Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Self-experience of a negative event alters responses to others in similar states through prefrontal cortex CRF mechanisms

Abstract

Our own experience of emotional events influences how we approach and react to others’ emotions. Here we observe that mice exhibit divergent interindividual responses to others in stress (that is, preference or avoidance) only if they have previously experienced the same aversive event. These responses are estrus dependent in females and dominance dependent in males. Notably, silencing the expression of the corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) within the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) attenuates the impact of stress self-experience on the reaction to others’ stress. In vivo microendoscopic calcium imaging revealed that mPFC CRF neurons are activated more toward others’ stress only following the same negative self-experience. Optogenetic manipulations confirmed that higher activation of mPFC CRF neurons is responsible for the switch from preference to avoidance of others in stress, but only following stress self-experience. These results provide a neurobiological substrate underlying how an individual’s emotional experience influences their approach toward others in a negative emotional state.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Restraint stress self-experience changes individual responses to others’ stress.
Fig. 2: Emotion recognition is altered only by matching observer and demonstrator with the same negative emotional experience.
Fig. 3: Sex-dependent differences in self-experience modulation of emotion recognition.
Fig. 4: Silencing CRF in the mPFC attenuated self-experience impact on stress emotion recognition.
Fig. 5: Negative self-experience reverts mPFC CRF neuronal activity toward others in stress.
Fig. 6: Inhibition of mPFC CRF neurons induces social preference in naive condition and abolishes self-experience impact on stress emotion recognition.
Fig. 7: Increased activity of mPFC CRF neurons induces social avoidance only in self-experienced dominant observers.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Individual data can be accessed from the open repository of the Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia (https://doi.org/10.48557/9BICTS). All raw images, calcium imaging recordings and videos can be shared upon request to the corresponding author because of their large sizes. Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

All custom analysis codes generated in MATLAB can be accessed from the open repository of the Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia (https://doi.org/10.48557/9BICTS).

