–E7909 (2017).
Cowen, A. S. et al. How emotion is experienced and expressed in multiple cultures: a large-scale experiment across North America, Europe, and Japan. Front. Psychol. 15, 1350631 (2024).
Davitz, J. R. The Language of Emotion (Academic Press, 1969).
Jackson, J. C. et al. Emotion semantics show both cultural variation and universal structure. Science 366, 1517–1522 (2019).
Ogarkova, A. Translatability of emotions. in Emotion Measurement 575–599. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-100508-8.00023-0 (Elsevier, 2016).
Russell, J. A. Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychol. Bull. 110, 426–450 (1991).
Wierzbicka, A. Emotion, language, and cultural scripts. in Emotion and Culture: Empirical Studies of Mutual Influence (eds Kitayama, S. & Markus, H. R.) 133–196 (American Psychological Association, 1994).
Ottenstein, C. & Lischetzke, T. Development of a novel method of emotion differentiation that uses open-ended descriptions of momentary affective states. Assessment 107319111983913. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191119839138 (2019).
Vine, V., Boyd, R. L. & Pennebaker, J. W. Natural emotion vocabularies as windows on distress and well-being. Nat. Commun. 11, 4525 (2020).
Williams, G. E. & Uliaszek, A. A. Measuring negative emotion differentiation via coded descriptions of emotional experience. Assessment 29, 1144–1157 (2022).
Hoemann, K. What’s in an emotion word? The multiple roles of labeling in emotional functioning and well-being. in Psychology of Learning and Motivation Vol. 80, 33–72 (Elsevier, 2024).
Capps, L. & Bonanno, G. Narrating bereavement: Thematic and grammatical predictors of adjustment to loss. discourse process. - discourse process 30, 1–25 (2000). Diminished self-agency in interviews with bereaved individuals at 6 months post-loss predicted grief-related symptoms at 2 years post-loss beyond negative content, demonstrating the importance of analyzing grammatical structuring of personal narratives.
Majid, A. Current emotion research in the language sciences. Emot. Rev. 4, 432–443 (2012).
Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. Language has a heart. Text 9, 7–25 (1989).
Gottschalk, L. A. & Gleser, G. The Measurement of Psychological States through the Content Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (University of California Press, 1969).
Tausczik, Y. R. & Pennebaker, J. W. The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 29, 24–54 (2010).
Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol. Rev. 84, 231 (1977).
Hoemann, K., Lee, Y., Kuppens, P., Gendron, M. & Boyd, R. L. Emotional granularity is associated with daily experiential diversity. Affec. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-023-00185-2 (2023).
Romano, M. Evaluation in emotion narratives. in Pragmatics & Beyond New Series Vol. 242 (eds. Thompson, G. & Alba-Juez, L.) 367–386 (John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2014). Qualitative discourse analysis of emotion narratives shows how recurrent linguistic and pragmatic strategies are strategically deployed to highlight speakers’ perspectives and key personal information.
Pennebaker, J. W. & Francis, M. E. Cognitive, emotional, and language processes in disclosure. Cogn. Emot. 10, 601–626 (1996).
Sun, J., Kern, M. L., Schwartz, H. A., Son, Y. & Vazire, S. The language of well-being: tracking fluctuations in emotion experience through everyday speech. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 118, 364–387 (2019). Estimates of pleasantness and unpleasantness derived from Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) dictionaries were not associated with self-reported experience, whereas other LIWC- and data-derived themes (e.g., entertainment, work) were, illustrating what can be learned about everyday well-being by looking beyond words for emotion and affect.
Choi, E., Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E. & Parrott, W. G. The effectiveness of somatization in communicating distress in Korean and American cultural contexts. Front. Psychol. 7, 383 (2016).
Dzokoto, V. A., Opare-Henaku, A. & Kpobi, L. A. Somatic referencing and psychologisation in emotion narratives: a USA–Ghana comparison. Psychol. Dev. Soc. 25, 311–331 (2013).
Tsai, J. L., Simeonova, D. I. & Watanabe, J. T. Somatic and social: Chinese Americans talk about emotion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 1226–1238 (2004). Less acculturated Chinese Americans used more somatic and social language when describing emotional events in English than European Americans, suggesting culture-specific patterns of attention related to emotion.
Uz, I. Individualism and first person pronoun use in written texts across languages. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 45, 1671–1678 (2014).
Brockmeyer, T. et al. Me, myself, and I: self-referent word use as an indicator of self-focused attention in relation to depression and anxiety. Front. Psychol. 6, 1564 (2015). First-person singular pronouns in natural language related in context-sensitive ways to symptoms of depression and anxiety in German-speaking adults, consistent with the hypothesis that they index (maladaptive) self-focus.
Shahane, A. D., Godfrey, D. A. & Denny, B. T. Predicting real-world emotion and health from spontaneously assessed linguistic distancing using novel scalable technology. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001211 (2023).
