Abstract
Extensive research has established the positive relationship between youths’ educational expectations and academic performance. However, limited attention has been paid to how the developmental and growth trajectories of educational expectations may dynamically shape the development and growth in science performance during middle school—a critical period for fostering long-term STEM achievement. Leveraging longitudinal panel data from the second-cohort sample of the Longitudinal Study of American Youth (LSAY), this study employs parallel-process latent growth curve modeling (PP-LGCM) to investigate how evolving educational expectations and science performance of youths during early adolescence jointly predict STEM degree attainment in adulthood. Results reveal that the development and growth in educational expectations of youths across middle school years positively drive their corresponding development and growth in science performance. These parallel trajectories collectively and significantly predict youths’ successful graduation with a four-year STEM degree in adulthood, with the developmental and growth trajectories of science performance significantly mediating the relationship. Monte Carlo simulations confirmed the robustness of these findings, demonstrating strong statistical power (>0.80) and minimal bias (<5%) for all focal parameters, with sensitivity analyses indicating model stability across varying sample sizes. The findings underscore the interconnected, cumulative, and progressive nature of academic motivation and science competency during early adolescence in relation to youths’ later STEM achievement, challenging static conceptualizations of these constructs. By framing educational expectations and science learning as dynamic, time-varying processes, this study advances theoretical and methodological approaches to STEM development research. Practical implications highlight the need for holistic educational policies and interventions that nurture both aspirational growth and foundational science competence during formative school years, thereby strengthening pathways to STEM success.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The educational development and achievement of young people hold profound implications not only for their long-term health and well-being in adulthood (Kosik et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2022) but also serve as critical drivers of socioeconomic advancement and cultural enrichment across national and societal contexts (Dolby and Rizvi, 2008; Menzies and Baars, 2021). Crucially, the attainment of an undergraduate degree apparently signifies the prospects of youths for employment, income stability, and quality of life across the adult life course (Anderson and Li, 2020; Bass and Besen-Cassino, 2016; Dolby and Rizvi, 2008), particularly for those pursuing science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) disciplines. Indeed, the proportion of STEM graduates within a nation directly and positively influences its political-economic competitiveness, national innovation capacity, societal development, and technological advancement (Penprase, 2020; Reid et al., 2025; Tasos et al., 2018). Despite this, the rates of students to enroll for STEM majors in matriculation and/or successful graduation with a STEM degree have been declining over the past few decades (Ji, 2021; Pov et al., 2024), underscoring the urgency of identifying factors that promote successful STEM degree completion. Although researchers have investigated the contributors of science- or STEM-related socialization, e.g., parental guidance in science education, home-based STEM resources, teacher support, and school STEM environment, to youths’ STEM development (de las Cuevas et al., 2022; Ji, 2021), little research has examined the dynamic relationships between youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school in contribution to later STEM attainment of youths in adulthood. This gap is notable given robust evidence linking educational expectations to academic achievement (Fishman, 2022; Keung and Ho, 2019; Pinquart and Ebeling, 2020). Importantly, both educational expectations and academic performance are developmental constructs that evolve over time, rather than static traits as often conceptualized. To address this, the present study employs a dynamic, longitudinal framework to investigate how shifts in youths’ educational expectations affect the changes in their science performance during middle school, and how these trajectories work collectively to predict eventual STEM degree completion in adulthood.
The current study’s focus on youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school is grounded in the critical developmental phase of early adolescence. This period represents a critical formative window for cultivating educational aspirations, academic motivation, and foundational learning competencies (Carolanl, 2017; Park, 2021), with implications that reverberate into adulthood through educational and career trajectories. Such a focus is particularly salient for youth STEM development, as STEM achievement is inherently cumulative and progressive, requiring sustained academic motivation and mastery of essential science knowledge and skills acquired during—or even prior to—middle school (Miller and Pearson, 2012; Penprase, 2020; Zhang, 2022). However, existing longitudinal studies often reduce the contributions of early educational development and science-related socialization factors to static, fixed effects, measuring these variables at singular timepoints rather than capturing their dynamic evolution over time (Kohen and Nitzan, 2022; LeBeau et al., 2012; Luo et al., 2022; Wang, 2024). This methodological limitation constrains the field’s ability to discern how evolving processes in adolescents’ educational development shape later STEM achievement, thereby hindering the design of targeted educational reforms and policies to advance science and STEM education. To address these research gaps, we intended in the current study to examine how the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations may contribute to their developmental and growth trajectories in science performance across middle school, which then jointly lead to youths’ later successful graduation with a STEM degree in adulthood.
Theoretical framework of youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school and STEM attainment in adulthood
Although the educational development of youths and their later academic achievement, including successful graduation with a STEM degree in adulthood, depend on various environmental and personal factors, youths’ educational expectations have been consistently found to positively predict better academic performance and success, such as higher standardized exam scores, GPAs, college education enrollment, and graduation with a university degree (Andrew and Hauser, 2011; Liu et al., 2025; Liu et al., 2020; Wang, 2016). These relationships align with core tenets of situated expectancy-value theory (SEVT; Eccles, 2009; Eccles and Wigfield, 2020), which posits that achievement-related choices (e.g., pursuing STEM degrees) are directly influenced by (a) expectations for success (beliefs about one’s ability to succeed) and (b) subjective task values (perceived importance, interest, or utility of a task). Educational expectations reflect both constructs: They embody youths’ success expectations in academia and the SEVT concept of attainment value (the importance placed on educational goals), which collectively motivate persistent engagement in learning activities—including science—and long-term goal pursuit, such as STEM degree attainment in adulthood.
Nevertheless, limited research has examined how educational expectations of youths may dynamically shape their science performance during middle school or how these evolving trajectories may jointly predict later STEM degree attainment. Manifestly, emerging advanced longitudinal studies reported that both educational expectations and academic performance of youths are time-varying processes evolving over time (Chykina, 2019; Dochow and Neumeyer, 2021; Marsh, 2023), denoting their developmental and changing nature. SEVT elucidates this dynamism: Expectations and values are situated and malleable, continually reshaped by contextual feedback (e.g., science performance outcomes) and sociocultural influences (e.g., parental support, school climate). Early success in science reinforces competence beliefs and interest values, creating recursive feedback loops that sustain motivation and performance growth (Eccles and Wigfield, 2020; Wang and Degol, 2013). Relevantly and critically, science performance, hence, serves as both a direct predictor of long-term STEM degree outcomes (by signaling foundational competence necessary for advanced study) and a mediator of the expectation-STEM link (by translating aspirational motivation into tangible academic achievement that validates STEM identity and task-related utility/attainment values). Additionally, middle school marks a pivotal transition from childhood—characterized by dependency and undefined self-concept—to adolescence (Reynolds et al., 2019), a critical period for cultivating identity, academic motivation, learning interests and performance, as well as future educational and occupational directions. This formative period is critical for youths to establish educational competencies that bridge childhood and early adulthood within the life course. In fact, SEVT underscores adolescence as a “critical window” for STEM identity formation, which means when early science experiences align with youths’ interest values (e.g., fascination with scientific inquiry) and utility values (e.g., belief that science aids career goals), they foster enduring STEM aspirations (Eccles and Wigfield, 2023; Lauermann et al., 2017). Therefore, it is of research worth to investigate how youths formulate their educational expectancy and performance in middle school (Reynolds et al., 2019; Zilanawala et al., 2017), which are believed to have profound impacts on their later academic achievement in adulthood, such as successful completion of a STEM degree.
As abovementioned, although the STEM development of youths is a cumulative and progressive process that meticulously relies on their educational motivations and essential science knowledge and skills adequately acquired in middle school years or earlier (Miller and Pearson, 2012; Penprase, 2020), there is, however, a paucity of research that has empirically explored the evolving relationship between youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school and scrutinized how these trajectories may jointly affect later STEM attainment in adulthood. For this, we intended in the current study to adopt a dynamic approach to study how the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations shape their developmental and growth trajectories in science performance across middle school years, which are both expected to contribute to later STEM degree completion in adulthood. Apparently, SEVT provides a mechanistic framework that strong educational expectations (expectancy beliefs) enhance science effort and persistence, improving performance (attainment); this success subsequently validates and strengthens STEM-related utility/interest values, ultimately predicting STEM degree pursuit (choice), (Eccles and Wigfield, 2020). Thus, science performance is theorized to mediate the expectation-STEM link—by operationalizing motivational beliefs into demonstrable competence—while also functioning as a direct predictor of STEM attainment due to its role as a gateway to advanced STEM coursework. This dual role is empirically tested in this study.
In this study, the educational expectations of youths refer to the Wisconsin model portraying as youths’ general educational expectancy and aspirations for achieving better academic performance (Roth, 2017). Therefore, the educational expectations of youths are understood as the possible educational attainment perceived as reachable (Andrew and Hauser, 2011; Roth, 2017). This approach to assess educational expectations and its effects on adolescents’ academic performance has been commonly applied by empirical researchers (Andrew and Hauser, 2011; Liu et al., 2020; O’Donnell et al., 2022; Zhang, 2014).