References

  1. Adolphs, R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 169–177 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Preston, S. D. & de Waal, F. B. Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25, 20–71 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Israelashvili, J., Sauter, D. A. & Fischer, A. H. Different faces of empathy: feelings of similarity disrupt recognition of negative emotions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 87, 103912 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Zaki, J. Empathy: a motivated account. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1608–1647 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Olderbak, S. & Wilhelm, O. Emotion perception and empathy: an individual differences test of relations. Emotion 17, 1092–1106 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Allen-Walker, L. & Beaton, A. A. Empathy and perception of emotion in eyes from the FEEST/Ekman and Friesen faces. Pers. Individ. Dif. 72, 150–154 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Ferretti, V. & Papaleo, F. Understanding others: emotion recognition in humans and other animals. Genes Brain Behav. 18, e12544 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ferretti, V. et al. Oxytocin signaling in the central amygdala modulates emotion discrimination in mice. Curr. Biol. 29, 1938–1953 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Scheggia, D. et al. Somatostatin interneurons in the prefrontal cortex control affective state discrimination in mice. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 47–60 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Dautan, D. et al. Cortico-cortical transfer of socially derived information gates emotion recognition. Nat. Neurosci. 27, 1318–1332 (2024).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Ben-Ami Bartal, I., Decety, J. & Mason, P. Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science 334, 1427–1430 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Burkett, J. P. et al. Oxytocin-dependent consolation behavior in rodents. Science 351, 375–378 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Jeon, D. et al. Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 482–488 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Langford, D. J. et al. Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice. Science 312, 1967–1970 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Scheggia, D. et al. Reciprocal cortico-amygdala connections regulate prosocial and selfish choices in mice. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 1505–1518 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Smith, M. L., Asada, N. & Malenka, R. C. Anterior cingulate inputs to nucleus accumbens control the social transfer of pain and analgesia. Science 371, 153–159 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Iarocci, G., Yager, J. & Elfers, T. What gene–environment interactions can tell us about social competence in typical and atypical populations. Brain Cogn. 65, 112–127 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Deussing, J. M. & Chen, A. The corticotropin-releasing factor family: physiology of the stress response. Physiol. Rev. 98, 2225–2286 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Papaleo, F., Kitchener, P. & Contarino, A. Disruption of the CRF/CRF1 receptor stress system exacerbates the somatic signs of opiate withdrawal. Neuron 53, 577–589 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Sterley, T. L. et al. Social transmission and buffering of synaptic changes after stress. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 393–403 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Hostetler, C. M. & Ryabinin, A. E. The CRF system and social behavior: a review. Front. Neurosci. 7, 92 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Lim, M. M. et al. CRF receptors in the nucleus accumbens modulate partner preference in prairie voles. Horm. Behav. 51, 508–515 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Cooper, M. A. & Huhman, K. L. Corticotropin-releasing factor type II (CRF-sub-2) receptors in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis modulate conditioned defeat in Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Behav. Neurosci. 119, 1042–1051 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. de Leon Reyes, N. S. et al. Corticotropin-releasing hormone signaling from prefrontal cortex to lateral septum suppresses interaction with familiar mice. Cell 186, 4152–4171 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kobayashi, T., Kiyokawa, Y., Takeuchi, Y. & Mori, Y. Pretreatment with CP-154526 blocks the modifying effects of alarm pheromone on components of sexual behavior in male, but not in female, rats. Chem. Senses 36, 623–632 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Grimm, S. et al. The interaction of corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor gene and early life stress on emotional empathy. Behav. Brain Res 329, 180–185 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Chen, P. et al. Prefrontal cortex corticotropin-releasing factor neurons control behavioral style selection under challenging situations. Neuron 106, 301–315 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Hiser, J. & Koenigs, M. The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 638–647 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Yuen, E. Y. et al. Acute stress enhances glutamatergic transmission in prefrontal cortex and facilitates working memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 14075–14079 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. McKlveen, J. M., Myers, B. & Herman, J. P. The medial prefrontal cortex: coordinator of autonomic, neuroendocrine and behavioural responses to stress. J. Neuroendocrinol. 27, 446–456 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Sanders, J., Mayford, M. & Jeste, D. Empathic fear responses in mice are triggered by recognition of a shared experience. PLoS ONE 8, e74609 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Bos, P. A. et al. Testosterone reduces functional connectivity during the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 68, 194–201 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Machida, T., Yonezawa, Y. & Noumura, T. Age-associated changes in plasma testosterone levels in male mice and their relation to social dominance or subordinance. Horm. Behav. 15, 238–245 (1981).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Jones, C. E. & Monfils, M. H. Dominance status predicts social fear transmission in laboratory rats. Anim. Cogn. 19, 1051–1069 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Fan, Z. et al. Using the tube test to measure social hierarchy in mice. Nat. Protoc. 14, 819–831 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Guapo, V. G. et al. Effects of sex hormonal levels and phases of the menstrual cycle in the processing of emotional faces. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1087–1094 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Mikosz, M., Nowak, A., Werka, T. & Knapska, E. Sex differences in social modulation of learning in rats. Sci. Rep. 5, 18114 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Cora, M. C., Kooistra, L. & Travlos, G. Vaginal cytology of the laboratory rat and mouse: review and criteria for the staging of the estrous cycle using stained vaginal smears. Toxicol. Pathol. 43, 776–793 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. McLean, A. C., Valenzuela, N., Fai, S. & Bennett, S. A. Performing vaginal lavage, crystal violet staining, and vaginal cytological evaluation for mouse estrous cycle staging identification. J. Vis. Exp. 15, e4389 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  40. Millan, M. J. & Bales, K. L. Towards improved animal models for evaluating social cognition and its disruption in schizophrenia: the CNTRICS initiative. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2166–2180 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Locci, A., Yan, Y., Rodriguez, G. & Dong, H. Sex differences in CRF1, CRF, and CRFBP expression in C57BL/6J mouse brain across the lifespan and in response to acute stress. J. Neurochem. 158, 943–959 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Hupalo, S. et al. Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) circuit modulation of cognition and motivation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 103, 50–59 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Berridge, C. W., Martin, A. J., Hupalo, S. & Nicol, S. E. Estrus cycle-dependent working memory effects of prefrontal cortex corticotropin-releasing factor neurotransmission. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 2016–2023 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Isaacson, J. S. & Scanziani, M. How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron 72, 231–243 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Grynberg, D. & Lopez-Perez, B. Facing others’ misfortune: personal distress mediates the association between maladaptive emotion regulation and social avoidance. PLoS ONE 13, e0194248 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Kim, E. J., Kim, E. S., Covey, E. & Kim, J. J. Social transmission of fear in rats: the role of 22-kHz ultrasonic distress vocalization. PLoS ONE 5, e15077 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Atsak, P. et al. Experience modulates vicarious freezing in rats: a model for empathy. PLoS ONE 6, e21855 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Toyoshima, M., Mitsui, K. & Yamada, K. Prior stress experience modulates social preference for stressed conspecifics in male rats. Neurosci. Lett. 765, 136253 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Larrieu, T. et al. Hierarchical status predicts behavioral vulnerability and nucleus accumbens metabolic profile following chronic social defeat stress. Curr. Biol. 27, 2202–2210 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Horii, Y. et al. Hierarchy in the home cage affects behaviour and gene expression in group-housed C57BL/6 male mice. Sci. Rep. 7, 6991 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Osorio, F. L., de Paula Cassis, J. M., Machado de Sousa, J. P., Poli-Neto, O. & Martin-Santos, R. Sex hormones and processing of facial expressions of emotion: a systematic literature review. Front. Psychol. 9, 529 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R. & Macrae, C. N. The link between social cognition and self-referential thought in the medial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1306–1315 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Adolphs, R. The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 231–239 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Adolphs, R. The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 693–716 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. Mechanisms of social cognition. Annu Rev. Psychol. 63, 287–313 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Kensinger, E. A. & Ford, J. H. Guiding the emotion in emotional memories: the role of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 30, 111–119 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Kai, Y. et al. A medial prefrontal cortex–nucleus acumens corticotropin-releasing factor circuitry for neuropathic pain-increased susceptibility to opioid reward. Transl. Psychiatry 8, 100 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Hupalo, S., Martin, A. J., Green, R. K., Devilbiss, D. M. & Berridge, C. W. Prefrontal corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) neurons act locally to modulate frontostriatal cognition and circuit function. J. Neurosci. 39, 2080–2090 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Yizhar, O. et al. Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature 477, 171–178 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Brockhurst, J., Cheleuitte-Nieves, C., Buckmaster, C. L., Schatzberg, A. F. & Lyons, D. M.Stress inoculation modeled in mice. Transl. Psychiatry 5, e537 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to M. Morini, D. Cantatore, G. Pruzzo, C. Diana, A. Monteforte and C. Chiabrera for technical support. We thank A. Chen and Y. Shemesh from the Weizmann Institute of Science for input and material. This work was supported by funding from the Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Fondazione Telethon Italia (project GGP19103), and Italian Ministry of University and Research (National Recovery and Resilience Plan, project MNESYS PE0000006) to F.P. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, F. Maltese and F.P. Methodology and investigation, F. Maltese, G.P., A.M.C., F. Manago, N.d.L., A.M., M.N., R.W., F.L. and F.P. Resources, F.P. Writing, F. Maltese and F.P. Visualization and analysis, F. Maltese, F.B. and F.P. Supervision, F.P. All authors revised and approved the paper.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Francesco Papaleo.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Neuroscience thanks Ioana Carcea, Stan Floresco and Jiang-Ning Zhou for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Emotion recognition in naive and restraint stress self-experienced male and female mice.