Orvell, A., Ayduk, Ö., Moser, J. S., Gelman, S. A. & Kross, E. Linguistic shifts: a relatively effortless route to emotion regulation? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 567–573 (2019).
Gudykunst, W. & Wiseman, R. L. Anxiety/uncertainty management (AUM) theory. in Intercultural Communication Theory (Sage, Thousand Oaks, 1995).
Hoemann, K., Şencan, R. S., Cochez, A., Beckers, C. & Mesquita de, B. G. Cultural models of emotion manifest in descriptions of everyday experience: a case study of the US and Belgium. Preprint at https://osf.io/mtd4a (2024).
Schwartz, H. A. & Ungar, L. H. Data-driven content analysis of social media: a systematic overview of automated methods. ANNALS Am. Acad. Political Soc. Sci. 659, 78–94 (2015).
Schwartz, H. A. et al. Personality, gender, and age in the language of social media: the open-vocabulary approach. PLoS ONE 8, e73791 (2013).
Günther, F., Rinaldi, L. & Marelli, M. Vector-space models of semantic representation from a cognitive perspective: a discussion of common misconceptions. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 1006–1033 (2019).
Kamiloğlu, R. G., Türkmen, İ. U., Sarnıç, T. E., Landman, D. & Sauter, D. What makes us feel good? A data-driven investigation of positive emotion experience. Emotion 25, 271–276 (2025).
Wilson, S., Mihalcea, R., Boyd, R. & Pennebaker, J. W. Disentangling topic models: a cross-cultural analysis of personal values through words. in Proceedings of the First Workshop on NLP and Computational Social Science 143–152. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/W16-5619.(Association for Computational Linguistics, 2016).
De Choudhury, M., Sharma, S. S., Logar, T., Eekhout, W. & Nielsen, R. C. Gender and cross-cultural differences in social media disclosures of mental illness. in Proceedings of the 2017 ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work and Social Computing 353–369 (ACM, 2017). doi:10/gmzfzm. Data-driven analyses of natural language reveal that English speakers from India and South Africa were less exaggerated in their expression of negative emotions and thoughts related to depression than English speakers from the US and UK, suggesting that linguistic hallmarks of affective disorder vary across cultures.
Ramírez-Esparza, N., Chung, C. K., Sierra-Otero, G. & Pennebaker, J. W. Cross-cultural constructions of self-schemas: Americans and Mexicans. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 43, 233–250 (2012).
Rodríguez-Arauz, G., Ramírez-Esparza, N., Pérez-Brena, N. & Boyd, R. L. Hablo inglés y español: cultural self-schemas as a function of language. Front. Psychol. 8, 885 (2017).
Labov, W. Oral narratives of personal experience. in Cambridge Encyclopedia of the Language Sciences (ed. Hogan, P. C.) 1–10 (Cambridge University Press, 2010).
Boyd, R. L., Blackburn, K. G. & Pennebaker, J. W. The narrative arc: revealing core narrative structures through text analysis. Sci. Adv. 6, eaba2196 (2020).
Dancygier, B. Narrative anchors and the processes of story construction: the case of Margaret Atwood’s The Blind Assassin. Style 41, 133–151 (2007).
Tannen, D. Talking Voices: Repetition, Dialogue, and Imagery in Conversational Discourse (Cambridge University Press, 1989).
Bonanno, G. A., Mihalecz, M. C. & Lejeune, J. T. The core emotion themes of conjugal loss. Motiv. Emot. 23, 175–201 (1999).
Braun, V. & Clarke, V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101 (2006).
Braun, V. & Clarke, V. Conceptual and design thinking for thematic analysis. Qual. Psychol. 9, 3–26 (2022).
Edwards, D. Emotion discourse. Cult. Psychol. 5, 271–291 (1999).
Kleres, J. Emotions and narrative analysis: a methodological approach. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 41, 182–202 (2011).
Saravani, S. M., Ghaffari, S., Luther, Y., Folkestad, J. & Moraes, M. Automated code extraction from discussion board text dataset. International Conference on Quantitative Ethnography 227–238 (2022).
Shahane, A. D., Pham, D. C., Lopez, R. B. & Denny, B. T. Novel computational algorithms to index lexical markers of psychological distancing and their relationship to emotion regulation efficacy over time. Affec. Sci. 2, 262–272 (2021).
Divjak, D., Milin, P. & Medimorec, S. Construal in language: a visual-world approach to the effects of linguistic alternations on event perception and conception. Cogn. Linguist. 31, 37–72 (2020).
Verhagen, A. Construal and perspectivization. in The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (eds Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H.) 48–81 (Oxford University Press, 2007).
Langacker, R. W. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar: Theoretical Prerequisites Vol. 1 (Stanford University Press, 1987).