Notably, while SEVT traditionally emphasizes domain-specific expectations (e.g., math self-efficacy), general educational expectations reflect overarching success beliefs that permeate academic domains. This aligns with SEVT’s acknowledgment that broader achievement beliefs scaffold domain-specific motivations (Wigfield and Eccles, 2023), particularly during early adolescence when career identities are nascent, e.g., science and STEM development. Empirically, contemporary advanced longitudinal research underscores that educational expectations and academic performance are dynamic, evolving constructs rather than static traits, necessitating analysis across developmental trajectories (Andrew and Hauser, 2011; Chykina, 2019; Marsh, 2023; Widlund et al., 2023). This is consonant with the life course perspective and Bayesian learning theory explicating that youths are cognizant agents capable of refining their cognitive and behavioral processes—including educational expectations and science learning practices—throughout middle school years in order to accomplish what they expect valuable and achievable in the future when assimilating new information and experiences across the life course (Morgan, 2005; Pallas, 2003). In the same vein, SEVT complements this: Expectations and values are continually updated via recursive processes where prior outcomes (e.g., science grades) recalibrate future expectations (success beliefs) and task values (Eccles and Wigfield, 2020).
In fact, the STEM development of youths in adulthood necessitates their well-equipped essential science knowledge and skills as well as strong educational aspirations cumulatively and progressively established in the early years of middle school (English, 2017; Larkin and Lowrie, 2022; Zhang, 2022). Notably, successful STEM development in adulthood hinges on foundational science competencies and sustained educational aspirations cultivated sustainably during early adolescence, particularly in middle school. This corresponds with what Mau et al. (1995) mentioned: “(e)ighth-grade students typically are in the crucial stage of exploring self and the world of work. Unlike some occupational fields, preparation for nontraditional occupations, especially in the areas of science and engineering, must begin early. …training in math and science needs to be sequential and uninterrupted from elementary school, and fundamentals must be mastered before high school (p. 324).” Accordingly, SEVT formalizes this: Middle school science mastery builds foundational competence beliefs, while early STEM exposure cultivates interest values—both prerequisites for later STEM choices (Ozulku and Kloser, 2023; Wang, 2013). In this study, science performance of youths during middle schools refers to their academic proficiency in core science subjects that include biology, chemistry, physical science, and environmental science, which are the pivotal scientific foundations for their later STEM development (Larkin and Lowrie, 2022; Tasos et al., 2018). Despite this, very few longitudinal studies have systematically examined how youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school jointly predict later STEM attainment. Pertinently, Larson et al. (2014) found in their short-term longitudinal study that first-year college students’ initial educational aspirations and science interests significantly predicted STEM degree completion. Similarly, Zhang et al. (2019) linked grade-10 academic aspirations to BA-STEM attainment, while Marsh, (2023) identified dynamic relationships between high-school math self-concept and postsecondary STEM credentials. These findings resonate with SEVT that proximal expectations/values predict distal choices, but crucially, middle school is the formative phase where these beliefs crystallize (Eccles, 2009).
While prior longitudinal studies affirm the connection between adolescents’ educational expectations, science performance, and eventual STEM achievement, they predominantly emphasize rank-order or autoregressive effects, overlooking how developmental and growth trajectories of these constructs during middle school prospectively shape STEM degree attainment in adulthood. Empirical evidence confirms that educational expectations and academic performance evolve dynamically during adolescence (O’Donnell et al., 2022; Zhao et al., 2019). For example, O’Donnell et al. (2022), tracking 1477 Australian adolescents from ages 12–13 to 16–17, demonstrated that intrapersonal shifts in educational expectations significantly predicted later academic outcomes. Besides, Zhao et al. (2019), following 775 Chinese students from grades 6–8, found that early educational exploration and commitment predicted developmental progression in these measures, ultimately enhancing grade 8 academic achievement. Yet, such work largely treats educational development as static, neglecting latent developmental and growth trajectories that may underpin long-term educational outcomes like STEM degree attainment. For this, Carolan (2017) mentioned: “(e)mpirical work emanating from this tradition, however, has treated this complex mental process of educational expectations formation as one that is relatively fixed by adolescence and not responsive to new, relevant information (p. 238).”
In this study, we conceptualize the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school as two interrelated and evolving processes: (1) the developmental trend of an individual youth relative to their peers over time (reflecting developmental status in educational expectations and science performance), and (2) intrapersonal changes within each youth across grades 7 to 9 (capturing growth in these measures through continuing and repeated assessments). These dual trajectories align with SEVT’s developmental principles that initial status (intercepts) reflects early-formed expectations/values, while growth (slopes) captures how school experiences reshape them (Eccles and Wigfield, 2023; Wigfield and Eccles, 2000). We hypothesize that the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations during middle school will positively shape their developmental and growth trajectories in science performance. Together, these two evolving trajectories are expected to predict youths’ later successful graduation with a STEM degree in adulthood. By modeling these longitudinal dynamic relationships, the current study contributes to a nuanced understanding of how early academic motivation and skill development coalesce to shape STEM trajectories. Thereby, this study bridges critical research gaps in education and behavioral research by reconceptualizing youths’ educational expectations and science performance as dynamic, co-evolving processes during middle school. It reveals how these developmental and growth trajectories synergistically predict youths’ later STEM degree attainment—a contribution that challenges static conceptual frameworks while advancing life course and Bayesian learning theories.
To the authors’ knowledge, this is the first empirical inquiry to conceptualize educational expectations and science performance as time-varying constructs and rigorously test their dynamic interplay during adolescence. By modeling these relationships through a longitudinal framework, the study advances understanding of how cumulative and progressive processes in early academic motivation and science learning—and their interdependencies—spill over into long-term STEM outcomes. The findings of this study help provide insights for how to design policies and plan educational interventions in nurturing educational aspirations and foundational science competencies of youths systematically in early school years with an aim to prepare their later STEM development across the life course.
The current study
Taking what has been reviewed above together, we planned to take a dynamic approach in this study to investigate the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance across middle school in prediction of their later successful graduation with a STEM degree in adulthood. This dynamic approach means that youths’ educational expectations and science performance are individually varying at standing levels (the development) in middle school and are intrapersonally changeable (the growth) across middle school, which helps scrutinize the importance of cultivability and malleability of educational motivations and academic performance during early school years in relation to youths’ long-term academic achievement, e.g., STEM degree attainment in adult life (Affuso et al., 2025; Chykina, 2019; Perinelli et al., 2022). In addition, as educational expectations are empirically reported a contributor to youths’ academic performance in school years (Dotterer, 2022; Perinelli et al., 2022; Zhang, 2014), which both are important for youths’ later completion of a college degree (Kim and Fong, 2014; Sommerfeld, 2016; Zhan and Sherraden, 2011; Zhang, 2022), we expect that the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ science performance would mediate the relationship between the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and their STEM degree completion in adulthood. Due to the cumulative and progressive processes of youths’ educational expectations and science performance in school, we hypothesize that the initial developmental trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance lead to their growth in these trajectories across middle school. Accordingly, we set the hypotheses as follows:
H1: The development and growth of youths’ educational expectations in middle school would positively lead to their development and growth in science performance across middle school.
H2: The development and growth of youths’ educational expectations and their science performance during middle school would positively predict their later successful graduation with a STEM degree in adulthood.
H3: The development and growth of youths’ science performance would mediate the effects of their development and growth in educational expectations during middle school on their later successful graduation with a STEM degree in adulthood.
H4: The growth of youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school would be a function of their initial development of educational expectations and science performance in middle school, anticipating that the initial development of youths’ educational expectations and science performance would predict their growth in educational expectations and science performance across middle school.
To ensure the estimated relationships between educational trajectories and STEM outcomes reflect core theoretical processes rather than sociodemographic disparities, we account for key contextual factors central to STEM equity research, which include youths’ gender, family composition, parental SES, and ethnicity (Ji, 2021; Luo et al., 2022; Penprase, 2020). These factors were included as statistical controls to isolate the unique predictive effects of motivational and performance trajectories while acknowledging their established roles in systemic opportunity gaps. The rationales are that female students are reported to have higher educational motivation and academic performance than their male counterparts (Atchia and Chinapah, 2023; Keung and Ho, 2019). Moreover, youths living in two-parent families with a biological mother and father and having higher parental SES were found to have higher educational expectations and better academic achievement compared to their student counterparts of other family structures and lower parental SES (Gu et al., 2024; Renzulli and Barr, 2017; Sun and Li, 2011; Zhang et al., 2021). Additionally, Asian youths are reported to have stronger academic motivation and better educational performance, especially in STEM development, compared to their peers of other ethnic origins (Feliciano and Lanuza, 2017; Hsieh and Simpkins, 2022). Thereby, the current study classified youths into five ethnic origins: White, African American, Hispanic, Asian, and Native American, with Asian youths as the reference group due to their higher academic motivation and educational outperformance (Feliciano and Lanuza, 2017; Portes and Rumbaut, 2014). Youth participants’ age was excluded as a control variable in the modeling procedures to mitigate potential collinearity, as educational expectations and science performance are time-varying constructs inherently aligned with school years and chronological age (Wang and Wang, 2019).
Methods
Data and sample
The present study utilized data from the Longitudinal Study of American Youth (LSAY), a nationwide representative study of public middle- and high-school students in the United States (Miller, 2014). LSAY surveyed two cohorts beginning in 1987: The first cohort included 2829 high school students in the 10th grade, and the second cohort recruited 3116 middle school students in the 7th grade. The longitudinal interviews were conducted annually from 1987 to 1994 (spanning 7 years). Cohort-1 participants were tracked for 3 years of high school and 4 years post-graduation after high school, while cohort-2 participants were followed for 3 years of middle school, 3 years of high school, and 1year postgraduation of high school. In 2006, the National Science Foundation (NSF) provided additional funding to trace the educational and occupational development of the cohort-1 and 2 participants in LSAY. Five additional longitudinal surveys were conducted in 2007, 2008, 2009, 2010, and 2011. In 2007, LSAY successfully relocated around 95% of the original sample of cohort-1 and 2 students (N = 5945), and these youth students were in an age range between 33 and 37 years old. LSAY employed a stratified sampling framework from a national population of public middle and high schools in 12 sampling strata defined by the geographic region and type of community in the country to randomly draw student participants as the final sample that was representative of the public middle- and high-school student populations in the United States. The original sample comprised ~48% female and 52% male students, and 70% Whites, 17% African Americans, 9% Hispanics, 3% Asian Americans, and 1% Native Americans. The present study used the data of cohort-2 students as the study sample because they contain pertinent information regarding educational expectations and science learning in middle school years and STEM development in adulthood.