(a, f) Experimental design of the EDT with one restraint stress (purple) and one neutral (gray) demonstrator in (a) naive and (f) negative self-experience condition. (b, d, g, i) Percentage of time spent sniffing demonstrators in restraint stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (b, g) male and (d, i) female (b, d) naïve and (g, i) self-experience observers (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 38 Naïve Males and 35 Naïve Females. mice. N = 38 Self-experience Males and 44 Self-experience Females). (c, e, h, j) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in restraint stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (c, h) male and (e, j) female (c, e) naïve and (h, j) self-experienced observers (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 38 Naïve Males and 35 Naïve Females. N = 38 Self-experience males and 44 Self-experience Females). (k) Time spent grooming during the first 2 min of the test displayed by neutral (gray bars) or restraint stress (purple bars) demonstrators in naïve and restraint stress self-experience condition (two-way ANOVA, Bonferroni correction. N = 32 neutral and N = 32 restraint stress demonstrators). (l, n) No significant correlation was found between observers’ discrimination index (in y axis) and restraint stress demonstrators’ grooming (in x axis) during the first 2 min of the test in (l) naive and (n) self-experience condition (two-tailed Pearson correlation; N = 25 observers/condition and N = 25 restraint stress demonstrators/condition). (m, o) No correlation was found between observers’ grooming behavior (in y axis) and restraint stress demonstrators’ grooming (in x axis) during the first 2 min of the test in (m) naïve and (o) restraint stress self-experience condition (two-tailed Pearson correlation; N = 89 observers/condition and N = 89 stress demonstrators/condition). Bar and line graphs show mean ± s.e.m. ***P < 0.0005.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 2 Effects of different stressful experiences on emotion recognition.