Talmy, L. Figure and ground in complex sentences. in Universals of Human Language Vol. 4 (eds Greenberg, J. H., Ferguson, C. & Moravcsik, J.) 625–649 (Stanford University Press, 1978).
Talmy, L. Toward a Cognitive Semantics Vol. 1 (MIT Press, 2000).
Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 117, 440–463 (2010).
Liberman, N. & Trope, Y. Traversing psychological distance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 364–369 (2014).
Liberman, N. & Trope, Y. The psychology of transcending the here and now. Science 322, 1201–1205 (2008).
Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. Temporal construal. Psychol. Rev. 110, 403–421 (2003).
Croft, W. & Cruse, D. A. Cognitive Linguistics (Cambridge University Press, 2004).
Paluck, E. L. & Shafir, E. The psychology of construal in the design of field experiments. in Handbook of economic field experiments. Vol. 1, 245–268 (Elsevier, 2017).
Bruner, J. S. Going beyond the information given. Contemp. Approaches Cogn. 1, 119–160 (1957).
Waxman, S. R. & Gelman, S. A. Different kinds of concepts and different kinds of words: What words do for human cognition. in The Making of Human Concepts (eds Mareschal, D., Quinn, P. C. & Lea, S. E. G.) 101–130 (Oxford University Press, 2010).
Rhodes, M., Leslie, S.-J. & Tworek, C. M. Cultural transmission of social essentialism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 13526–13531 (2012).
Rhodes, M., Cardarelli, A. & Leslie, S.-J. Asking young children to “do science” instead of “be scientists” increases science engagement in a randomized field experiment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 117, 9808–9814 (2020).
Foroni, F. & Semin, G. R. Language that puts you in touch with your bodily feelings: the multimodal responsiveness of affective expressions. Psychol. Sci. 20, 974–980 (2009).
Freddi, S., Esteban, J. & Dru, V. Emotions and language about motion: differentiating affective dominance with syntax from valence with semantics. Conscious. Cogn. 38, 22–37 (2015). Ratings of experienced dominance (control, power) were stronger for verbs than nouns, suggesting that motor actions linked with verbs play a role in understanding and conveying emotional experience in language.
Semin, G. R., Görts, C. A., Nandram, S. & Semin-Goossens, A. Cultural perspectives on the linguistic representation of emotion and emotion events. Cogn. Emot. 16, 11–28 (2002).
Pavlenko, A. Emotions and the body in Russian and English. Pragmat. Cogn. 10, 207–241 (2002). An analysis of elicited oral narratives shows that Russian encodes and emphasizes the embodied nature of emotional experience to a higher degree than English, suggesting that patterns of attention to experience are systematically shaped by language and culture.
Wierzbicka, A. The semantics of interjection. J. Pragmat. 18, 159–192 (1992).
Wierzbicka, A. Russian emotional expression. Ethos 26, 456–483 (1998).
Wierzbicka, A. Emotions across Languages and Cultures: Diversity and Universals (Cambridge University Press, 1999).
Seih, Y.-T., Beier, S. & Pennebaker, J. W. Development and examination of the linguistic category model in a computerized text analysis method. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 36, 343–355 (2017).
Moran, T. & Eyal, T. Emotion regulation by psychological distance and level of abstraction: two meta-analyses. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 26, 112–159 (2022).
Nook, E. C., Ahn, H. E., Schleider, J. L. & Somerville, L. H. Emotion regulation is associated with increases in linguistic measures of both psychological distancing and abstractness. Affec. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-024-00269-7 (2024).
Kneeland, E. T., Dovidio, J. F., Joormann, J. & Clark, M. S. Emotion malleability beliefs, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: Integrating affective and clinical science. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 45, 81–88 (2016).
Lindquist, K. A., Gendron, M., Oosterwijk, S. & Barrett, L. F. Do people essentialize emotions? Individual differences in emotion essentialism and emotional experience. Emotion 13, 629–644 (2013).
McMullen, K. M., MacVittie, A., Leffers, J. S., Betz, N. & Wormwood, J. B. Categorization processes in emotion expression recognition: the roles of language and essentialism. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001483 (2025).
Carreiras, M., Carriedo, N., Alonso, M. A. & Fernández, A. The role of verb tense and verb aspect in the foregrounding of information during reading. Mem. Cogn. 25, 438–446 (1997).
Hong, J. P., Ferretti, T. R., Craven, R. & Hepburn, R. D. Time, verbs, and imagining events. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 73, 55–63 (2019).
Madden, C. J. & Ferretti, T. R. Verb aspect and the mental representation of situations. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110199031.217 (2009).
Madden, C. J. & Zwaan, R. A. How does verb aspect constrain event representations? Mem. Cogn. 31, 663–672 (2003).