Measures
Educational expectations of youths were measured annually from grades 7 through 9 by an item provided in LSAY that is “As things stand now, how far in school do you think you will get?” It was rated on a 6-point scale: 1 = high school only, 2 = vocational/ trade school, 3 = some college, 4 = bachelor’s degree, 5 = master’s degree, and 6 = doctorate/professional degree, which means higher responses represent higher educational expectations. The measurement item is consistent with the Wisconsin model to assess educational expectations of youths (Liu et al., 2020; O’Donnell et al., 2022; Roth, 2017), which has been commonly used to rate educational expectancy and aspirations in existing empirical research (Feliciano and Lanuza, 2016; Karlson, 2015; Liu et al., 2020).
Science performance of youths was assessed annually from grades 7 to 9 using standardized scores provided by the LSAY personnel, capturing students’ proficiency in core STEM disciplines: biology, chemistry, physical science, and environmental science. These foundational subjects are critical for future STEM engagement and development (Ayuso et al., 2022; Liversidge, 2009). Scores were derived via item-response theory (IRT) methodology to adjust for measurement reliability, guessing, and item difficulty (DeMars, 2010). To ensure cross-cohort comparability, LSAY employed BILOG-MG software (Zimowski et al., 1996) to recalibrate scores using multiple-group IRT modeling (MGM). This aligned Cohort-1 students’ middle school performance with high school benchmarks and integrated middle-to-high school data for Cohort-2 students (the study sample). Annual score ranges spanned 26–88 (7th grade), 22–83 (8th grade), and 27–91 (9th grade), with higher scores reflecting better science achievement.
STEM degree completion in college was measured in 2007 by an item provided by the LSAY personnel to indicate whether the participants had successfully graduated with a four-year STEM baccalaureate degree in science, technology, engineering, mathematics, or medicine. This dichotomous classification of graduates with a STEM degree compared to their counterparts otherwise has been used in prior research (Luo et al., 2022; Wright et al., 2017), which is coded: 0 = no baccalaureate or non-STEMM major and 1 = STEMM major.
Contextual factors of youths’ gender, family composition, parental SES, and ethnicity were included in the modeling procedures for precluding confounding effects. Youths’ gender (0=female, 1=male) and family composition (0 = otherwise, 1 = two-parent family) are dichotomous variables. Parental SES is a continuous variable measured by the Duncan’s Socioeconomic Index (SEI) to calculate parental responses of educational level, income, and occupational prestige (Caston, 1989). SEI is constructed by weighting an occupation’s median education and income on the metric of occupational prestige (Caston, 1989), which has been widely used in empirical research to indicate socioeconomic status, with higher scores indicating better parental SES (Montero et al., 2021; Pitt and Zhu, 2019). Youths’ ethnicity is classified into four ethnic dummy variables, with Asian youths serving as reference (0) and White, African American, Hispanic, and Native American youths setting as comparison (1).
Modeling procedures
Given the longitudinal design of this study, parallel-process latent growth curve modeling (PP-LGCM) was employed to examine how the developmental (initial levels) and growth trajectories (rates of changes) in youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school (grades 7–9) jointly predict later successful graduation with a STEM degree in adulthood. PP-LGCM, belonging to growth modeling, is a robust modeling procedure to analyze longitudinal relationships between evolving constructs and their association with distal outcomes (Grimm et al., 2017), such as STEM degree completion. The basic latent growth curve model (LGCM) is expressed as:
where \(Y\) is observed scores, \({\tau }_{y}\) denotes population means of \(Y\), \({\varLambda }_{y}\) contains factor loadings, \(\eta\) includes latent intercept (\({\eta }_{0})\) and slope (\({\eta }_{1}\)) factors of youths’ educational expectations and/or science learning performance, and \(\varepsilon\) represents residuals. Intercept factors capture initial levels, while slope factors quantify changes over time. As both educational expectations and science performance of youths were measured repeatedly in an equal time interval from grades 7 through 9, the time scores were set to be t0, t1, and t2 across the middle school years in the current study.
PP-LGCM extends LGCM by simultaneously modeling two parallel processes that include the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance within a unified framework in the current study, which can be written as:
The first equation is the within-subject model, in which \({Y}_{{ti}}^{m}\) indicates the educational expectations of youth and their science performance observed in middle school, and \({\eta }_{0i}^{{y}_{m}}\) in the second equation and \({\eta }_{1i}^{{y}_{m}}\) in the third equation are the model parameters representing the latent intercepts and slopes of the mth growth process that refers to youths’ educational expectations and science performance. The second and third equations denote the between-subject models, in which \({\eta }_{0}^{{y}_{m}}\) and \({\eta }_{1}^{{y}_{m}}\) are the dependent variables of the intercept and slope factors regarding youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school years, \({\eta }_{0}^{{y}_{m}}\) is the estimated overall initial levels of youths’ educational expectations and science performance in middle school (the development of youths’ educational expectations and science performance), \({\eta }_{1}^{{y}_{m}}\) is the average rate of changes referring to youths’ educational expectations and science performance across middle school (the growth of youths’ educational expectations and science performance), and \({\varsigma }_{0i}^{{y}_{m}}\) and \({\varsigma }_{{1}_{i}}^{{y}_{m}}\) are the residuals.
To combine the above equations with a distal outcome of youths’ successful graduation with a STEM degree in adulthood, we have:
where \({\eta }_{{Di}}\) is the distal outcome of students’ graduation with a STEM degree, \({\beta }_{1}{\eta }_{{Ii}}\) and \({\beta }_{2}{\eta }_{{Si}}\) are the regression parameters of the intercept and slope factors of youths’ educational expectations and science performance to represent their developmental and growth trajectories in middle school, and \({\beta }_{3}{z}_{i}\) is the regression parameters for the contextual factors of youths’ gender, family composition, parental SES, and ethnicity. These contextual factors were adjusted in the modeling procedures to preclude background confounding effects on the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance and their later STEM degree completion in adulthood, with \({a}_{D0}\) indicating as the intercept of the distal STEM attainment outcome and \({\xi }_{{Di}}\) referring as the person-specific difference between \({\eta }_{{Di}}\) and \({a}_{D0}\) respectively.
The modeling procedures were fit in Mplus 8.10 (Muthen and Muthen, 1998–2017). To address the nested structure of the LSAY data (clustered at the school level), the COMPLEX function with <TYPE = COMPLEX> was applied to adjust standard errors and chi-square tests for interdependence (Wang and Wang, 2019). Missing values are the concern of longitudinal research. In this study, missing data (1.3–12.7%) indicated low attrition rates, consistent with longitudinal population-based studies (Gustavson et al., 2012). Traditional Little’s test (Little and Rubin, 2002) is commonly used to evaluate missingness patterns, e.g., missing completely at random (MCAR), missing at random (MAR), or missing not at random (MNAR), for missing data. However, it is recently found to risk biased MCAR rejection due to its sensitivity to large sample size and nonnormality of longitudinal data (Ahmad and Zhang, 2021; Enders, 2022). Instead, we adopted the TestMCARNormality procedure in MissMech package of R programming to test the pattern of missing data (Jamshidian et al., 2014), which accounts for longitudinal data complexities, temporal dependencies, and large-scale samples via bootstrapping. The result supports the MCAR assumption of the data used in this study (p = 0.099)Footnote 1. Missing data were handled using full information maximum likelihood (FIML), a likelihood-based method that leverages all available participant data for efficient estimation (Lee and Shi, 2021), thereby preserving statistical power and minimizing bias under the assumption that data were missing at MCAR or MAR. In the context of growth modeling, FIML can accommodate missingness due to attrition or sporadic non-response by estimating model parameters directly from the observed data likelihood, avoiding the need for ad hoc procedures like listwise deletion or imputation, which can distort results (Enders, 2022). Model fit was evaluated by comparative fit index (CFI), root mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean-square residual (SRMR). Acceptable model fit is: CFI > 0.90, RMSEA < 0.08, and SRMR < 0.1; and excellent model fit is: CFI > 0.95, RMSEA < 0.06, and SRMR < 0.08 (Wang and Wang, 2019).
Results
Descriptive analysis
Descriptive statistics are summarized in Table 1. The sample comprised 48% female (n = 1495) and 52% male (n = 1621) youths. Most participants (87.1%, n = 2715) resided in two-parent households, with 12.9% (n = 401) from other family structures. Parental socioeconomic status (SES) averaged M = 39.72 (SD = 16.575). Ethnically, the majority identified as White (69.6%, n = 2169), followed by African American (16.2%, n = 504), Hispanic (9.1%, n = 284), Asian (3.5%, (n = 112), and Native American (1.5%, n = 47). Youths’ educational expectations showed a slight decline across grades: M = 4.011 (Grade 7), M = 4.005 (Grade 8), and \(M\) = 3.886 (Grade 9). Conversely, science performance increased progressively (M = 49.85 in Grade 7, M = 53.28 in Grade 8, M = 57.44 in Grade 9). By 2007, only 9% of youths (n = 281) had attained a four-year STEM baccalaureate degree. The decrease in educational expectations across middle school may reflect youth students more realistically perceiving their educational development gradually along with the harder academic standards required in higher grades, even with their improvement in science performance observed.