(a, c) Experimental design of the EDT with one footshock (green) vs one neutral (gray) demonstrator in (a) naïve and (c) footshock self-experienced observers. (b, d) Time (in seconds) spent sniffing demonstrators in footshock stress (green bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (b) naïve and (d) footshock self-experienced observer mice (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 8 naïve and 16 footshock self-experience observers). (e) Mice in naïve (white) and footshock self-experience (blue) condition do not differ based on the amount of time spent grooming across the 6 min test (two-tailed unpaired t-test; N = 8 naïve and N = 16 footshock self-experienced mice). (f) No significant correlation was found between discrimination index (in y axis) and grooming (in x axis) of footshock self-experienced observer (two-tailed Pearson correlation; N = 16 mice). (g, i) Experimental design of the EDT with one restraint stress (purple) vs one neutral (gray) demonstrator in (g) naïve and (i) footshock-experienced mice. (h, j) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in restraint stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (h) naïve and (j) footshock-experienced observer mice (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 14 naïve and 11 footshock-experience observers). (k) Naïve (white) and footshock-experienced (green) observers did not differ based on the amount of time spent grooming across the 6 min test (two-tailed unpaired t-test; N = 25 naïve and N = 11 footshock-experienced mice). (l) No significant correlation was found between discrimination index (in y axis) and grooming (in x axis) of footshock-experienced observer (two-tailed Pearson correlation; N = 11 mice). (m, o) Experimental design of the EDT with one fear vs one neutral demonstrator in (m) naïve and (o) restraint-experienced mice. (n, p) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in fear (red bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (n) naïve and (p) restraint-experienced observer mice (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 10 naïve and 12 restraint-experience observers). (q) Naïve (white) and restraint-experienced observers (purple) did not differ based on the amount of time spent grooming across the 6 min test (two-tailed unpaired t-test; N = 10 naïve and N = 12 restraint-experienced mice). (r) No significant correlation was found between discrimination index (in y axis) and grooming (in x axis) of footshock-experienced observer (two-tailed Pearson correlation; N = 12 mice). Bar and line graphs show mean ± s.e.m. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.0005.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 3 Social hierarchy and estrus cycle in negative self-experience modulation of emotion recognition.

(a, d) Experimental design of the EDT in (a) naïve and (d) self-experience condition. (b, e) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (left) dominant or (right) subordinate (b) naïve and (e) self-experience males (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 8 dominant and 13 subordinate observers). (c, f) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (left) estral or (right) diestral (c) naïve and (f) self-experienced females (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. (N = 7 naïve estral and 7 diestral observers; N = 15 self-experience estral and 13 diestral observers). (g) Both dominant and subordinate self-experienced observers differ from naïve mice in the total social exploration towards stress, but not neutral (Three-way RM ANOVA, Bonferroni correction; N = 7 naïve and 7 self-experienced dominants, N = 11 naïve and 13 self-experienced subordinates). (h) Time spent grooming (two-tailed unpaired t-test; N = 7 dominant and N = 13 subordinate mice) and (i) corticosterone levels (two-tailed unpaired t-test; N = 5 dominant and N = 4 subordinate mice) by self-experience dominant (orange) and subordinate (dark green) observers. (j) Both estral and diestral negative self-experienced observers significantly differ from naïve mice in the total social exploration towards stress, but not neutral, demonstrators (Three-way RM ANOVA, Bonferroni correction; N = 5 naïve and 15 self-experienced estral, N = 7 naïve and 13 negative self-experienced diestral). (k) Estral (light purple) and diestral (purple) self-experienced female mice do not differ in the time spent grooming across the 6 min test (two-tailed unpaired t-test; N = 15 estral and N = 13 diestral observers). (l, m) Time in (l) seconds or (m) percentage of time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test displayed by (left) rank 2 or (right) rank 3 self-experienced subordinate mice (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 7 rank 2 and 6 rank 3 observers). (n) Time in seconds and (o) percentage of time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, which occurred 24 hr after self-experience, displayed by observer mice that were in (left) estrus or (right) diestrus when self-experience of stress occurred (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. (N = 16 in estrus and 6 in diestrus). Bar and line graphs show mean ± s.e.m. *P < 0.05. **P < 0.005. ***P < 0.0005.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 4 Silencing of CRF in mPFC modulates negative self-experience effect on stress emotion recognition.