Nook, E. C., Schleider, J. L. & Somerville, L. H. A linguistic signature of psychological distancing in emotion regulation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 146, 337–346 (2017). In an experimental setting, English-speaking participants spontaneously used fewer first-person singular pronouns and present-tense verbs when asked to down-regulate negative emotions, and vice versa, consistent with the hypothesis that language indexes psychological distance to the event in question.
Nook, E. C., Hull, T. D., Nock, M. & Somerville, L. Linguistic measures of psychological distance track symptom levels and treatment outcomes in a large set of psychotherapy transcripts. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2114737119 (2022).
Comrie, B. Aspect: An Introduction to the Study of Verbal Aspect and Related Problems (Cambridge University Press, 1976).
Dowty, D. R. The semantics of aspectual classes of verbs in English. in Word Meaning and Montague Grammar: The Semantics of Verbs and Times in Generative Semantics and in Montague’s Ptq 37–132 (Springer, 1979).
Dery, J. E. & Bittner, D. Time and causation in discourse: temporal proximity, implicit causality, and re-mention biases. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 45, 883–899 (2016).
Ferretti, T. R. & Katz, A. N. Verb aspect and the retrieval of events from autobiographical memory. in Advances in Psychology Research Vol. 64 (ed. Columbus, A. M.) 1–23 (Nova Science Publishers, 2010).
Hart, W. & Albarracín, D. What I was doing versus what I did: verb aspect influences memory and future actions. Psychol. Sci. 20, 238–244 (2009).
Havas, D. A. & Chapp, C. B. Language for winning hearts and minds: verb aspect in U.S. presidential campaign speeches for engaging emotion. Front. Psychol. 7, 899 (2016). Phrases containing verbs in the imperfective aspect were found more often in politicians’ stump speeches, and were rated more emotionally intense, than verbs in the perfective aspect, suggesting that the imperfective corresponds with an online, involved mode of emotional processing.
Magliano, J. P. & Schleich, M. C. Verb aspect and situation models. Discourse Process. 29, 83–112 (2000).
Huston, J., Meier, S., Faith, M. & Reynolds, A. Exploratory study of automated linguistic analysis for progress monitoring and outcome assessment. Couns. Psychother. Res. 19, 321–328 (2019).
Reyes, L. et al. Language and therapeutic change: a speech acts analysis. Psychother. Res. 18, 355–362 (2008).
Galinsky, A. D. & Kray, L. J. From thinking about what might have been to sharing what we know: the effects of counterfactual mind-sets on information sharing in groups. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40, 606–618 (2004).
Vieira, P. P., Silva, A. S. D. & Geeraerts, D. Construing factivity: mood alternations in epistemic contexts. Máthesis 21, 103–127 (2012).
Galinsky, A. D. & Moskowitz, G. B. Perspective-taking: decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 78, 708–724 (2000).
Kray, L. J., Galinsky, A. D. & Wong, E. M. Thinking within the box: the relational processing style elicited by counterfactual mind-sets. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 91, 33–48 (2006).
Roese, N. J. The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 66, 805–818 (1994).
Janocko, A., Larche, A., Raso, J. & Zembroski, K. Counterfactuals in the language of social media: a natural language processing project in conjunction with the World Well-Being Project. (2016). Usage of counterfactual statements in social media posts was found to relate to life satisfaction across counties in the United States, consistent with the hypothesis that counterfactuals support meaning-making by positioning current events relative to what might or could have been.
Kray, L. J. et al. From what might have been to what must have been: counterfactual thinking creates meaning. J. Personal. Social Psychol. 98, 106–118 (2010).
Bamberg, M. Language, concepts and emotions: the role of language in the construction of emotions. Lang. Sci. 19, 309–340 (1997).
Garvey, C. & Caramazza, A. Implicit causality in verbs. Linguist. Inq. 5, 459–464 (1974).
Kasof, J. & Lee, J. Y. Implicit causality as implicit salience. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 65, 877–891 (1993).
Flam, H. & Kleres, J. Inequality and prejudice. German social scientist as producers of feeling rules. Sociol. Res. Online 21, 161–175 (2016).
Baumeister, R. F. & Newman, L. S. How stories make sense of personal experiences: motives that shape autobiographical narratives. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 20, 676–690 (1994).
Capps, L. & Ochs, E. Constructing Panic: The Discourse of Agoraphobia (Harvard University Press, 1995).
Capps, L. & Ochs, E. Out of place: narrative insights into agoraphobia. Discourse Process. 19, 407–439 (1995).
Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. The cultural construction of the self and emotion: implications for social behavior. in Emotion and Culture: Empirical Studies of Mutual Influence (eds Kitayama, S. & Markus, H. R.) 89–130 (American Psychological Association, 1994).
McAdams, D. & McLean, K. Narrative identity. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 233–238 (2013).
Rouhizadeh, M. et al. Identifying locus of control in social media language. in Proceedings of the 2018 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing 1146–1152. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/D18-1145 (Association for Computational Linguistics, 2018). Lexical and syntactical features are combined to identify language associated with speakers’ perceived control of their own circumstances, illustrating the value of incorporating structure in addition to words when studying how experience is articulated in natural language.