Unconditional LGCM models of educational expectations and science performance
Table 2 presents standardized results of the unconditional latent growth curve model (LGCM) analyzing youths’ educational expectations (Model 1 A). The model demonstrated excellent fit (CFI = 1.000, RMSEA = 0.000, SRMR = 0.000). Factor loadings for the intercept factor (grade 7–9 educational expectations) were significant (λ = 0.857, 0.886, 0.826; p < 0.001), while slope factor loadings (grade 8–9) were λ = 0.227 and 0.424 (p < 0.001). A significant negative correlation emerged between intercept and slope factors (r = −0.167, p < 0.01), suggesting that youths with higher initial development of educational expectations exhibited slower subsequent growth. Both intercept (σ² = 1.456) and slope (σ² = 0.096) variances were significant (p < 0.001), indicating substantial individual variation. Table 3 summarizes LGCM results for youths’ science performance (Model 1B), which also showed excellent model fit (CFI = 1.000, RMSEA = 0.000, SRMR = 0.000). Intercept factor loadings (grade 7–9 scores) were λ = 0.964, 0.908, 0.904 (p < 0.001), with slope loadings (grade 8–9) at λ = 0.292 and 0.581 (p < 0.001). Similarly, a negative intercept-slope correlation (r = −0.134, p < 0.001) implied that higher initial development of science performance was related to slower later growth. Significant variances for intercept (σ² = 91.419) and slope (σ² = 10.080) factors (p < 0.001) further highlighted individual differences. Collectively, both models fit the data well, accurately capturing developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school.
Conventional PP-LGCM model of educational expectations and science performance
A conventional parallel-process latent growth curve model (Conventional PP-LGCM) was estimated to examine associations between youths’ educational expectations and science performance in middle school (Model 2). This model regressed the intercept and slope factors of science performance on the intercept factor of educational expectations, and the science performance slope factor on the educational expectations slope factor (Figs. 1–3). The model demonstrated good fit: CFI = 0.997, RMSEA = 0.049, SRMR = 0.018. As shown in Fig. 1, the intercept of educational expectations positively predicted both the intercept (β = 0.424, p < 0.001) and slope factors (β = 0.065, p < 0.01) of science performance, indicating that higher initial development of educational expectations was associated with better initial development and modestly faster growth in science performance. Additionally, the slope of educational expectations positively predicted the science performance slope (β = 0.141, p < 0.001), suggesting that greater growth in educational expectations parallelly led to greater gains in science performance. The factor loadings were robust: educational expectations intercept (λ = 0.845, 0.874, 0.815) and slope (λ = 0.196 and 0.366), and science performance intercept (λ = 0.955, 0.909, 0.899) and slope (λ = 0.281 and 0.556), all p < 0.001. These results provide empirical justification for estimating conditional PP-LGCM modeling to predict distal STEM degree attainment. Full parameter estimates of Model 2, including cross-domain regressions and within-domain covariances, are reported in Table S1 in Appendix.
Note. For model fit, CFI = 0.997, RMSEA = 0.049, SRMR = 0.018. InpEdExp = Intercept Factor of Youths’ Educational Expectations; SlpEdExp= Slope Factor of Youths’ Educational Expectations; InpScPef= Intercept Factor of Youths’ Science Performance; SlpScPef= Slope Factor of Youths’ Science Performance. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Note. For model fit, CFI = 0.997, RMSEA = 0.026, SRMR = 0.011. InpEdExp = Intercept Factor of Youths’ Educational Expectations; SlpEdExp = Slope Factor of Youths’ Educational Expectations; InpScPef = Intercept Factor of Youths’ Science Performance; SlpScPef = Slope Factor of Youths’ Science Performance. Dotted arrows are the effects of youths’ gender, family composition, parental SES, and ethnicity on the intercept and slope factors of youths’ expectations and science performance in middle school years and their STEM degree attainment in adulthood. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Note. For model fit, CFI = 0.997, RMSEA = 0.026, SRMR = 0.011. InpEdExp= Intercept Factor of Youths’ Educational Expectations; SlpEdExp = Slope Factor of Youths’ Educational Expectations; InpScPef = Intercept Factor of Youths’ Science Performance; SlpScPef = Slope Factor of Youths’ Science Performance. Dotted arrows are the effects of youth gender, family composition, parental SES, and ethnicity on the intercept and slope factors of youths’ expectations and science performance in middle school years and their STEM degree attainment in adulthood. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Conditional PP-LGCM models of educational expectations and science performance in prediction of STEM degree completion
A conditional parallel-process latent growth curve model (Conditional PP-LGCM) was estimated to examine how youths’ educational expectations, science performance, and contextual factors (youths’ gender, family composition, parental SES, and ethnicity) predict STEM degree completion in adulthood (Model 3 A). STEM degree completion was modeled as an ordered categorical outcome. The model demonstrated excellent fit: CFI = 0.997, RMSEA = 0.026, SRMR = 0.011. Fig. 2 shows that higher initial development of youths’ educational expectations (β = 0.148, p < 0.001) and science performance (β = 0.150, p < 0.001) in middle school predicted greater odds of STEM degree attainment (15.95% and 16.18% per standard deviation increase, respectively). Similarly, growth in educational expectations (β = 0.087, p < 0.01) and science performance (β = 0.045, p < 0.05) predicted modest increases in STEM degree attainment (9.08% and 5% per SD). Full parameter estimates of Model 3A are reported in Table S2 in Appendix. To test whether growth in educational expectations and science performance depends on its initial developmental trajectories, a conditional and directional parallel-process latent growth curve model (Conditional and Directional PP-LGCM) was conducted to regress the slope factors of educational expectations and science performance on their respective intercept factors while retaining the structure of Model 3A intact (Model 3B). The model fit of this conditional and directional PP-LGCM model was excellent: CFI = 0.997, RMSEA = 0.026, SRMR = 0.011. Fig. 3 shows that the developmental trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance negatively predicted the growth rates of educational expectations (β = −0.186, p < 0.001) and science performance (β = −0.165, p < 0.001), respectively, indicating that higher baseline development of educational expectations and science performance were associated with slower subsequent growth. Notably, the effect of educational expectations’ intercept on science performance’s slope strengthened in Model 3B (β = 0.151 vs. 0.091 in Model 3 A; p < 0.001), suggesting that modeling intercept-slope relationships as directional (vs. correlational) better captures the developmental trajectories in educational expectations and science performance. Besides, other parts of Model 3B had similar effects to those of Model 3A. See Table S3 for the details of parameter estimates of Model 3B in Appendix.
Effects of youths’ contextual factors
For the effects of youth gender, family composition, parental SES, and ethnicity, male youths showed higher science performance (β = 0.217, p < 0.001) and greater STEM degree attainment (β = 0.065, p < 0.01; OR = 1.069), and youths from two-parent families (β = 0.037, p < 0.01; OR = 1.037) and of higher parental SES (β = 0.080, p < 0.01; OR = 1.083) had greater STEM odds. In addition, higher parental SES contributed to stronger developmental trajectories of youths’ educational expectations (β = 0.322, p < 0.001) and science performance (β = 0.144, p < 0.001). Hispanic, African American, White, and Native American youths reported lower developmental trajectories of educational expectations (β = −0.148 to −0.051, p < 0.001–0.05) and science performance (Hispanic: β = −0.139; African American: β = −0.210, p < 0.001) compared to Asian peers. These groups also had reduced odds of STEM degree completion (OR = 0.913–0.894, p < 0.01).
Mediation analysis
Latent mediation tests (Table 4) revealed significant indirect effects of educational expectations’ intercept on successful graduation with a STEM degree via science performance’s intercept (β = 0.055, p < 0.001) and slope (β = 0.007, p < 0.05). A marginally significant mediation emerged for science performance’s intercept and slope (β = −0.003, p < 0.10). While the slope-to-slope path from educational expectations to science performance was nonsignificant (β = −0.003, p = 0.131), the total indirect effect was significant (β = 0.058, p < 0.001), indicating that science performance of youths jointly mediated the link between their educational expectations and the STEM outcome.
Monte Carlo simulation for power test and sensitivity analysis
The application of parallel-process latent growth curve modeling (PP-LGCM) to examine dynamic longitudinal relationships between youths’ educational expectations, science performance, and STEM degree attainment is a sophisticated analytical approach. Given the complexity of these longitudinal models, we conducted Monte Carlo simulations to assess (1) statistical power in detecting true parameter estimates and (2) model reliability and stability (sensitivity analysis). Conventional analytical-based methods for power test (e.g., multivariate methods and exact tests) cannot handle the complexities of growth modeling employed in the current study as they depend on the priori and deterministic method (Arend and Schäfer, 2019; Carsey and Harden, 2014). Moreover, employing Monte Carlo simulations by testing varying sample sizes to verify model reliability and stability is an effective way of sensitivity analysis to corroborate replications in complicated mixed-effects and growth models (Lee and Hong, 2021). Hence, Monte Carlo simulation is adopted to conduct power test and sensitivity analysis for Model 3B, the final growth model in the current studyFootnote 2.