(a, d) Experimental design of the EDT in (a) naïve and (d) negative self-experience condition. (b, c) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by naïve (b) scrambled and (c) anti-CRF shRNA naïve observers (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. (b) SCRAMBLED. 2 min: P = 0.0002, 4 min: P = 0.01, 6 min: P = 0.01, N = 14 observers. (c) anti-CRF shRNA. 2 min: P = 0.0006; 4 min: P = 0.03; 6 min: P = 0.01; N = 15 observers). (e, f) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by negative self-experienced (e) scrambled and (f) anti-CRF shRNA observers (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. (e) SCRAMBLED: no significant differences; N = 14 observers. (f) anti-CRF shRNA. 6 min: P = 0.01; N = 16 observers). (g, h) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by anti-CRF shRNA negative self-experienced (g) dominants and (h) subordinates (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. (g) DOMINANTS. 6 min: P = 0.02; N = 7 mice. (h) SUBORDINATES. 6 min: P = 0.052; N = 9 mice). Bar and line graphs show mean ± s.e.m. *P < 0.05; ***P < 0.0005.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 5 mPFC-CRF neurons activity during EDT and falcon restraint.

(a) CRF-Cre mice were injected into the mPFC with Syn.Flex.GCaMP6f.eYFP and implanted with a GRIN lens (n = 4/5mice). (b) Representative images of viral infection and GRIN lens placement of the five mice tested. (c, e) Experimental design of the EDT in (c) naïve and (e) negative self-experience condition. (d) Averaged percentage of time spent in the zone around a stress (purple bars) or a neutral (gray bars) demonstrator, displayed by naïve observers (two-tailed paired t-test; N = 4 observers). (f) Averaged percentage of time spent in the zone around a stress (purple bars) or a neutral (gray bars) demonstrator, displayed by negative self-experienced observers (left) with or (right) without preference towards stress demonstrators (two-tailed paired t-test. (left) Preference for stress: N = 3 mice. (right) No preference for stress: N = 2 mice. (g-j) Heatmaps of entrances in stress (g, i) or neutral (h, j) zone associated with neuronal inactivation in (g, h) naïve and (i, j) negative self-experience condition. (k) Experimental design: mice were placed for 10 min inside an empty cage (before restraint stress), and subsequently moved inside a 50 ml falcon for 30 min. (l, m) Average peaks’ (l) frequency and (m) amplitude before (yellow) and after (purple) restrain stress (two-tailed Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed ranks test. (l) Number of pairs: 62, p < 0.0001. (m) Number of pairs: 62, p = 0.84). Bar and line graphs show mean ± s.e.m. ***P < 0.0005.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 6 Optogenetic inhibition of mPFC-CRF neurons during Emotion Recognition, Object Discrimination and Tube Test.

(a) Experimental design of the EDT with one stress and one neutral demonstrator in naïve condition. Photoinhibition (λ = 532 nm) was paired to exploration of neutral demonstrator. (b) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by naive observers tested in (left) light OFF and (right) light ON condition (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 5 observers/condition). (c) Total social exploration during the 6-min test, displayed by naive observers tested in light OFF and light ON condition (two-tailed paired t-test; N = 5 observers/condition). (d) Experimental design of the EDT with two neutral demonstrators in naïve condition. Photoinhibition (λ = 532 nm) was paired to the exploration of one demonstrator. (e, f) Time spent sniffing two neutral demonstrators in (e) percentage or (f) seconds, during the 6-min test divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by naïve observers tested in (left) light OFF or (right) light ON condition (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 11 observers/condition). (g) Left: Cumulative time spent sniffing two neutral demonstrators in light OFF and light ON conditions (three-way RM ANOVA; N = 11 observers). Right: representative tracking plot for light OFF (top) and light ON (bottom) conditions. (h) Total social exploration during the 6-min test, displayed by naïve observers tested in light OFF and light ON condition (two-tailed paired t-test; N = 11 observers/condition). (i) Experimental design of the EDT with one stress and one neutral demonstrator in self-experience condition. Photoinhibition (λ = 532 nm) was paired to exploration of stress demonstrator. (j) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by self-experience observers tested in (left) light OFF and (right) light ON condition (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 9 observers/condition). (k) Total social exploration during the 6-min test, displayed by negative self-experienced observers tested in light OFF and light ON condition (two-tailed paired t-test; N = 9 observers/condition). (l, m) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by self-experience (left) dominant or (right) subordinate mice tested in (l) light OFF and (m) light ON condition (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 4 dominant and 5 subordinate observers). (n) Experimental design of the EDT with two inanimate objects in naïve condition. Photoinhibition (λ = 532 nm) was paired to exploration of one of the two objects. (o, p) Time spent sniffing two objects in (o) percentage or (p) seconds, during the 6-min test divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by naïve observers. (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 10 observers). (q) Experimental design of photoinhibition (λ = 532 nm) during tube test performed on cages of two or three mice. (r) Summary of mPFC-CRF neurons photoinhibition–induced rank change. Each line represents one animal. (s) Mean rank change induced by mPFC-CRF neurons photoinhibition. N = 11 mice. Bar and line graphs show mean ± s.e.m. *P < 0.05. **P < 0.005.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 7 Optogenetic activation of mPFC-CRF neurons during Emotion Recognition, Object Discrimination and Tube Test.