Rotter, J. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol. Monogr. 80, 1–28 (1966).
Henley, N. M., Miller, M. & Beazley, J. A. Syntax, semantics, and sexual violence: agency and the passive voice. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 14, 60–84 (1995).
Knobloch-Westerwick, S. & Taylor, L. D. The blame game: Elements of causal attribution and its impact on siding with agents in the news. Commun. Res. 35, 723–744 (2008). Experimental manipulations show that agents were perceived as playing a more causal role when active (vs passive) voice was used to describe actions relative to an event, suggesting that verb voice reflects situated power dynamics.
Haspelmath, M. Non-canonical marking of core arguments in European languages. 53. https://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.46.04has (2001).
Kalyuga, M. Russian verbs for emotions. N.Z. Slavon. J. 39, 139–149 (2005).
Lambie, J. A. & Marcel, A. J. Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: a theoretical framework. Psychol. Rev. 109, 219–259 (2002).
Mesquita, B. & Markus, H. R. Culture and emotion: models of agency as sources of cultural variation in emotion. Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium 341–358 (2004).
Cole, P. M., Bruschi, C. J. & Tamang, B. L. Cultural differences in children’s emotional reactions to difficult situations. Child Dev. 73, 983–996 (2002).
Uskul, A. K., Cross, S. E., Sunbay, Z., Gercek-Swing, B. & Ataca, B. Honor bound: the cultural construction of honor in Turkey and the Northern United States. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 43, 1131–1151 (2012).
Jackendoff, R. S. Language, Consciousness, Culture: Essays on Mental Structure (MIT Press, 2007).
Liu, M. Emotion in lexicon and grammar: lexical-constructional interface of Mandarin emotional predicates. Ling. Sin. 2, 4 (2016).
Landau, I. The Locative Syntax of Experiencers (MIT Press, 2010).
Martin, J. R. & White, P. R. The Language of Evaluation (Palgrave Macmillan, 2003).
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J. & Tannenbaum, P. H. The Measurement of Meaning Vol. 47 (University of Illinois Press, 1957).
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R. & Frijda, N. H. Appraisal theories of emotion: state of the art and future development. Emot. Rev. 5, 119–124 (2013).
Lyons, J. Semantics. Vol. 2 (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1977).
Besnier, N. Language and affect. Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 19, 419–451 (1990)
Barrett, L. F. & Russell, J. A. Structure of current affect: controversies and emerging consensus. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 8, 10–14 (1999).
Fontaine, J. R. J., Scherer, K. R., Roesch, E. B. & Ellsworth, P. C. The world of emotions is not two-dimensional. Psychol. Sci. 18, 1050–1057 (2007).
Russell, J. A. A circumplex model of affect. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 39, 1161–1178 (1980).
Thornton, M. A., Wolf, S., Reilly, B. J., Slingerland, E. G. & Tamir, D. I. The 3d Mind Model characterizes how people understand mental states across modern and historical cultures. Affec. Sci. 3, 93–104 (2022).
Fast, E., Chen, B. & Bernstein, M. S. Empath: Understanding topic signals in large-scale text. in Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems 4647–4657. https://doi.org/10.1145/2858036.2858535 (ACM, 2016).
Pepe, A. & Bollen, J. Between conjecture and memento: shaping a collective emotional perception of the future. AAAI spring symposium: Emotion, personality, and social behavior 111–116 (2008).
Young, L. & Soroka, S. Affective news: The automated coding of sentiment in political texts. Political Commun. 29, 205–231 (2012).
Bradley, M. M. & Lang, P. J. Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report C-1, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology (University of Florida, 1999).
Hutto, C. J. & Gilbert, E. VADER: a parsimonious rule-based model for sentiment analysis of social media text. Proc. Int. AAAI Conf. Web Soc. Media 8, 216–225 (2014).
Mohammad, S. Obtaining reliable human ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance for 20,000 English words. in Proceedings of the 56th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (Volume 1: Long Papers) (eds Gurevych, I. & Miyao, Y.) 174–184. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/P18-1017 (Association for Computational Linguistics, 2018).
Stevenson, R. A., Mikels, J. A. & James, T. W. Characterization of the affective norms for English words by discrete emotional categories. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 1020–1024 (2007).
Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V. & Brysbaert, M. Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behav. Res. Methods 45, 1191–1207 (2013).
Hancock, J. T., Landrigan, C. & Silver, C. Expressing emotion in text-based communication. in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems 929–932 (Association for Computing Machinery, 2007). doi:10/bkbfp6. Experimental participants told to act as if they were experiencing a positive emotion used more punctuation overall, and more exclamation points specifically, than participants in a negative emotion condition, suggesting that punctuation is a primary strategy used to convey emotion in text.
Frank, M. R., Mitchell, L., Dodds, P. S. & Danforth, C. M. Happiness and the patterns of life: a study of geolocated tweets. Sci. Rep. 3, 2625 (2013).
Alpers, G. W. et al. Evaluation of computerized text analysis in an Internet breast cancer support group. Comput. Hum. Behav. 21, 361–376 (2005).
Carlier, C. et al. In search of state and trait emotion markers in mobile-sensed language: a field study. JMIR Ment. Health 9, e31724 (2022).
Hoemann, K. et al. Using freely generated labels instead of rating scales to assess emotion in everyday life. Assessment 10731911241283623. https://doi.org/10.1177/10731911241283623 (2024).
Kahn, J. H., Tobin, R. M., Massey, A. E. & Anderson, J. A. Measuring emotional expression with the linguistic inquiry and word count. Am. J. Psychol. 120, 263–286 (2007).
Kross, E. et al. Does counting emotion words on online social networks provide a window into people’s subjective experience of emotion? A case study on Facebook. Emotion 19, 97–107 (2019).
Tov, W., Ng, K. L., Lin, H. & Qiu, L. Detecting well-being via computerized content analysis of brief diary entries. Psychol. Assess. 25, 1069–1078 (2013).
Eichstaedt, J. C. & Weidman, A. C. Tracking fluctuations in psychological states using social media language: a case study of weekly emotion. Eur. J. Pers. 34, 845–858 (2020).
Mairesse, F., Walker, M. A., Mehl, M. R. & Moore, R. K. Using linguistic cues for the automatic recognition of personality in conversation and text. jair 30, 457–500 (2007).
Pennebaker, J. W. & King, L. Linguistic styles: language use as an individual difference. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 77, 1296–1312 (1999).
Yarkoni, T. Personality in 100,000 Words: a large-scale analysis of personality and word use among bloggers. J. Res. Personal. 44, 363–373 (2010).
Kramer, A. D. An unobtrusive behavioral model of ‘gross national happiness’. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems 287–290 (2010).
Pressman, S. D. & Cohen, S. Positive emotion word use and longevity in famous deceased psychologists. Health Psychol. 31, 297–305 (2012).
Coppersmith, G., Dredze, M. & Harman, C. Quantifying mental health signals in Twitter. Proceedings of the workshop on computational linguistics and clinical psychology: From linguistic signal to clinical reality 51–60 (2014).
De Choudhury, M., Gamon, M., Counts, S. & Horvitz, E. Predicting depression via social media. Proceedings of the international AAAI conference on web and social media 7, 128–137 (2013).
Eichstaedt, J. C. et al. Facebook language predicts depression in medical records. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115, 11203–11208 (2018).
Loveys, K., Torrez, J., Fine, A., Moriarty, G. & Coppersmith, G. Cross-cultural differences in language markers of depression online. Proceedings of the fifth workshop on computational linguistics and clinical psychology: from keyboard to clinic 78–87 (2018).
Rai, S. et al. Key language markers of depression on social media depend on race. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 121, e2319837121 (2024).
Barker, M. S., Bidstrup, E. M., Robinson, G. A. & Nelson, N. L. Grumpy” or “furious”? Arousal of emotion labels influences judgments of facial expressions. PLoS ONE 15, e0235390 (2020).
Heath, M. Orthography in social media: pragmatic and prosodic interpretations of caps lock. Proc. Linguist. Soc. Am. 3, 55–13 (2018). A corpus study of social media data shows that users employ single-word capitalization in positions indicative of emphatic stress and semantic focus, illustrating how orthography can serve the same functions in written text as prosody does in spoken.
Newfield, C., Alexandrova, A. & John, S. Limits of the Numerical: The Abuses and Uses of Quantification (University of Chicago Press, 2022).
Tafreshi, D., Slaney, K. L. & Neufeld, S. D. Quantification in psychology: critical analysis of an unreflective practice. J. Theor. Philos. Psychol. 36, 233 (2016).
Harris, R. B. & Paradice, D. An investigation of the computer-mediated communication of emotions. J. Appl. Sci. Res. 3, 2081–2090 (2007).
Bednarek, M. Emotion Talk across Corpora (Springer, 2008).
Glynn, D. Correspondence analysis: Exploring data and identifying patterns. in Human Cognitive Processing Vol. 43 (eds Glynn, D. & Robinson, J. A.) 443–485 (John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2014).
Fuoli, M. A step-wise method for annotating appraisal. Funct. Lang. 25, 229–258 (2018).
Fuoli, M. & Hommerberg, C. Optimising transparency, reliability and replicability: annotation principles and inter-coder agreement in the quantification of evaluative expressions. Corpora 10, 315–349 (2015).
Tenbrink, T. Cognitive Discourse Analysis: accessing cognitive representations and processes through language data. Lang. Cogn. 7, 98–137 (2014).
Edwards, D. Categories are for talking: on the cognitive and discursive bases of categorization. Theory Psychol. 1, 515–542 (1991).
Ruba, A. L., Pollak, S. D. & Saffran, J. R. Acquiring complex communicative systems: statistical learning of language and emotion. Top. Cogn. Sci. 14, 432–450 (2022).
Bakhtin, M. Problems of Dostoevsky’s Poetics (University of Minnesota Press, 1963).
Bruner, J. S. Acts of Meaning (Harvard University Press, 1990).
Vygotsky, L. S. The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky, Volume 1: Problems of General Psychology (Plenum Press, 1934).
Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (De Gruyter Mouton, 2015).
Geeraerts, D. Cognitive semantics. in The Routledge Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (eds Wu, X. & Taylor, J. R.) 19–29 (Routledge, 2021).
Hoemann, K. et al. What we can learn about emotion by talking with the Hadza. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916231178555 (2023).
Emotion and Culture: Empirical Studies of Mutual Influence. (American Psychological Association, 1994).
Lindquist, K. A. The role of language in emotion: existing evidence and future directions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17, 135–139 (2017).
Lutz, C. A. & Abu-Lughod, L. Language and the Politics of Emotion (Cambridge University Press, 1990).
Languages of Sentiment (John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1999).
Barrett, L. F. Emotions are real. Emotion 12, 413–429 (2012).
Evans, V. How Words Mean: Lexical Concepts, Cognitive Models, and Meaning Construction (Oxford University Press on Demand, 2009).
Gendron, M. & Barrett, L. F. Emotion perception as conceptual synchrony. Emot. Rev. 10, 101–110 (2018).
Graesser, A. C., Singer, M. & Trabasso, T. Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychol. Rev. 101, 371–395 (1994).
Wittgenstein, L. Philosophical Investigations (Blackwell, 1953).
Eckert, P. & McConnell-Ginet, S. Think practically and look locally: language and gender as community-based practice. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 21, 461–490 (1992).
Cowen, A. S. & Keltner, D. Semantic space theory: a computational approach to emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 124–136 (2021).
Tracy, J. L. & Randles, D. Four models of basic emotions: a review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and Watt. Emot. Rev. 3, 397–405 (2011).
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P. & ter Schure, E. Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 57, 212–228 (1989).
Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Hari, R. & Hietanen, J. K. Bodily maps of emotions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 646–651 (2014).
Ekman, P. & Cordaro, D. T. What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emot. Rev. 3, 364–370 (2011).
Keltner, D., Sauter, D., Tracy, J. L. & Cowen, A. S. Emotional expression: advances in basic emotion theory. J. Nonverbal Behav. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00293-3 (2019).
Aldao, A. The future of emotion regulation research: capturing context. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 155–172 (2013).
Greenaway, K. H., Kalokerinos, E. K. & Williams, L. A. Context is everything (in emotion research). Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 12, e12393 (2018).
Baes, N., Haslam, N. & Vylomova, E. A multidimensional framework for evaluating lexical semantic change with social science applications. in Proceedings of the 62nd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (Volume 1: Long Papers) (eds Ku, L.-W., Martins, A. & Srikumar, V.) 1390–1415. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/2024.acl-long.76 (Association for Computational Linguistics, 2024).
Hayati, S. A., Kang, D. & Ungar, L. Does BERT learn as humans perceive? Understanding linguistic styles through lexica. 2021 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, 6323–6331 (2021).
Kokalj, E., Škrlj, B., Lavrač, N., Pollak, S. & Robnik-Šikonja, M. BERT meets shapley: Extending SHAP explanations to transformer-based classifiers. Proceedings of the EACL hackashop on news media content analysis and automated report generation, 16–21 (2021).
Tan, Z. et al. Large language models for data annotation and synthesis: a survey. Proceedings of the 2024 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, 930–957 (2024).
Gligorić, K., Zrnic, T., Lee, C., Candès, E. J. & Jurafsky, D. Can unconfident LLM annotations be used for confident conclusions? Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2408.15204 (2024).
Rathje, S. et al. GPT is an effective tool for multilingual psychological text analysis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 121, e2308950121 (2024).
Niu, M., Jaiswal, M. & Provost, E. M. From text to emotion: unveiling the emotion annotation capabilities of LLMs. Proc. Interspeech 2024, 2650–2654 (2024).
Havaldar, S., Singhal, B., Liu, L. & Ungar, L. Multilingual language models are not multicultural: a case study in emotion. The 13th Workshop on Computational Approaches to Subjectivity, Sentiment, & Social Media Analysis, 202–214 (2023).
Ahmad, I. S., Dudy, S., Ramachandranpillai, R. & Church, K. Are generative language models multicultural? A study on Hausa culture and emotions using ChatGPT. Proceedings of the 2nd Workshop on Cross-Cultural Considerations in NLP, 98–106 (2024).
Kellert, O. Probing sociodemographic influence on code-switching and language choice in Quebec with geolocation of tweets. Front. Psychol. 14, 1137038 (2023).
Kellert, O. & Matlis, N. H. Geolocation of multiple sociolinguistic markers in Buenos Aires. PLoS ONE 17, e0274114 (2022).
Matz, S. C. & Harari, G. M. Personality–place transactions: mapping the relationships between Big Five personality traits, states, and daily places. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 120, 1367–1385 (2021).
Müller, S. R., Peters, H., Matz, S. C., Wang, W. & Harari, G. M. Investigating the relationships between mobility behaviours and indicators of subjective well‐being using smartphone‐based experience sampling and GPS tracking. Eur. J. Personal. 34, 714–732 (2020).
Horn, A. B., Meier, T., Dehghani, M. & Boyd, R. L. Language in close relationships. in The Handbook of Language Analysis in Psychology 102–124 (Guildford Press, 2022).
Mehl, M. R., Gosling, S. D. & Pennebaker, J. W. Personality in its natural habitat: manifestations and implicit folk theories of personality in daily life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 862–877 (2006).
Rice, C. & Pasupathi, M. Reflecting on self-relevant experiences: adult age differences. Dev. Psychol. 46, 479–490 (2010).
Boiger, M., Deyne, S. D. & Mesquita, B. Emotions in “the world”: cultural practices, products, and meanings of anger and shame in two individualist cultures. Front. Psychol. 4, 867 (2013).
Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil, C., Lee, L., Pang, B. & Kleinberg, J. Echoes of power: Language effects and power differences in social interaction. in Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on World Wide Web—WWW ’12 699–708 (ACM Press, 2012).
Fuoli, M. & Bednarek, M. Emotional labor in webcare and beyond: a linguistic framework and case study. J. Pragmat. 191, 256–270 (2022).
Giles, H., Coupland, N. & Coupland, J. Accommodation theory: communication, context, and consequence. in Contexts of accommodation: Developments in applied sociolinguistics (eds. Giles, H., Coupland, J. & Coupland, N.) 1–68 (Cambridge University Press, 1991).
Adams, G. & Markus, H. R. Culture as patterns: an alternative approach to the problem of reification. Cult. Psychol. 7, 283–296 (2001).
Heft, H. Environment, cognition, and culture: reconsidering the cognitive map. J. Environ. Psychol. 33, 14–25 (2013).
Kashima, Y., Bratanova, B. & Peters, K. Social transmission and shared reality in cultural dynamics. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 23, 15–19 (2018).
Kleres, J. Emotional expertise: emotions and the expert interview. in Methods of Exploring Emotions 90–100 (Routledge, 2015).
Omitaomu, D. et al. Empathic conversations: a multi-level dataset of contextualized conversations. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2205.12698 (2022).
Reece, A. et al. The CANDOR corpus: insights from a large multimodal dataset of naturalistic conversation. Sci. Adv. 9, eadf3197 (2023).
Holler, J. & Levinson, S. C. Multimodal language processing in human communication. Trends in Cogn. Sci. 23, 639–652 (2019).
Ong, D. C. et al. Modeling emotion in complex stories: the Stanford Emotional Narratives Dataset. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 12, 579–594 (2021).
Timmons, A. C. et al. New frontiers in ambulatory assessment: big data methods for capturing couples’ emotions, vocalizations, and physiology in daily life. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 552–563 (2017).
Trujillo, J. P. & Holler, J. Conversational facial signals combine into compositional meanings that change the interpretation of speaker intentions. Sci. Rep. 14, 2286 (2024).
Low, D. M. et al. Using Generative AI to create lexicons for interpretable text models with high content validity. Preprint at https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/vf2bc_v2 (2024).
Kövecses, Z. Metaphor and Emotion: Language, Culture, and Body in Human Feeling. (Cambridge University Press, 2003).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by funding from the European Commission (Marie Skłodowska-Curie Individual Fellowship 892379 to K.H.) and the European Research Council (Advanced Grant 83458 to B.M.) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program. This work was also supported by funding from the Research Foundation—Flanders (FWO 12A3923N to K.H.). This paper reflects only the authors’ views; the funders had no role in the preparation of the manuscript or decision to publish; the European Commission and European Research Council are not liable for any use that may be made of the contained information.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
K.H., Y.L. and È.D. reviewed the literature and drafted the manuscript. S.D., L.U., D.G. and B.M. reviewed the manuscript and revised it together with K.H. All authors approved the final version.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Psychology thanks Erik Nook and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editor: Jennifer Bellingtier. [A peer review file is available].
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Hoemann, K., Lee, Y., Dussault, È. et al. The construction of emotional meaning in language. Commun Psychol 3, 99 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-025-00255-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-025-00255-0