For power test, a total of 4000 replications were applied for repeated random sampling in the simulation (by setting seed = 12345) with the original sample size (N = 3116) to generate synthetic datasetsFootnote 3. The parameter estimates of the fitted PP-LGCM Model 3B were input in the population model and analysis model to verify their statistical power of replications. The results showed that all the focused parameter estimates regarding the regression effects between the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance as well as STEM degree completion were all found to have a statistical power > 0.80, ranging from \(\hat{P}=0.897{to}1.000\) (Table 5). Bias in parameter estimates (0–1.91%) and standard errors (0–3.62%) all fell below the 5% benchmark, indicating high precision. The coverage rates of 95% confidence intervals (95% Coverage) that include the true population parameter values from replications were between 0.942 and 0.963, well within the 0.91 to 0.98 threshold. In addition, the average model fit across replications was excellent: CFI = 1.000, RMSEA = 0.004, SRMR = 0.004, The results of power test indicate the sample size (N = 3116) employed in the current study is well adequate to detect true parameter estimatesFootnote 4. To ensure simulation reliability, we repeated the simulation analyses with 5000 and 10,000 replications. The results of these replications all meet the required Monte Carlo criteria.
We further conducted Monte Carlo sensitivity analysis to test model robustness through simulating smaller and larger samples (with N = 2500, 4000, 5000, and 7000)Footnote 5 for checking reliability and stability of growth modeling analyzed in the current study. Table 6 shows the results of Monte Carlo sensitivity analysis for Model 3B. The average model fits across varying samples were excellent (CFIs= 1.000, RMSEAs = 0.002 to 0.004, and SRMRs = 0.003 to 0.004) and parameter recovery was highly stable as the ranges of bias in parameter estimates and standard errors were all less than 5% across different sample sizes. Besides, 95% confidence intervals (CIs = 0.943 to 0.960) were all within the 0.91 and 0.98 threshold, suggesting consistent coverage even with reduced Ns. The statistical powers of different sample sizes ranged from 0.829 to 1.000, demonstrating reliability across conditions. The results of Monte Carlo sensitivity analysis support the reliability and stability of Model 3B in terms of model structure and parameter estimates for the longitudinal relationships between developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance in contribution to later STEM degree attainment in adulthood. Full Mplus code and result outputs of all the growth models and Monte Carlo simulation analyses for power test and sensitivity analysis are available at https://osf.io/m4eyn/files/osfstorage.
Discussion
The cultivation of STEM talent is crucial for national and global socioeconomic, cultural, and technological advancement (Larkin and Lowrie, 2022; Penprase, 2020). While prior studies have identified factors influencing youth STEM development, little research has explored how middle school trajectories of educational expectations and science performance jointly shape later STEM degree attainment of youths in adulthood. Existing work often treats these factors as static rather than dynamic (Kohen and Nitzan, 2022; Premraj et al., 2021). This study demonstrates that the developmental and growth trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school are critical contributors to adult STEM degree completion. Furthermore, the trajectories of science performance are significantly influenced by concurrent development and growth in educational expectations, with both pathways jointly enhancing the likelihood of earning a STEM degree. These findings underscore the need for educational strategies that nurture both academic performance and aspirational growth during middle school, as these dynamic trajectories collectively lay the groundwork for long-term STEM success.
Evidently, educational expectations are a well-established driver of academic performance (Park, 2021; Reynolds and Johnson, 2011). The current study extends this understanding by demonstrating that higher initial development of general educational expectations in middle school predict better development and steeper growth in science performance during early adolescence. Critically, these general expectations—reflecting youths’ overarching beliefs about their academic potential and the value of educational attainment (Wisconsin Model; Roth, 2017)—were robust predictors of later STEM degree completion, even when measured independently of STEM-specific aspirations. This aligns with SEVT’s principle that broad achievement beliefs scaffold domain-specific motivations (Wigfield and Eccles, 2023), particularly during early adolescence when STEM identities are nascent. Further, growth in educational expectations itself significantly predicts growth in science performance. These findings are particularly salient given the heightened malleability of educational expectations during middle school compared to later developmental stages (Andrew and Hauser, 2011; Pyne et al., 2018), underscoring the importance of cultivating positive general educational expectations of students in early adolescence to bolster science achievement and long-term STEM outcomes.
Parallel-process latent growth curve modeling (PP-LGCM) analyses revealed that initial development (intercept factors) of expectations and science performance shapes their subsequent growth (slopes), respectively. While higher initial levels were associated with slower growth rates, both intercepts and slopes independently contributed to STEM degree attainment. This highlights the dual significance of fostering strong aspirational and science learning foundations and sustaining growth trajectories during middle school to support later STEM success. This study further demonstrates that growth trajectories in general educational expectations during middle school significantly predict STEM degree attainment in adulthood. This relationship operates through two key, interrelated mechanisms: (1) General expectations foster persistence in science learning, building foundational competence necessary for advanced STEM study (as evidenced by mediation via science performance trajectories); and (2) They establish a credentialing pathway—youths with higher expectations are more likely to pursue and complete college degrees, thereby accessing the institutional gateway to STEM majors (Bass and Besen-Cassino, 2016; Khattab et al., 2022). These findings corroborate evidence that the link between adolescent expectations, science proficiency, and adult STEM achievement is inherently developmental and cumulative (Dokme et al., 2022; Miller and Pearson, 2012). Given the formative role of middle school in shaping educational and career motivations and directions, policymakers and educators must prioritize these dynamic trajectories when designing interventions (Bozick et al., 2010; Penprase, 2020). To this end, fostering supportive ecosystems—encompassing schools, families, and communities—is essential for cultivating expectations and science engagement during early adolescence, thereby anchoring equity-driven educational reforms (Ji, 2021; Zhou et al., 2019).
Latent mediation analyses revealed that both the developmental and growth trajectories of science performance mediated the effects of educational expectations on STEM degree attainment, both collectively and independently. These findings offer actionable insights for advancing science education and STEM equity (Anderson and Li, 2020; Penprase, 2020). The significant relationship between general educational expectations and STEM attainment challenges the assumption that only domain-specific motivations (e.g., science self-efficacy) are relevant for STEM pathways. Instead, it highlights the foundational role of broad academic aspirations in creating the preconditions for STEM engagement: Sustained effort in foundational science courses (mediating the expectation-STEM link) and progression into higher education where STEM specialization occurs. Beyond fostering direct engagement in STEM, policymakers and educators should design programs that cultivate general educational expectations as foundational drivers of motivation—for instance, through mentorship initiatives or project-based learning—to amplify student agency and sustained growth in science proficiency. While the indirect path linking growth in educational expectations (slopes) to STEM attainment via science performance growth (slopes) was nonsignificant, the total indirect effect—combining initial levels and growth trajectories—underscores the cumulative and progressive role of middle school expectations and science development in shaping adult STEM outcomes. This highlights the need for early, targeted efforts to strengthen general educational expectations at the onset of adolescence. Such efforts may catalyze cascading gains in both educational expectations and science performance, ultimately enhancing STEM achievement. In fact, by modeling general educational expectations as dynamic trajectories, this study reveals how their cumulative development during middle school—propelled by recursive feedback loops where early academic success reinforces aspirations (Eccles and Wigfield, 2020)—creates momentum toward college completion, within which STEM degree attainment becomes a viable pathway. This broader credentialing effect complements the direct motivational influence on science mastery, underscoring why general expectations uniquely predict long-term STEM degree outcomes even after accounting for science performance.
The current study provides robust evidence for the long-term effects of youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school on their later STEM degree completion in adulthood. Using a nationally representative longitudinal sample, we found that both the initial development (intercepts) and growth in (slopes) educational expectations and science performance significantly predicted STEM attainment, with standardized effects ranging from β = 0.045 to 0.150. While these effects may appear modest by conventional benchmarks (e.g., Cohen’s guidelines), they are substantively meaningful given the long-term, population-level nature of the study. For example, a 1-standard-deviation increase in the intercept of science performance (β = 0.150) corresponds to a 15% higher likelihood of STEM degree completion, which could translate to thousands of additional STEM graduates in a national cohort. While few studies have examined the evolving trajectories of educational expectations and science performance as predictors of STEM attainment in adulthood, our findings resonate with, yet critically extend, prior research examining static predictors of STEM outcomes. For example, Jiang and Simpkins (2024) reported comparable effect sizes (β = 0.11–0.15) for students’ self-concept in mathematics and science abilities—measured at discrete time points (grades 9 and 11)—on STEM major enrollment. Relevantly, Moakler and Kim (2014) identified modest effects of academic confidence (β = 0.14) and high school GPA (β = 0.15) on STEM major selection in cross-sectional analyses. However, by modeling educational expectations and science performance as evolving trajectories dynamically rather than static snapshots, our study uniquely reveals that their rates of change (slopes: β = 0.087 and 0.045, respectively) independently contribute to long-term STEM attainment. This underscores the necessity of conceptualizing academic motivation and achievement as fluid processes, where cumulative gains (or declines) over time amplify their impact on career pathways. These results challenge the reliance on single-timepoint measurements in prior literature and advocate for educational policies and interventions that nurture both initial competencies and sustained growth in science engagement and aspirations during adolescence.
Manifestly, our findings underscore the critical role of educational expectations and science performance during middle school in shaping later STEM degree attainment. To leverage these insights, schools should integrate aspirational support with skill-building—for instance, through mentorship initiatives or project-based learning (PBL) workshops to connect classroom science to real-world challenges for middle school students (Larkin and Lowrie, 2022). In addition, schools should consider integrating science performance with aspirational support by pairing academic skill-building with interventions (Ayuso et al., 2022; English, 2017), such as growth mindset training and STEM industry and school partnership schemes, to boost students’ confidence and aspirations as well as science competence. Besides, schoolteachers and educators should be equipped to acknowledge and support both aspirational and science learning development of middle school students, which are crucial to provide timely assistance and remedies for struggling student learners to catch up their aspirational and STEM development (Ozulku and Kloser, 2024). More importantly, longitudinal tracking and accountability of students’ development and progression in educational expectations and academic performance (including science learning) during middle school are pivotal to monitoring academic readiness and inform resource allocation for facilitating students’ learning trajectories of STEM development. This is critical to cultivate next-generational STEM professionals due to the cumulative and progressive nature of STEM development found in this study. Moreover, policymakers must prioritize funding for middle school STEM resources, particularly in underserved communities, to address equity gaps (English, 2017; Larkin and Lowrie, 2022). Early identification systems can flag students at risk of declining engagement, enabling timely interventions such as mentorship and learning guidance initiatives. By aligning educational practices with these dual pathways of educational expectations and science performance, stakeholders can cultivate a robust STEM pipeline, ensuring students are well prepared earlier to meet future workforce demands.
Besides, this study highlights the contextual influences on youths’ educational expectations, science performance, and STEM degree attainment. Male youths demonstrated higher STEM degree attainment and stronger initial development in science performance, reflecting persistent gender disparities often attributed to gendered academic preferences (de las Cuevas et al., 2022; Dokme et al., 2022; Owen, 2023). However, recent studies challenge these patterns, reporting comparable science interest and ability across genders (Zhao and Perez-Felkner, 2022). Family background also played a pivotal role: youths from two-parent families and higher socioeconomic status (SES) households exhibited greater STEM attainment and stronger initial development in academic trajectories. This underscores the role of home resources, family socialization, and parental human capital as critical assets for STEM development (Ji, 2021; Luo et al., 2022; Miller and Pearson, 2012). Ethnic disparities further emerged, with Asian youths showing higher baseline expectations and STEM attainment—a pattern linked to cultural emphasis on academic achievement and communal educational support (Feliciano and Lanuza, 2016; Portes and Rumbaut, 2014; Toyokawa and Toyokawa, 2019). These findings collectively advocate for policies that need to prioritize equitable resource allocation to mitigate contextual barriers, ensuring inclusive pathways to STEM success. Such measures are important for transforming education into a true equalizer, empowering students from diverse family, ethnic, and societal backgrounds.
Conclusion
Utilizing longitudinal data of a representative sample of middle school youths from the Longitudinal Study of American Youth (LSAY), spanning early adolescence to adulthood, the current study corroborated that youths’ educational expectations developmentally enhance science performance during middle school, which then later collectively foster STEM degree completion. Furthermore, science performance of youths in middle school emerged not only a direct predictor of later STEM degree attainment but also as a mediator linking educational expectations to the STEM outcome. Nevertheless, some limitations of the current study should be noted. First, although the LSAY sample is nationally representative, its exclusion of private school students leaves unclear whether developmental and growth trajectories of expectations, science learning, and STEM attainment differ across educational settings. Hence, replication with recent, multi-cohort data including students in public and private schools is critical to validate these dynamics in modern contexts. Second and more importantly, the findings of this study should be interpreted in light of potential cohort effects and historical context tied to the LSAY data, which spanned 1987–2011. Manifestly, the experiences of youth participants were shaped by era-specific factors, such as pre-internet educational practices and STEM policies now outdated by contemporary reforms (Larkin and Lowrie, 2022; Penprase, 2020). Consequently, generalizability to current educational environments—marked by technological integration (e.g., AI tools, online learning) and emerging equity initiatives-may be limited. Third, educational systems—particularly in science and STEM fields—have undergone significant reforms over the past decade (Ayuso et al., 2022; Klees, 2018). Consequently, contemporary data reflecting these shifts are essential to validate the generalizability of this study’s findings. Fourth, while this study examines the developmental trajectories of youths’ educational expectations and science performance during middle school as predictors of later STEM degree attainment, it is important to acknowledge that STEM outcomes are shaped by a complex interplay of interconnected systems, including familial, peer, societal, institutional, and intrapersonal influences. Future research should adopt multi-system frameworks to better disentangle these dynamic interactions across educational contexts (Anderson and Li, 2020; Penprase, 2020). Additionally, due to LSAY only using a single measurement item to evaluate educational expectations, it is suggested to employ well-validated multi-item measures to assess youths’ educational expectations in the future, as this would enable more nuanced comparisons with the current study’s findings by different approaches in measuring this construct. Finally, although middle school represents a critical period for fostering STEM motivations, these trajectories develop dynamically across the lifespan, spanning early childhood through postgraduate education (Larkin and Lowrie, 2022; Pallas, 2003). To inform comprehensive policy and educational reforms, future research should explore how intersecting systems—across various educational stages—interact to shape STEM achievement, thereby enabling the design of more nuanced and effective interventions, especially for those students under disadvantaged learning and less science-stimulated environments.
Data availability
The data of LSAY used in the current study can be accessible at https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/ICPSR/studies/30263/summary.
Notes
The TestMCARNormality procedure in MissMech package of R programming is a robust bootstrapping method to account patterned missingness and nonnormality in longitudinal data, which can more accurately test whether the MCAR assumption of missingness is held. The equation of TestMCARNormality procedure is written as \({{\rm{Q}}}_{2}=\mathop{\sum }\limits_{g=1}^{G}\frac{{n}_{g}}{2}\,\left[{t}_{r}\,\left({\hat{\varSigma }}^{-1}\,\hat{{\varSigma }_{g}}\right)-{In}\left|{\hat{\varSigma }}^{-1}\,\hat{{\varSigma }_{g}}\right|-{P}_{g}\right]\), in which the covariance comparison (Q2) is obtained by adjusting for sample size for pattern (ng), number of missingness patterns (G), estimated covariance matrix (\(\hat{\varSigma }\)), and number of observed variables in pattern g (Pg). Thereby, the procedure evaluates missingness patterns in a less biased way through taking longitudinal data complexities, temporal dependencies, and the sample scale into account. R code and result output can be found in the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/m4eyn/files/osfstorage.
Monte Carlo simulation (MCS) is a computational algorithm that leverages repeated stochastic sampling from probability distributions to probabilistically estimate outcomes by generating and analyzing large-scale synthetic datasets (e.g., thousands to millions of iterations). In the context of statistical power testing and sensitivity analysis, MCS serves as a robust methodological framework to evaluate (1) the reliability of inferential conclusions under uncertainty and (2) the influence of variability in parameter estimates and model assumptions on result precision and stability. Statistical power is computed as the proportion of simulations in which the null hypothesis (H0) is rejected at a specified significance threshold (e.g., α ≤ 0.05), relative to the total number of replications: \(\hat{P}=\,\frac{{\sum }_{k=1}^{K}1({P}^{\left(k\right)}\, < \alpha }{K}\), where \(\hat{P}\) is the estimated power, K denotes the total simulations, and 1(⋅) is an indicator function. In addition, based on the procedure of power test in Monte Carlo simulation, sensitivity analysis can be conducted by changing in inputs (e.g., effect sizes, missing data rates, and sample sizes) and examine how these inputs affect outputs (e.g., power, effect estimates).
To ensure the precision of power estimates derived from MCS, the required number of replications (NREPS) must be determined based on a predefined margin of error (ME). The equation to determine required replications with reference to margin ME for power estimates is \({NREPS}\ge {(\frac{{Z}_{\alpha /2}\times \sqrt{p(1-p)}}{{ME}})}^{2}\), where P is expected power and Zα/2 (e.g., P = 0.80) is critical value for confidence level (e.g., 1.96 for 95% CI). In this power test by Monte Carlo simulation, we set ME to 0.01 with a power of 0.80, which has \({NREPS}\ge {\left(\frac{1.96\times \sqrt{0.8\times 0.2}}{{ME}}\right)}^{2}\approx 3.843\), meaning that an approximation of 4000 replications is needed to ensure the empirical power estimate reliably reflecting the true parameter values with the specified precision. This approach minimizes sampling variability and enhances the robustness of inferences drawn from simulation study.
In Monte Carlo simulation for power test, three criteria need to be examined in determining whether a sample size is sufficient to detect true parameter values: (i) the estimated statistical power of all focused parameter estimates must be \(\hat{P}\) ≥ 0.08; (ii) the bias in parameter estimates and standard errors for the focused parameters should be less than 5%; and (iii) the coverage rates of 95% confidence intervals containing the true parameter values from replications must be within 0.91 to 0.98. Besides, the average model fit across 4000 replications is also considered as a criterion to support the adequacy of sample size (N = 3116) in detecting precision of parameter estimates.
Growth modeling, including the PP-LGCM models analyzed in the current study, is within the structural equation modeling (SEM) framework, which ascribes to large-sample theory. For this, we set the minimum sample size to N = 2500, which generates a statistical power of 0.829, indicating that if a sample size is <N = 2500, it is inadequate for statistical power to detect true parameter estimates.
References
Affuso G, Zannone A, Esposito C, De Angelis G, Dragone M, Pannone M, Miranda MC, Aquilar S, Bacchini D (2025) The reciprocal effects of learning motivation, perceived academic self-efficacy and academic performance in adolescence: a four-wave longitudinal study. Eur J Psychol Educ 40(1):6. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-024-00900-y
Ahmad Z, Zhang L-C (2021) Nonparametric estimation for longitudinal data with informative missingness. In: Lynn P (ed) Advances in Longitudinal Survey Methodology, pp 491–512. Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119376965
Anderson J, Li Y (2020) Integrated Approaches to STEM Education: An International Perspective. Springer International Publishing
Andrew M, Hauser RM (2011) Adoption? Adaptation? Evaluating the formation of educational expectations. Soc Forces 90(2):497–520. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/sor005
Arend MG, Schäfer T (2019) Statistical power in two-level models: a tutorial based on Monte Carlo simulation. Psychol Methods 24(1):1–19. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000195
Atchia SMC, Chinapah V (2023) Factors influencing the academic achievement of secondary school students: a structural equation model. Int J Instr 16(1):999–1020
Ayuso A, Merayo N, Ruiz I, Fernández P (2022) Challenges of STEM vocations in secondary education. IEEE Trans Educ 65(4):713–724. https://doi.org/10.1109/te.2022.3172993
Bass L, Besen-Cassino Y (2016) Education and youth today. Emerald Publishing Limited
Bozick R, Alexander K, Entwisle D, Dauber S, Kerr K (2010) Framing the future: revisiting the place of educational expectations in status attainment. Soc Forces 88(5):2027–2052. <Go to ISI>://WOS:000281058700004
Carolanl BV (2017) Assessing the adaptation of adolescents’ educational expectations: variations by gender. Soc Psychol Educ 20(2):237–257. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-017-9377-y
Carsey TM, Harden JJ (2014) Monte Carlo simulation and resampling methods for social science. SAGE. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781483319605
Caston RJ (1989) Dimensions of occupational inequality and Duncan Socioeconomic Index. Sociol Forum 4(3):329–348. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01115013
Chykina V (2019) Educational expectations of immigrant students: does tracking matter? Sociol Perspect 62(3):366–382. https://doi.org/10.1177/0731121419828397
De las Cuevas P, Garcia-Arenas M, Rico N (2022) Why not STEM? A study case on the influence of gender factors on students’ higher education choice. Mathematics 10(2):239. https://doi.org/10.3390/math10020239
DeMars C (2010) Item response theory. Oxford University Press
Dochow S, Neumeyer S (2021) An investigation of the causal effect of educational expectations on school performance. Behavioral consequences, time-stable confounding, or reciprocal causality? Res Soc Stratif Mobil 71:100579. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rssm.2020.100579
Dokme I, Aciksoz A, Koyunlu-Unlu Z (2022) Investigation of STEM fields motivation among female students in science education colleges. Int J STEM Educ 9(1):8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40594-022-00326-2
Dolby N, Rizvi F (2008) Youth moves: Identities and education in global perspective (Vol. 7). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203938836
Dotterer AM (2022) Parent involvement, expectancy values, and STEM outcomes among underrepresented adolescents. Soc Psychol Educ 25(1):113–127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-021-09677-0
Eccles J (2009) Who am I and what am I going to do with my life? Personal and collective identities as motivators of action [Article]. Educ Psychol 44(2):78–89. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520902832368
Eccles JS, Wigfield A (2020) From expectancy-value theory to situated expectancy-value theory: a developmental, social cognitive, and sociocultural perspective on motivation [Article]. Contemp Educ Psychol 61:101859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101859
Eccles JS, Wigfield A (2023) Expectancy-value theory to situated expectancy-value theory: reflections on the legacy of 40+ Years of working together [Article]. Motiv Sci 9(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000275
Enders CK (2022) Applied Missing Data Analysis. Guilford Press
English LD (2017) Advancing elementary and middle school STEM education. Int J Sci Math Educ 15:S5–S24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-017-9802-x
Feliciano C, Lanuza YR (2016) The immigrant advantage in adolescent educational expectations. Int Migr Rev 50(3):758–792. https://doi.org/10.1111/imre.12183
Feliciano C, Lanuza YR (2017) An immigrant paradox? Contextual attainment and intergenerational educational mobility. Am Sociol Rev 82(1):211–241. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122416684777
Fishman SH (2022) College-for-some or college-for-all?: Inequality in the relationship between educational expectations and educational attainment across academic achievement. Soc Sci Res 107:102747. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2022.102747
Grimm KJ, Ram N, Estabrook R (2017) Growth Modeling: Structural Equation and Multilevel Modeling Approaches. The Guilford Press
Gu XX, Hassan NC, Sulaiman T (2024) The relationship between family factors and academic achievement of junior high school students in rural China: mediation effect of parental involvement. Behav Sci 14(3):221. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14030221
Gustavson K, Soest TV, Karevold E, Røysamb E (2012) Attrition and generalizability in longitudinal studies: findings from a 15-year population-based study and a Monte Carlo simulation study. BMC Public Health 12:918. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-12-918
Hsieh TY, Simpkins SD (2022) The patterns of adolescents’ math and science motivational beliefs: examining within-racial/ethnic group changes and their relations to STEM outcomes. Aera Open 8:23328584221083673. https://doi.org/10.1177/23328584221083673
Jamshidian M, Jalal S, Jansen C (2014) MissMech: an R Package for testing homoscedasticity, multivariate normality, and Missing Completely at Random (MCAR). J Stat Softw 56(6):1–31. <Go to ISI>://WOS:000332109200001
Ji QN (2021) Supporting indigenous students in science and STEM education: a systematic review. Educ Sci 11(9):555. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11090555
Jiang S, Simpkins SD (2024) Examining changes in adolescents’ high school math and science motivational beliefs and their relations to parental STEM support and STEM major choice at the intersectionality of gender and college generation status. Dev Psychol 60(4):693–710. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001683
Karlson KB (2015) Expectations on track? High school tracking and adolescent educational expectations. Soc Forces 94(1):115–141. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/sov006
Keung CPC, Ho ESC (2019) Social capital and educational expectations exploring adolescents’ capabilities to aspire for postsecondary education. In: Bartee RD, George PL (eds). Contemporary Perspectives on Social Capital in Educational Contexts, pp 181–199
Khattab N, Madeeha M, Samara M, Modood T, Barham A (2022) Do educational aspirations and expectations matter in improving school achievement? Soc Psychol Educ 25(1):33–53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-021-09670-7
Kim SW, Fong VL (2014) Homework help, achievement in middle school, and later college attainment in China. Asia Pac Educ Rev 15(4):617–631. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12564-014-9350-9
Klees SJ (2018) Capitalism and global education reform. In: Saltman KJ, Means AJ (eds) The Wiley handbook of global educational reform, pp 11–26. Wiley
Kohen Z, Nitzan O (2022) Excellence in mathematics in secondary school and choosing and excelling in STEM professions over significant periods in life. Int J Sci Math Educ 20(1):169–191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-020-10138-x
Kosik R, Mandell G, Fan AP, Nguyen T, Chen J, Eaton W (2018) The association between childhood educational attainment and adult mental health and status: a thirty-year longitudinal follow up study. Eur J Psychiatry 32(2):53–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpsy.2018.01.001
Larkin K, Lowrie T (2022) STEM Education in the Early Years-Thinking About Tomorrow. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-19-2810-9
Larson LM, Pesch KM, Bonitz VS, Wu TF, Werbel JD (2014) Graduating with a science major: the roles of first-year science interests and educational aspirations. J Career Assess 22(3):479–488. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072713498680
Lauermann F, Tsai YM, Eccles JS (2017) Math-related career aspirations and choices within Eccles et al.‘s expectancy-value theory of achievement-related behaviors [Article]. Dev Psychol 53(8):1540–1559. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000367
LeBeau B, Harwell M, Monson D, Dupuis D, Medhanie A, Post TR (2012) Student and high-school characteristics related to completing a science, technology, engineering or mathematics (STEM) major in college. Res Sci Technol Educ 30(1):17–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/02635143.2012.659178
Lee E, Hong S (2021) Adequate sample sizes for a three-level growth model. Front Psychol 12:685496. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.685496
Lee T, Shi DX (2021) A comparison of full information maximum likelihood and multiple imputation in structural equation modeling with missing data. Psychol Methods 26(4):466–485. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000381
Little R, Rubin D (2002) Statistical analysis with missing data. Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119482260
Liu Q, Dong ZR, Han HW (2025) The influence of peers’ parental education on adolescent academic performance: the mediating role of educational aspirations. Soc Psychol Educ 28(1):36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-024-09995-z
Liu T, Chen XM, Liu MJ, Zhang YB, Xin T, Wang YH (2020) The effects of children’s self-educational aspiration and self-efficacy on mathematics achievement: a moderated chained mediation model. An De Psicologia 36(2):262–270. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.366621
Liversidge T (2009) Teaching science. SAGE, Los Angeles, CA
Luo LL, Stoeger H, Subotnik RF (2022) The influences of social agents in completing a STEM degree: an examination of female graduates of selective science high schools. Int J STEM Educ 9(1):7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40594-021-00324-w
Marsh HW (2023) Extending the reciprocal effects model of math self-concept and achievement: long-term implications for end-of-high-school, age-26 outcomes, and long-term expectations. J Educ Psychol 115(2):193–211. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000750
Mau WC, Domnick M, Ellsworth RA (1995) Characteristics of female students who aspire to science and engineering or homemaking occupations. Career Dev Q 43(4):323–337. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-0045.1995.tb00437.x
Menzies L, Baars S (2021) Young people on the margins: Priorities for action in education and youth. Taylor & Francis Group
Miller JD (2014) Longitudinal Study of American Youth, 1987–1994, 2007–2011: User guide. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research
Miller JD, Pearson WJ (2012) Pathways to STEMM professions for students from noncollege homes. Peabody J Educ 87(1):114–132. https://doi.org/10.1080/0161956X.2012.642277
Moakler MW, Kim MM (2014) College Major Choice in STEM: revisiting confidence and demographic factors. Career Dev Q 62(2):128–142. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-0045.2014.00075.x
Montero R, Vargas M, Vasquez D (2021) Segregation and life satisfaction. Front Psychol 11:604194. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.604194
Morgan SL (2005) On the Edge of Commitment: Educational Attainment and Race in the United States. Stanford University Press
Muthen LK, Muthen BO (1998-2017) Mplus user’s guide, 8th edn. Muthen & Muthen
O’Donnell AW, Redmond G, Thomson C, Wang JJJ, Turkmani S (2022) Reducing educational disparities between australian adolescents in regional and metropolitan communities: the compensatory effects of extracurricular activities. Dev Psychol 58(12):2358–2371. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001434
Owen S (2023) College major choice and beliefs about relative performance: an experimental intervention to understand gender gaps in STEM. Econ Educ Rev 97:102479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econedurev.2023.102479
Ozulku E, Kloser M (2023) Middle school students’ motivational dispositions and interest in STEM careers. International Journal of Science Education, Early Access. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2023.2234778
Ozulku E, Kloser M (2024) Middle school students’ motivational dispositions and interest in STEM careers. Int J Sci Educ 46(4):382–402. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2023.2234778
Pallas AM (2003) Educational transitions, trajectories, and pathways. In: JT Mortimer JT & Shanahan MJ (eds) Handbook of the Life Course, pp 165–184. Springer
Park K (2021) Adolescents’ relative position in school and educational attainment: the mediating role of educational expectations. Soc Sci Res 94(11):102520. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2020.102520
Penprase, BE (2020) STEM education for the 21st century. Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-41633-1
Perinelli E, Pisanu F, Checchi D, Scalas LF, Fraccaroli F (2022) Academic self-concept change in junior high school students and relationships with academic achievement. Contemp Educ Psychol 69:102071. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2022.102071
Pinquart M, Ebeling M (2020) Students’ expected and actual academic achievement-A meta-analysis. Int J Educ Res 100:101524. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2019.101524
Pitt RN, Zhu L (2019) The relationship between college major prestige/status and post-baccalaureate outcomes. Sociol Perspect 62(3):325–345. https://doi.org/10.1177/0731121418803325
Portes A, Rumbaut RG (2014) Immigrant America: A portrait,4th edn. University of California Press. https://doi.org/10.1525/j.ctt7zw0nw
Pov S, Kawai N, Chey R, Khut S, Nov S (2024) Examining factors influencing students to leave science classes in Cambodia: a two-level hierarchical linear modeling [Article]. Asia Pac Educ Rev 25(1):117–130. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12564-024-09928-7
Premraj D, Thompson R, Hughes L, Adams J (2021) Key factors influencing retention rates among historically underrepresented students groups in STEM fields. J Coll Stud Retent-Res Theory Pract 23(2):457–478. https://doi.org/10.1177/1521025119848763
Pyne J, Rozek CS, Borman GD (2018) Assessing malleable social-psychological academic attitudes in early adolescence. J Sch Psychol 71:57–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2018.10.004
Reid N, Al-Madani KA, Al-Ahmadi FM (2025) Science Education for the Future: Evidence to Guide Policy and Practice. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003564515
Renzulli L, Barr AB (2017) Adapting to family setbacks: malleability of students’ and parents’ educational expectations. Soc Probl 64(3):351–372. https://doi.org/10.1093/socpro/spw052
Reynolds JR, Johnson MK (2011) Change in the stratification of educational expectations and their realization. Soc Forces 90(1):85–109. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/90.1.85
Reynolds LM, Yetnikoff L, Pokinko M, Wodzinski M, Epelbaum JG, Lambert LC, Cossette MP, Arvanitogiannis A, Flores C (2019) Early adolescence is a critical period for the maturation of inhibitory behavior. Cereb Cortex 29(9):3676–3686. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy247
Roth T (2017) Interpersonal influences on educational expectations: new evidence for Germany. Res Soc Stratif Mobil 48:68–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rssm.2016.12.001
Sommerfeld AK (2016) Education as a collective accomplishment: How personal, peer, and parent expectations interact to promote degree attainment. Soc Psychol Educ 19(2):345–365. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-015-9325-7
Sun YM, Li YZ (2011) Effects of family structure type and stability on Children’s Academic performance trajectories. J Marriage Fam 73(3):541–556. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011.00825.x
Tasos B, Nicky C, Grant C (2018) STEM education: An emerging field of inquiry. Brill Sense. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004391413
Toyokawa N, Toyokawa T (2019) Interaction effect of familism and socioeconomic status on academic outcomes of adolescent children of Latino immigrant families. J Adolesc 71:138–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.10.005
Wang J, Wang X (2019) Structural equation modeling: Applications using Mplus. Wiley
Wang MT, Degol J (2013) Motivational pathways to STEM career choices: Using expectancy-value perspective to understand individual and gender differences in STEM fields [Review]. Dev Rev 33(4):304–340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2013.08.001
Wang XL (2013) Why students choose STEM majors: motivation, high school learning, and postsecondary context of support. Am Educ Res J 50(5):1081–1121. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831213488622
Wang XL (2016) Educational expectations and progress of community college students: does socialization matter? Teach Coll Rec 118(5):050308
Wang YP (2024) Understanding Asians’ Success in Pursuing and Completing STEM Degrees. Res High Educ 65(7):1561–1586. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11162-024-09801-x
Widlund A, Tuominen H, Korhonen J (2023) Reciprocal effects of mathematics performance, school engagement and burnout during adolescence. Br J Educ Psychol 93(1):183–197. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12548
Wigfield A, Eccles JS (2000) Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemp Educ Psychol 25(1):68–81. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1015
Wigfield A, Eccles JS (2023) The relevance of situated expectancy-value theory to understanding motivation and emotion in different contexts. In: Motivation and Emotion in Learning and Teaching across Educational Contexts: Theoretical and Methodological Perspectives and Empirical Insights, pp 3–18. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003303473-2
Wright R, Ellis M, Townley M (2017) The matching of STEM degree holders with STEM occupations in large metropolitan labor markets in the United States. Econ Geogr 93(2):185–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/00130095.2016.1220803
Zhan M, Sherraden M (2011) Assets and liabilities, educational expectations, and children’s college degree attainment. Child Youth Serv Rev 33(6):846–854. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.12.006
Zhang F, Jiang Y, Huan SL, Ming H, Ren Y, Wang L (2021) Family socioeconomic status, parental involvement, and academic achievement: the moderating role of adolescents’ subjective social mobility. J Early Adolesc 41(9):1425–1454. https://doi.org/10.1177/02724316211002254. Article 02724316211002254
Zhang J, Cheng HL, Barrella K (2022) Longitudinal pathways to educational attainment and health of immigrant youth in young adulthood: a comparative analysis. J Child Fam Stud 31(1):197–210. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-021-02170-4
Zhang YL (2022) Early academic momentum: Factors contributing to community college transfer students’ STEM degree attainment. J Coll Stud Retent-Res Theory Pract 23(4):873–902. https://doi.org/10.1177/1521025119881130
Zhang YL, Adamuti-Trache M, Connolly J (2019) From community college attendants to baccalaureate recipients: a planned behavior model for transfer students in STEM fields of study. J High Educ 90(3):373–401. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221546.2018.1536935
Zhang YP (2014) Educational expectations, school experiences and academic achievements: a longitudinal examination. China-an Int J 12:43–65
Zhao JX, Li RJ, Ma JL, Zhang WX (2019) Longitudinal relations between future planning and adolescents’ academic achievement in China. J Adolesc 75:73–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.07.002
Zhao T, Perez-Felkner L (2022) Perceived abilities or academic interests? Longitudinal high school science and mathematics effects on postsecondary STEM outcomes by gender and race. Int J STEM Educ 9(1):42. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40594-022-00356-w
Zhou SN, Zeng H, Xu SR, Chen LC, Xiao H (2019) Exploring changes in primary students’ attitudes towards science, technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM) across genders and grade levels. J Balt Sci Educ 18(3):466–480. https://doi.org/10.33225/jbse/19.18.466
Zilanawala A, Sacker A, Kelly Y (2017) Longitudinal latent cognitive profiles and psychosocial well-being in early adolescence. J Adolesc Health 61(4):493–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.05.008
Zimowski MF, Muraki E, Mislevy RJ, Bock RD (1996) Bilog–MG: Multiple-group IRT Analysis and Test Maintenance for Binary Items. SSI Scientific Software
Acknowledgements
The author expresses his thankfulness to Dr Jon D. Miller who worked as a principal investigator of Longitudinal Study of American Youth (LSAY) and shared the data for public use. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Jerf W. K. Yeung contributed by accessing and analyzing the data of LSAY, conceptualizing the study, and writing the original and revised drafts of the manuscript. Akira Igarashi participated in reviewing and giving advice for revising the study. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethical approval
This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all LSAY participants during original data collection by the principal investigators. This study involves secondary analysis of de-identified, publicly available data from the LSAY repository.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Yeung, J.W.K., Igarashi, A. Evolving trajectories of educational expectations and science performance during middle school and STEM degree attainment of youth in adulthood. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 12, 1233 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-025-05563-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-025-05563-8