(a) Experimental design of the EDT with one neutral and one stress demonstrator in naive condition. Photostimulation (λ = 473 nm) was paired with one stress demonstrator. (b) Time spent sniffing demonstrators in stress (purple bars) or neutral (gray bars) state during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by naïve observers tested in (left) light OFF and (right) light ON condition (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 6 observers/condition). (c) Total social exploration during the 6-min test, displayed by self-experience observers tested in light OFF and light ON condition (two-tailed paired t-test; N = 6 observers/condition). (d) Experimental design of the EDT with two neutral demonstrators in naive condition. Photostimulation (λ = 473 nm) was paired to exploration of one of the two neutral demonstrators. (e, f) Time spent sniffing two neutral demonstrators in (e) percentage or (f) seconds, during the 6-min test divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by naïve observers tested in (left) light OFF and (right) light ON condition (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 7 observers/condition). (g) Left: Cumulative time spent sniffing two neutral demonstrators in light OFF and light ON conditions (three-way RM ANOVA). Right: representative tracking plot for light OFF (top) and light ON (bottom) conditions. N = 7 observers/condition. (h) Total social exploration during the 6-min test, displayed by naïve observers tested in light OFF and light ON condition (two-tailed paired t-test; N = 7 observers/condition). (i) Experimental design of the EDT with two neutral demonstrators in self-experience condition. Photostimulation (λ = 473 nm) was paired to exploration of one of two neutral demonstrators. (j) Time spent sniffing two neutral demonstrators during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by self-experienced observers tested in (left) light OFF and (right) light ON condition (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 5 observers/condition). (k) Total social exploration during the 6-min test, displayed by naive observers tested in light OFF and light ON condition (two-tailed paired t-test; N = 6 observers/condition). (l, m) Time spent sniffing two neutral demonstrators during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by negative self-experienced (left) dominant and (right) subordinate mice tested in (l) light OFF and (m) light ON condition (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 3 observers/condition). (n, q) Experimental design of the EDT with two inanimate objects in (n) naïve or (q) self-experience condition. Photostimulation (λ = 473 nm) was paired to exploration of one of the two object. (o, r) Percentage of time spent sniffing two objects during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (o) naïve or (r) self-experienced observers (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 8 naïve and 7 self-experience observers). (p, s) Time spent sniffing two objects during the 6-min test, divided into three consecutive 2-min epochs, displayed by (p) naïve or (s) self-experienced observers (two-tailed multiple t-test, Bonferroni correction. N = 8 naïve and 7 self-experience observers). (t) Experimental design of photostimulation (λ = 473 nm) during tube test performed on cages of two mice. (u) Summary of mPFC-CRF neurons photostimulation–induced rank change. Each line represents one animal. (v) Mean rank change induced by mPFC CRF neurons photostimulation. Bar and line graphs show mean ± s.e.m. *P < 0.05.

Source data

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figs. 1–7 and Tables 1–3.

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Video 1

Miniscope recording of CRF mPFC neurons during EDT in naive condition.

Supplementary Video 2

Miniscope recording of CRF mPFC neurons during EDT in self-experience condition.

Supplementary Data 1

Source data for supplementary figures.

Source data

Source Data Fig. 1

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 2

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 3

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 4

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 5

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 6

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 7

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 1

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 2

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 3

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 4

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 5

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 6

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 7

Statistical source data.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Maltese, F., Pacinelli, G., Monai, A. et al. Self-experience of a negative event alters responses to others in similar states through prefrontal cortex CRF mechanisms. Nat Neurosci 28, 122–136 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-024-01816-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-024-01816-y

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing